Dog communication

Last updated

Ano-Genital-Kontrolle.jpg
Dogs that are familiar with each other may lick each other's faces in greeting, then they begin to sniff any moist membranes where odours are strongest. [1] :124
Cooper the bloodhound meets and greets friend.jpg
The mucus on a dog's wet nose traps particles from everything the dog has recently smelled or eaten. When dogs meet, they smell each other's noses to see what the other dog did that day and if there is any food nearby.
Husky (1).jpg
Dogs yawn when they are tired (like humans) or under stress. [1] :120–122

Dog communication is the transfer of information between dogs, as well as between dogs and humans. Behaviors associated with dog communication are categorized into visual and vocal.[ citation needed ] Visual communication includes mouth shape and head position, licking and sniffing, ear and tail positioning, eye gaze, facial expression, and body posture. Dog vocalizations, or auditory communication, can include barks, growls, howls, whines and whimpers, screams, pants and sighs. Dogs also communicate via gustatory communication, utilizing scent and pheromones. [2]

Contents

Humans can communicate with dogs through a wide variety of methods. Broadly, this includes vocalization, hand signals, body posture and touch. The two species also communicate visually: through domestication, dogs have become particularly adept at "reading" human facial expressions, and they are able to determine human emotional status. When communicating with a human, their level of comprehension is generally comparable to a toddler.[ citation needed ]

Dog–human communication

A drawing by Konrad Lorenz showing facial expressions of a dog - a communication behavior. y-axis = fear, x-axis = aggression Lorenz emotions.png
A drawing by Konrad Lorenz showing facial expressions of a dog – a communication behavior. y-axis = fear, x-axis = aggression

Both humans and dogs are characterized by complex social lives with complex communication systems, but it is also possible that dogs, perhaps because of their reliance on humans for food, have evolved specialized skills for recognizing and interpreting human social-communicative signals. [3] Four basic hypotheses have been put forward to account for the findings.

  1. Dogs, by way of their interactions with humans, learn to be responsive to human social cues through basic conditioning processes. [4]
  2. By undergoing domestication, dogs not only reduced their fear of humans but also applied all-purpose problem-solving skills to their interactions with people. This largely innate gift for reading human social gestures was inadvertently selected via domestication. [5] [6]
  3. Dogs' co-evolution with humans equipped them with the cognitive machinery to not only respond to human social cues but to understand human mental states; a so-called theory of mind. [7] [8]
  4. Dogs are adaptively predisposed to learn about human communicative gestures. They come with a built-in "head start" to learn the significance of people's gestures, in much the same way that white-crowned sparrows acquire their species-typical song [9] and ducklings imprint on their own kind. [10]

Dogs tend to be highly responsive to human cues, especially the direction of a gaze and the direction in which a human points. Dogs rely on the gestures of humans more than verbal cues, most importantly eye contact. Eye contact is considered an ostensive cue. A human-dog gaze helps dogs establish stronger relationships by being able to communicate better with humans, as well as other dogs. [11] Dogs will start to act and react much like their owners do. Dogs will pick up on how their owners respond to strangers and non-friendly dogs. [11]

The pointing gesture is a human-specific signal and is referential.[ citation needed ] Human infants acquire it weeks before the first spoken word. [12] In 2009, a study compared the responses to a range of pointing gestures by dogs and human infants. The study showed little difference in the performance of 2-year-old children and dogs, while 3-year-old children's performances were higher. The results also showed that all subjects were able to generalize from their previous experience to respond to relatively novel pointing gestures. This can be explained as a joint outcome of their evolutionary history as well as their socialization in a human environment. [13]

One study has indicated that dogs are able to tell how big another dog is just by listening to its growl. The research also shows that dogs do not, or cannot, misrepresent their size, and this is the first time research has shown animals can determine another's size by the sound they make. The test, using images of many kinds of dogs, showed a small and big dog and played a growl. Twenty of the 24 test dogs looked at the image of the appropriately sized dog first and looked at it longest. [14]

Depending on the context, a dog's bark can vary in timing, pitch, and amplitude. It is possible that these have different meanings. [15]

Most people can tell from a bark whether a dog was alone or being approached by a stranger, playing or being aggressive, [16] and able to tell from a growl how big the dog is. [17] This is thought to be evidence of human-dog coevolution. [17]

Visual

Dogs communicating emotions through body positioning were illustrated in Charles Darwin's The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals published in 1872.

In her book On Talking Terms with Dogs, [18] Turid Rugaas identifies around 30 signals that she calls calming signals. The notion of dominance and submission is much debated. [19] [20] In her book, she does not use these terms to differentiate behaviour. She describes calming signals as a way for dogs to calm themselves or other humans/dogs around them. These are some of the signals she identifies:

By moving different parts of their bodies, whether facial expressions or postures, dogs can express a wide range of emotions and signals.

Dog with ears erect means it is alerted, and baring its teeth is a warning signal. Do not take his bone.jpg
Dog with ears erect means it is alerted, and baring its teeth is a warning signal.
Licking can mean different things depending on the context. Malta - Mellieha - Triq Selmun - Pharaoh Hound 04 ies.jpg
Licking can mean different things depending on the context.

Mouth shape

Head position

Yawn

Similarly to humans, dogs yawn in an attempt to awaken. Dogs will also yawn when under stress, or as a pacifying signal when being menaced by aggression signals from another dog. Yawning, accompanied by a head turned away from the aggressor, can defuse a potentially threatening situation. [1] :120–122 It is also recognized as a calming signal. [18]

Licking and sniffing

Licking behavior has multiple causes and meanings and should not be simply interpreted as affection. Dogs that are familiar with each other may lick each other's faces in greeting, then sniff any moist membranes where odors are strongest (i.e. mouth, nose, anal region, or urogenital region.) Mating behaviors are characterized by licking in a more vigorous manner than used during greetings. [1] :124 Licking can communicate information about dominance, intentions, and state of mind, and, like the yawn, is mainly a pacifying behavior. All pacifying behaviors contain elements of puppy behavior, including licking. Puppies lick themselves and their littermates as part of the cleaning process, and it appears to build bonds. Later in life, licking ceases to be a cleaning function and forms a ritualized gesture indicating friendliness. [1] :124–125 When stressed, a dog might lick the air, its own lips, or drop down and lick its paws or body. [1] :126 Lip-licking and sniffing are also recognized as calming signals. [18]

Ears

Dogs' ability to move their ears is different among each breed. In addition, some ears move very little if they are artificially altered or cropped by humans. [11]

Tail held lower than the horizontal, perhaps with an occasional swishing back and forth - an unconcerned, relaxed dog PERRO.jpg
Tail held lower than the horizontal, perhaps with an occasional swishing back and forth – an unconcerned, relaxed dog

Eyes

When dogs want to threaten a perceived adversary, they will stare. In contrast, dogs will avoid eye contact if trying to decrease tension. [11] They can communicate emotional states by having "soft" eyes or "hard" eyes. Soft eyes are used when a dog feels relaxed and not threatened. Hard eyes are used when feeling tension and unease about a potentially threatening situation. The eyes are slightly closed, the brow area is wrinkled, and their teeth may be showing. A dog should not be approached when they are communicating this way and should be removed from the high tension situation, if possible. [11]

Tail between legs, lying down, ears back, body tight - a submissive dog who is worried or frightened ScaredSubmissiveDog.jpg
Tail between legs, lying down, ears back, body tight – a submissive dog who is worried or frightened

Tail

A dog rolls on its back and rubs its shoulders on the ground to display contentment Golden Retriever Wurftreffen Lightning Dreams (10576700306).jpg
A dog rolls on its back and rubs its shoulders on the ground to display contentment

Dogs are said to exhibit a left-right asymmetry of the tail when interacting with strangers, and will show the opposite right-left motion with people and dogs they know. [21]

Dogs' ability to move their tails can be different among different breeds due to the fact that humans can dock their tails or remove them completely. [11] In addition, some breeds, such as the Australian Stumpy Tail Cattle Dog, are born with extremely short tails, or other breeds, such as the Pembroke Welsh Corgi, may be born without a tail altogether.

Body

Dogs' bodies alone can communicate a lot. By increasing the size and tension of their bodies and making themselves look larger, they can communicate confidence, alertness, or even threat. Actions meant to reduce the size of the body, such as lowering to the floor with tail tucked and lowered ears, can communicate stress, fear, nerves, or a desire to avoid conflicts. [11]

Auditory

Long-distance contact calls are common in Canidae, typically in the form of either barks (termed "pulse trains") or howls (termed "long acoustic streams"). [22] [23] The long-distance howling of wolves [24] and coyotes [25] [26] [27] is one way in which dogs communicate.

By the age of four weeks, the dog has developed the majority of its vocalizations. The dog is the most vocal canid and is unique in its tendency to bark in myriad situations. Barking appears to have little more communication functions than excitement, fighting, the presence of a human, or simply because other dogs are barking. Subtler signs such as discreet bodily and facial movements, body odors, whines, yelps, and growls are the main sources of actual communication. The majority of these subtle communication techniques are employed at a close proximity to another, but for long-range communication, only barking and howling are employed. [28] :Ch10

Rapid barking with a midrange pitch is the basic alarm bark. Military dog barking.JPG
Rapid barking with a midrange pitch is the basic alarm bark.

Barks

Two dogs communicating a warning; note the teeth baring and lip curl. Two dogs seems like fighting but are NOT Jan 2008 Shot in Jalandhar Punjab India by gopal1035 003.jpg
Two dogs communicating a warning; note the teeth baring and lip curl.

Growls

Howls

Dog howling indicates the dog is present or in its territory. Dog howling 001.jpg
Dog howling indicates the dog is present or in its territory.

Whines and whimpers

Whining and whimpers are short, high pitched sounds designed to bring the listener closer to show either fear or submission on the behalf of the whiner or whimperer. These are also the sounds that puppies make as pacifying and soliciting sounds. [1] :89

Screaming

A yelp for several seconds in length much like a human child, then repeated – anguish or agony, a call to the pack-mates for help, is rarely heard. It could be an indication of severe injury. It is recommended to take a dog to a veterinarian immediately if they scream. [1] :92–93

Panting

Panting is an attempt to regulate body temperature. Excitement can raise the body temperature in both humans and dogs. Although not an intentional communication, if the dog pants rapidly even though it is not exposed to warm conditions or intense physical activity, then this signals excitement due to stress. [1] :95

Sighs

Sighs are an expression of emotion, usually when the dog is lying down with its head on its paws. When the eyes are half-closed, it signals pleasure and contentment. When the eyes are fully open, it signals displeasure or disappointment. [1] :96

Play Sneezing

Play sneezing is another calming signal that dogs use [29] to indicate that they are not being aggressive, or that they are just playing. Play sneezes are not actual sneezes, but more of an expulsion of air that resembles a sneeze, and occur frequently during play. [30]

Olfactory

Dog urinating on lifeguard's chair.jpg
"Raised-leg urination" posture [31] [32]

Dogs have an olfactory sense 40 times more sensitive than a human's and they commence their lives operating almost exclusively on smell and touch. [1] :247 The special scents that dogs use for communication are called pheromones. Pheromones are composed of natural chemicals that mediate olfactory communication with conspecifics. These pheromones that contain chemical signals serve to send information to other dogs about social status, age, sexual or reproductive status, aggressive behavior, and territorial marking. [33] [34] They can serve to either attract or repel other dogs. [34] Pheromone signals are received in the vomeronasal organ (VNO) that is included in the olfactory tract. [31] In order for a dog to detect a stimulus, or odor, the VNO has to be opened. It is opened when a pheromone attaches to the membrane of the nervous cells in the vomeronasal organ. [35] From the VNO, the pheromone signals are sent to the accessory olfactory bulb and transferred to the amygdala. The pheromone will finally be sent to the ventromedial hypothalamus, where the signal is perceived. The Flehmen Response is a behavior in dogs in which the upper lip curves up to reveal to their teeth; this behavior strengthens the intake of pheromones. [33]

Chemical signals are diffused into the environment by anal secretions, pedal glands, urine and fecal deposits, body odor, and rubbing their body on certain items. [34] This is called scent marking, when individuals leave strong smelling scents on specific areas in the environment in order to mark their territory. The scent mark contains chemical messages about the sender. For example, when a female is in estrus, she will urinate more frequently to attract a potential mate. The urine contains information about a female's reproductive status and it also conveys messages to the receiver about the female's location. [34] The compound p-hydroxybenzoate is found in the pheromones that attract a male to a female in estrus. [33]

The most common type of scent marking is urine-marking to identify their territory. [36] Adult males prefer to mark vertical surfaces with urine, using the raised-leg posture. [36] The raised-leg posture provides increased dispersal of the elevated mark, allowing the wind to carry it further. [34] Females; however, tend to utilize a squatting posture and urinate less frequently than males. [36] Additionally, smaller dogs can use a higher leg raise when urinating to embellish their competitive ability. [36]

Overmarking is when an individual covers another's scent mark with its own. Both males and females practice overmarking, although it is more common in males. Overmarking is used to send information about social status and dominance. It enables dogs to find potential mates or acknowledge potential competitors. Such as males may overmark a female's urine to guard a potential mate. [36]

Scratching the ground is a common behavior seen after urination or defecation and is another way in which chemical signals are secreted. [34] Pheromones are excreted from the pedal glands, aiding in territorial marking or adding additional odor to the scent mark. The chances of a dog scratching the ground increases when the individual is aggressively aroused. [34] It is also usually followed by another male counter marking with urine in order to cover the area with their own scent and more prevalent when there is a female in estrus around. [35]

Dogs also receive information about a conspecific by sniffing particular glands. When greeting, dogs tend to be more attentive to the neck, face, inguinal, and peripheral areas. There are specific glands in these areas that produce different odors, such as glands at the corner of the mouth, in the ear pinnae, the preputial and vaginal glands, and anal glands. [37] Dogs gain social information by sniffing particular odors secreted from these areas. [37] Dogs can also gain information from anal secretions. All canines have two symmetric sacs on either side of the anal sphincter that produce anal secretions during defecation. These are important in marking territory and sending information about social status. [34] [35]

See also

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Canidae</span> Family of mammals

Canidae is a biological family of dog-like carnivorans, colloquially referred to as dogs, and constitutes a clade. A member of this family is also called a canid. The family includes three subfamilies: the Caninae, the extinct Borophaginae and Hesperocyoninae. The Caninae are known as canines, and include domestic dogs, wolves, coyotes, foxes, jackals and other species.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Prairie dog</span> Genus of ground squirrels

Prairie dogs are herbivorous burrowing ground squirrels native to the grasslands of North America. There are five recognized species of prairie dog: black-tailed, white-tailed, Gunnison's, Utah, and Mexican prairie dogs. In Mexico, prairie dogs are found primarily in the northern states, which lie at the southern end of the Great Plains: northeastern Sonora, north and northeastern Chihuahua, northern Coahuila, northern Nuevo León, and northern Tamaulipas. In the United States, they range primarily to the west of the Mississippi River, though they have also been introduced in a few eastern locales. They are also found in the Canadian Prairies. Despite the name, they are not actually canines; prairie dogs, along with the marmots, chipmunks, and several other basal genera belong to the ground squirrels, part of the larger squirrel family (Sciuridae).

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Animal communication</span> Transfer of information from animal to animal

Animal communication is the transfer of information from one or a group of animals to one or more other animals that affects the current or future behavior of the receivers. Information may be sent intentionally, as in a courtship display, or unintentionally, as in the transfer of scent from predator to prey with kairomones. Information may be transferred to an "audience" of several receivers. Animal communication is a rapidly growing area of study in disciplines including animal behavior, sociology, neurology and animal cognition. Many aspects of animal behavior, such as symbolic name use, emotional expression, learning and sexual behavior, are being understood in new ways.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Licking</span> Action of passing the tongue over a surface

Licking is the action of passing the tongue over a surface, typically either to deposit saliva onto the surface, or to collect liquid, food or minerals onto the tongue for ingestion, or to communicate with other animals. Many animals both groom themselves, eat or drink by licking.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Bark (sound)</span> Sound mainly produced by Dogs

A bark is a sound most often produced by dogs. Other animals that make this noise include, but are not limited to, wolves, coyotes, foxes, seals, and barking owls. "Bark" is also a verb that describes the sound of many canids.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Petit Basset Griffon Vendéen</span> Dog breed

The Petit Basset Griffon Vendéen, or PBGV, is a breed of dog of the scent hound type, bred to trail hares in bramble-filled terrain of the Vendée district of France. The breed is known in the United States as "Petit" or "PBGV," in England as "Roughie," and in Denmark as "Griffon" or "Petit". The PBGV is one of six types of "basset"-type breeds recognised by the Fédération Cynologique Internationale (FCI).

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Human–animal communication</span> Verbal and non-verbal interspecies communication

Human–animal communication is the communication observed between humans and other animals, ranging from non-verbal cues and vocalizations to the use of language.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">West Siberian Laika</span> Dog breed

The West Siberian Laika or WSL, is a breed of spitz–type hunting dog. Russian publications indicate that the term West Siberian Laika loosely applied to hunting dogs originating with the Mansi and Khanty people in Ural and West Siberia, but there were no standards or registrations of WSL as such until 1930. Then WWII disrupted it for a while, but systematic breeding with registrations resumed after the war ended, in 1946. This was the time the breed began taking modern shape. Before that hunters only knew of Mansi Laika and Khanty Laika. In early 1960 many hunters in Ural still preferred the term Mansi Laika, when speaking of West Siberian Laika. In Russian language, the term Laika originated from the word layat that means to bark. The word Laika simply means barker. Any hunting Laika is a bark pointer. It is a versatile dog depending on use and environment, but in certain parts of the country they have become more specialized.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Cat communication</span> Feline means of sending or receiving information

Cats need to communicate for a variety of reasons, including to show happiness, to express anger, to solicit attention, and to observe potential prey; cats collaborate, play, and share resources with each other. When they communicate with humans, they do so to get what they need or want, such as food, water, attention, or play.

Dogs, as with all mammals, have natural odors. Natural dog odor can be unpleasant to dog owners, especially when dogs are kept inside the home, as some people are not used to being exposed to the natural odor of a non-human species living in proximity to them. Dogs may also develop unnatural odors as a result of skin disease or other disorders or may become contaminated with odors from other sources in their environment.

Growling is a low, guttural vocalization produced by animals as an aggressive warning but can also be found in other contexts such as playful behaviors or mating. Different animals will use growling in specific contexts as a form of communication. In humans, low or dull rumbling noises may also be emitted when they are discontent with something or they are angry, although this human sound is often termed "groaning".

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Dog behavior</span> Internally coordinated responses of dogs to internal and external stimuli

Dog behavior is the internally coordinated responses of individuals or groups of domestic dogs to internal and external stimuli. It has been shaped by millennia of contact with humans and their lifestyles. As a result of this physical and social evolution, dogs have acquired the ability to understand and communicate with humans. Behavioral scientists have uncovered a wide range of social-cognitive abilities in domestic dogs.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Gunnison's prairie dog</span> Species of rodent

Gunnison's prairie dog is one of five species of prairie dog. This species belongs to the squirrel family of rodents, and are predominantly related to the North American and Eurasian ground squirrels. Gunnison's prairie dogs are primarily distributed in the Four Corners region of the United States.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Cat behavior</span> Behavior of cats

Cat behavior is a cat's behavior and responses to events and other stimuli. Cat behavior includes body language, elimination habits, aggression, play, communication, hunting, grooming, urine marking, and face rubbing. It varies among individuals, colonies, and breeds.

Calming signals is a term conceived by Norwegian dog trainer and canine ethologist, Turid Rugaas, to describe the patterns of behavior used by dogs when interacting with each other in environments that cause heightened stress and when conveying their desires or intentions. The term has been used interchangeably with "appeasement signals." Calming signals, or appeasement signals, are communicative cues used by dogs to de-escalate aggressive encounters or to prevent the development of aggressive encounters completely. Calming signals are performed by one dog and directed towards one or more individual(s), which could be dogs or individuals of other species, such as humans. When calming signals are ignored, a dog may display warning signals of aggression, and this has the potential to escalate to outright conflict between individuals.

The body language of dogs is one form of non-verbal communication whereby dogs can express emotions and intentions through bodily movements. It refers to the interpretation of posture and behaviour of species in the genus Canis. This form of visual communication is generally used for identifying emotions and intentions of domestic dogs, though it can also be applied to wild canines such as wolves. Understanding the body language of dogs is particularly important in preventing dog bites, especially of children. This communication can occur between dogs, or during a dog-human interaction. Such movements primarily involve the tail, the ears, and the head/body. Tail-wagging is a common tail movement used by dogs to communicate. Additionally, ear flattening or heightening are typical movements made using the ears. In terms of the head/body, it is of interest to study turning of the head, as well as the overall posture of the dog.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Tail wagging by dogs</span> Dog behaviour

Tail wagging is the behavior of the dog observed as its tail moves back and forth in the same plane. Within Canidae, specifically Canis lupus familiaris, the tail plays multiple roles, which can include balance, and communication. It is considered a social signal. The behaviour can be categorized by vigorous movement or slight movement of the tip of the tail. Tail wagging can also occur in circular motions, and when the tail is held at maximum height, neutral height, or between the legs.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Wolf communication</span> Overview about the communication of wolves

Wolves communicate using vocalizations, body postures, scent, touch, and taste. The lunar phases have no effect on wolf vocalisation. Despite popular belief, wolves do not howl at the Moon. Gray wolves howl to assemble the pack, usually before and after hunts, to pass on an alarm particularly at a den site, to locate each other during a storm or while crossing unfamiliar territory, and to communicate across great distances. Other vocalisations include growls, barks and whines. Wolves do not bark as loudly or continuously as dogs do but they bark a few times and then retreat from a perceived danger. Aggressive or self-assertive wolves are characterized by their slow and deliberate movements, high body posture and raised hackles, while submissive ones carry their bodies low, sleeken their fur, and lower their ears and tail. Raised leg urination is considered to be one of the most important forms of scent communication in the wolf, making up 60–80% of all scent marks observed.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Howling</span> Animal sound

Howling is a vocal form of animal communication seen in most canines, particularly wolves, coyotes, foxes, and dogs, as well as cats and some species of monkeys. Howls are lengthy sustained sounds, loud and audible over long distances, often with some variation in pitch over the length of the sound. Howling is generally used by animals that engage in this behavior to signal their positions to one another, to call the pack to assemble, or to note their territory. The behavior is occasionally copied by humans, and has been noted to have varying degrees of significance in human culture.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Olfactic communication</span> Social interaction through smell

Olfactic communication is a channel of nonverbal communication referring to the various ways people and animals communicate and engage in social interaction through their sense of smell. Our human olfactory sense is one of the most phylogenetically primitive and emotionally intimate of the five senses; the sensation of smell is thought to be the most matured and developed human sense.

References

  1. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65 66 67 68 69 70 71 72 73 74 75 76 77 78 79 80 81 82 Coren, Stanley (2012). How To Speak Dog. New York: Simon and Schuster. ISBN   9781471109416.
  2. Pageat, Patrick; Gaultier, Emmanuel (March 2003). "Current research in canine and feline pheromones". Veterinary Clinics of North America: Small Animal Practice. 33 (2): 187–211. doi:10.1016/S0195-5616(02)00128-6. PMID   12701508.
  3. Kaminski, Juliane; Waller, Bridget M.; Diogo, Rui; Hartstone-Rose, Adam; Burrows, Anne M. (2019). "Evolution of facial muscle anatomy in dogs". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences . 116 (29): 14677–14681. Bibcode:2019PNAS..11614677K. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1820653116 . PMC   6642381 . PMID   31209036.
  4. Udell, MA; Wynne, CD (March 2008). "A review of domestic dogs' (Canis familiaris) human-like behaviors: or why behavior analysts should stop worrying and love their dogs". Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior . 89 (2): 247–61. doi:10.1901/jeab.2008.89-247. PMC   2251326 . PMID   18422021.
  5. Hare, Brian (23 June 2016). "From nonhuman to human mind. What changed and why?". Current Directions in Psychological Science . 16 (2): 60–64. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00476.x. hdl: 11858/00-001M-0000-000F-FE6B-B . S2CID   3402472.
  6. Hare, Brian; Tomasello, Michael (2005). "Human-like social skills in dogs?". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 9 (9): 439–44. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.07.003. PMID   16061417. S2CID   9311402.
  7. Miklósi, A'.; Polgárdi, R.; Topál, J.; Csányi, V. (1 December 2000). "Intentional behaviour in dog-human communication: an experimental analysis of "showing" behaviour in the dog". Animal Cognition . 3 (3): 159–166. doi:10.1007/s100710000072. S2CID   18448656.
  8. Miklósi, Á.; Topál, J.; Csányi, V. (June 2004). "Comparative social cognition: what can dogs teach us?". Animal Behaviour . 67 (6): 995–1004. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.10.008. S2CID   15248027.
  9. Marler, Peter (1970). "A comparative approach to vocal learning: Song development in white-crowned sparrows". Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology . 71 (2, Pt.2): 1–25. doi:10.1037/h0029144.
  10. Lorenz, Konrad (1965). Evolution and modification of behavior . University of Chicago Press.
  11. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 Siniscalchi, Marcello; d’Ingeo, Serenella; Minunno, Michele; Quaranta, Angelo (31 July 2018). "Communication in Dogs". Animals. 8 (8): 131. doi: 10.3390/ani8080131 . ISSN   2076-2615. PMC   6116041 . PMID   30065156.
  12. Butterworth, George (2003). "Pointing is the royal road to language for babies".{{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help)
  13. Lakatos, Gabriella (2009). "A comparative approach to dogs' (Canis familiaris) and human infants' comprehension of various forms of pointing gestures". Animal Cognition. 12 (4): 621–31. doi:10.1007/s10071-009-0221-4. PMID   19343382. S2CID   18078591.
  14. Faragó, T; Pongrácz P; Miklósi Á; Huber L; Virányi Z; Range, F (2010). Giurfa, Martin (ed.). "Dogs' Expectation about Signalers' Body Size by Virtue of Their Growls". PLOS ONE. 5 (12): e15175. Bibcode:2010PLoSO...515175F. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0015175 . PMC   3002277 . PMID   21179521.
  15. "What dogs are saying Scientific American". Scientific American . Retrieved 9 August 2015.
  16. Brian Hare; Vanessa Woods (8 February 2013), "What Are Dogs Saying When They Bark? [Excerpt]", Scientific America, retrieved 17 March 2015
  17. 1 2 Katherine Sanderson (23 May 2008), "Humans can judge a dog by its growl", Nature, doi:10.1038/news.2008.852 research available here Archived 2 April 2015 at the Wayback Machine
  18. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 Rugaas, Turid (2006). On talking terms with dogs : calming signals (2nd ed.). Wenatchee, Wash.: Dogwise Pub. ISBN   1929242360.
  19. Eatron, Barry. Dominance in dogs: Fact or Fiction. Dogwise Publishing. ASIN   B011T6R50G.
  20. Rousseau, Steph. "Digging the dirt on dominance". Pet Dog Trainers of Europe Blog.
  21. "Asymmetric tail-wagging responses by dogs to different emotive stimuli", Current Biology, 17(6), 20 March 2007, pp R199-R201
  22. Robert L. Robbins, "Vocal Communication in Free-Ranging African Wild Dogs", Behavior, vol. 137, No. 10 (October 2000), pp. 1271-1298.
  23. J.A. Cohen and M.W. Fox, "Vocalizations in Wild Canids and Possible Effects of Domestication," Behavioural Processes, vol. 1 (1976), pp. 77-92.
  24. John B. Theberge and J. Bruce Falls, "Howling as a Means of Communication in Timber Wolves," American Zoologist, vol. 7, no. 2 (May 1967), pp. 331-338.
  25. P.N. Lehner, "Coyote vocalizations: a lexicon and comparisons with other canids," Animal Behavior, vol. 26 (1978) pp. 712-722.
  26. H. McCarley, "Long distance vocalization of coyotes (Canis latrans)," J. Mammal., vol. 56 (1975), pp. 847-856.
  27. Charles Fergus, "Probing Question: Why do coyotes howl?" Penn State News (15 January 2007).
  28. Fox, Michael W. (1971). Behaviour of Wolves, Dogs, and Related Canids (1st United States ed.). New York: Harper & Row. pp. 183–206. ISBN   0-89874-686-8.
  29. Rugaas, Turid (December 2005). On talking terms with dogs : calming signals (Second ed.). Wenatchee, Wash. ISBN   1-929242-36-0. OCLC   63519566.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link)
  30. Eldredge, Debra (14 March 2019). "Why Do Dogs Sneeze When They're Excited?". Dogster. Retrieved 22 November 2020.
  31. 1 2 Serpell, James (1995). The Domestic Dog: Its Evolution, Behaviour and Interactions with People. Cambridge University Press. ISBN   978-0-521-42537-7.
  32. L. David Mech; Luigi Boitani (1 October 2010). Wolves: Behavior, Ecology, and Conservation. University of Chicago Press. ISBN   978-0-226-51698-1.
  33. 1 2 3 Heath, Sarah (2007). "Understanding pheromones". Veterinary Nursing Journal. 22 (9): 22–23. doi:10.1080/17415349.2007.11013611. S2CID   219319909 via Taylor and Francis Group.
  34. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Petak, Irena (1 June 2010). "Patterns of carnivores' communication and potential significance for domestic dogs". Periodicum Biologorum.
  35. 1 2 3 Pageat, Patrick; Gaultier, Emmanuel (2003). "Current research in canine and feline pheromones". The Veterinary Clinics of North America. Small Animal Practice. 33 (2): 187–211. doi:10.1016/s0195-5616(02)00128-6. ISSN   0195-5616. PMID   12701508 via PubMed.
  36. 1 2 3 4 5 Kokocińska-Kusiak, Agata; Woszczyło, Martyna; Zybala, Mikołaj; Maciocha, Julia; Barłowska, Katarzyna (2021). "Canine Olfaction: Physiology, Behavior, and Possibilities for Practical Applications". Animals. 11 (8): 2463. doi: 10.3390/ani11082463 . PMC   8388720 . PMID   34438920.
  37. 1 2 Siniscalchi, Marcello; d’Ingeo, Serenella; Minunno, Michele; Quaranta, Angelo (31 July 2018). "Communication in Dogs". Animals. 8 (8): 131. doi: 10.3390/ani8080131 . ISSN   2076-2615. PMC   6116041 . PMID   30065156.