Dog intelligence

Last updated

Dog intelligence or dog cognition is the process in dogs of acquiring information and conceptual skills, and storing them in memory, retrieving, combining and comparing them, and using them in new situations. [1]

Contents

Studies have shown that dogs display many behaviors associated with intelligence. They have advanced memory skills, and are able to read and react appropriately to human body language such as gesturing and pointing, and to understand human voice commands. Dogs demonstrate a theory of mind by engaging in deception, and self-awareness by detecting their own smell during the "sniff test", a proposed olfactory equivalent to the mirror test.

Evolutionary perspective

Dogs have often been used in studies of cognition, including research on perception, awareness, memory, and learning, notably research on classical and operant conditioning. In the course of this research, behavioral scientists uncovered a surprising set of social-cognitive abilities in the domestic dog, abilities that are neither possessed by dogs' closest canine relatives nor by other highly intelligent mammals such as great apes. Rather, these skills resemble some of the social-cognitive skills of human children. [2] This may be an example of convergent evolution, which happens when distantly related species independently evolve similar solutions to the same problems. For example, fish, penguins and dolphins have each separately evolved flippers as solution to the problem of moving through the water. With dogs and humans, we may see psychological convergence; that is, dogs have evolved to be cognitively more similar to humans than we are to our closest genetic relatives. [3] :60 [4]

However, it is questionable whether the cognitive evolution of humans and animals may be called "independent". The cognitive capacities of dogs have inevitably been shaped by millennia of contact with humans. [5] [6] As a result of this physical and social evolution, many dogs readily respond to social cues common to humans, [7] [8] [9] [10] quickly learn the meaning of words, [11] show cognitive bias [12] and exhibit emotions that seem to reflect those of humans. [13]

Research suggests that domestic dogs may have lost some of their original cognitive abilities once they joined humans. For example, one study showed compelling evidence that dingoes (Canis dingo) can outperform domestic dogs in non-social problem-solving experiments. Another study indicated that after being trained to solve a simple manipulation task, dogs that are faced with an unsolvable version of the same problem look at a nearby human, while socialized wolves do not. Thus, modern domestic dogs seem to use humans to solve some of their problems for them. [3] [14]

In 2014, a whole genome study of the DNA differences between wolves and dogs found that dogs did not show a reduced fear response; they showed greater synaptic plasticity. Synaptic plasticity is widely believed to be the cellular correlate of learning and memory, and this change may have altered the learning and memory abilities of dogs. [15]

Most modern research on dog cognition has focused on pet dogs living in human homes in developed countries, a small fraction of the dog population. Dogs from other populations may show different cognitive behaviors. [16] Breed differences possibly could impact on spatial learning and memory abilities. [17]

Studies history

The first intelligence test for dogs was developed in 1976. It included measurements of short-term memory, agility, and ability to solve problems such as detouring to a goal. It also assessed the ability of a dog to adapt to new conditions and cope with emotionally difficult situations. The test was administered to 100 dogs and standardized, and breed norms were developed. [18] Stanley Coren used surveys done by dog obedience judges to rank dog breeds by intelligence and published the results in his 1994 book The Intelligence of Dogs .

Perception

Perception refers to mental processes through which incoming sensory information is organized and interpreted in order to represent and understand the environment. [19] Perception includes such processes as the selection of information through attention, the organization of sensory information through grouping, and the identification of events and objects. In the dog, olfactory information (the sense of smell) is particularly salient (compared with humans) but the dog's senses also include vision, hearing, taste, touch and proprioception. There is also evidence that dogs sense the Earth's magnetic field.

One researcher has proposed that dogs perceive the passing of time through the dissipation of smells. [20] [21]

Awareness

The concept of object permanence is the ability of an animal to understand that objects continue to exist even when they have moved outside of their field of view. This ability is not present at birth, and developmental psychologist Jean Piaget described six stages in the development of object permanence in human infants. A similar approach has been used with dogs, and there is evidence that dogs go through similar stages and reach the advanced fifth stage by an age of 8 weeks. At this stage they can track "successive visible displacement" in which the experimenter moves the object behind multiple screens before leaving it behind the last one. It is unclear whether dogs reach Stage 6 of Piaget's object permanence development model. [22] [23]

A study in 2013 indicated that dogs appear to recognize other dogs regardless of breed, size, or shape, and distinguish them from other animals. [24]

In 2014, a study using magnetic resonance imaging demonstrated that voice-response areas exist in the brains of dogs and that they show a response pattern in the anterior temporal voice areas that is similar to that in humans. [25]

Dogs can pass the "sniff test" suggesting potential self-awareness in the olfactory sense, and also show awareness of the size and movement of their bodies. [26]

Social cognition

Social learning: observation and rank

A dog being instructed by its owner to resist eating a dog biscuit Dog with treat.jpg
A dog being instructed by its owner to resist eating a dog biscuit

Dogs are capable of learning through simple reinforcement (e.g., classical or operant conditioning), but they also learn by watching humans and other dogs. [23] [27]

One study investigated whether dogs engaged in partnered play would adjust their behavior to the attention-state of their partner. The experimenters observed that play signals were only sent when the dog was holding the attention of its partner. If the partner was distracted, the dog instead engaged in attention-getting behavior before sending a play signal. [28]

Puppies learn behaviors quickly by following examples set by experienced dogs. [23] This form of intelligence is not particular to those tasks dogs have been bred to perform, but can be generalized to various abstract problems. For example, Dachshund puppies were set the problem of pulling a cart by tugging on an attached piece of ribbon in order to get a reward from inside the cart. Puppies that watched an experienced dog perform this task learned the task fifteen times faster than those left to solve the problem on their own. [23] [29]

The social rank of dogs affects their performance in social learning situations. In social groups with a clear hierarchy, dominant individuals are the more influential demonstrators and the knowledge transfer tends to be unidirectional, from higher rank to lower. In a problem-solving experiment, dominant dogs generally performed better than subordinates when they observed a human demonstrator's actions, a finding that reflects the dominance of the human in dog-human groups. Subordinate dogs learn best from the dominant dog that is adjacent in the hierarchy. [30]

Following human cues

Dogs show human-like social cognition in various ways. [8] [9] [31] For example, dogs can react appropriately to human body language such as gesturing and pointing, and they also understand human voice commands. [32] In one study, puppies were presented with a box, and shown that, when a handler pressed a lever, a ball would roll out of the box. The handler then allowed the puppy to play with the ball, making it an intrinsic reward. The pups were then allowed to interact with the box. Roughly three quarters of the puppies subsequently touched the lever, and over half successfully released the ball, compared to only 6% in a control group that did not watch the human manipulate the lever. [33]

Similarly, dogs may be guided by cues indicating the direction of a human's attention. [23] In one task a reward was hidden under one of two buckets. The experimenter then indicated the location of the reward by tapping the bucket, pointing to the bucket, nodding at the bucket, or simply looking at the bucket. The dogs followed these signals, performing better than chimpanzees, wolves, and human infants at this task; even puppies with limited exposure to humans performed well. [34] :1634–6

Dogs can follow the direction of pointing by humans. New Guinea singing dogs are a half-wild proto-dog endemic to the remote alpine regions of New Guinea and these can follow human pointing as can Australian dingoes. These both demonstrate an ability to read human gestures that arose early in domestication without human selection. Dogs and wolves have also been shown to follow more complex pointing made with body parts other than the human arm and hand (e.g. elbow, knee, foot). [35] Dogs tend to follow hand/arm pointed directions more when combined with eye signaling as well. In general, dogs seem to use human cues as an indication on where to go and what to do. [36] Overall, dogs appear to have several cognitive skills necessary to understand communication as information; however, findings on dogs' understanding of referentiality and others' mental states are controversial and it is not clear whether dog themselves communicate with informative motives. [37]

For canines to perform well on traditional human-guided tasks (e.g. following the human point) both relevant lifetime experiences with humans—including socialization to humans during the critical phase for social development—and opportunities to associate human body parts with certain outcomes (such as food being provided by humans, a human throwing or kicking a ball, etc.) are required. [38]

In 2016, a study of water rescue dogs that respond to words or gestures found that the dogs would respond to the gesture rather than the verbal command. [39]

Memory

Episodic memory

Dogs have demonstrated episodic-like memory by recalling past events that included the complex actions of humans. [40] In a 2019 study, a correlation has been shown between the size of the dog and the functions of memory and self-control, with larger dogs performing significantly better than smaller dogs in these functions. However, in the study brain size did not predict a dog's ability to follow human pointing gestures, nor was it associated with their inferential and physical reasoning abilities. [41] A 2018 study on canine cognitive abilities found that various animals, including pigs, pigeons and chimpanzees, are able to remember the what, where and when of an event, which dogs cannot do. [42]

Learning and using words

Various studies have shown that dogs readily learn the names of objects and can retrieve an item from among many others when given its name. For example, in 2008, Betsy, a Border Collie, knew over 345 words by the retrieval test, and she was also able to connect an object with a photographic image of the object, despite having seen neither before. [43] In another study, a dog watched as experimenters handed an object back and forth to each other while using the object's name in a sentence. The dog subsequently retrieved the item given its name. [44]

In humans, "fast mapping" is the ability to form quick and rough hypotheses about the meaning of a new word after only a single exposure. In 2004, a study with Rico, a Border Collie, showed he was able to fast map. Rico initially knew the labels of over 200 items. He inferred the names of novel items by exclusion, that is, by knowing that the novel item was the one that he did not already know. Rico correctly retrieved such novel items immediately and four weeks after the initial exposure. Rico was also able to interpret phrases such as "fetch the sock" by its component words (rather than considering its utterance to be a single word). Rico could also give the sock to a specified person. This performance is comparable to that of 3-year-old humans. [11]

Chaser the Border Collie Chaser the BC, snow full body.jpg
Chaser the Border Collie

In 2013, a study documented the learning and memory capabilities of a Border Collie, "Chaser", who had learned the names and could associate by verbal command over 1,000 words at the time of its publishing. Chaser was documented as capable of learning the names of new objects "by exclusion", and capable of linking nouns to verbs. It is argued that central to the understanding of the Border Collie's remarkable accomplishments is the dog's breeding background—collies bred for herding work are uniquely suited for intellectual tasks like word association which may require the dog to work "at a distance" from their human companions, and the study credits this dog's selective breeding in addition to rigorous training for her intellectual prowess. [45]

Some research has suggested that while dogs can easily make a distinction between familiar known words and nonsensical dissimilar words, they struggle to differentiate between known familiar words and nonsense words that differ by only a single sound, as measurements of the dogs' brain activity showed no difference in response between a known word and a similar but nonsensical word. This would give dogs the word processing capability equivalent to the average 14-month human infant. [46] An fMRI study found that the dog brain distinguished, without training, a familiar from an unfamiliar language. The study also found that older dogs were better at discriminating one language from the other, suggesting an effect of the amount of exposition to the language. [47]

Emotions

Mix-breed dog showing curiosity and fear while staring at a cat. Cautious dog.jpg
Mix-breed dog showing curiosity and fear while staring at a cat.

Studies suggest that dogs feel complex emotions, like jealousy and anticipation. [48] [49] However, behavioral evidence of seemingly human emotions must be interpreted with care. For example, in his 1996 book Good Natured , ethologist Frans de Waal discusses an experiment on guilt and reprimands conducted on a female Siberian Husky. The dog had the habit of shredding newspapers, and when her owner returned home to find the shredded papers and scold her she would act guilty. However, when the owner himself shredded the papers without the dog's knowledge, the dog "acted just as 'guilty' as when she herself had created the mess." De Waal concludes that the dog did not display true guilt as humans understand it, but rather simply the anticipation of reprimand. [50]

One limitation in the study of emotions in non-human animals, is that they cannot verbalize to express their feelings. However, dogs' emotions can be studied indirectly through cognitive tests, called cognitive bias test, which measure a cognitive bias and allow to make inference about the mood of the animal. Researchers have found that dogs suffering from separation anxiety have a more negative cognitive bias, compared to dogs without separation anxiety. [51] On the other hand, when dogs' separation anxiety is treated with medications and behavior therapy, their cognitive bias becomes less negative than before treatment. [52] Also administration of oxytocin, rather than a placebo, induces a more positive cognitive bias and positive expectation in dogs. [53] It is therefore suggested that the cognitive bias test can be used to monitor positive emotional states and therefore welfare in dogs. [53] [54]

There is evidence that dogs can discriminate the emotional expressions of human faces. [55] In addition, they seem to respond to faces in somewhat the same way as humans. For example, humans tend to gaze at the right side of a person's face, which may be related to the use of right brain hemisphere for facial recognition. Research indicates that dogs also fixate the right side of a human face, but not that of other dogs or other animals. Dogs are the only non-primate species known to do so. [56]

Problem solving

Dogs learned to activate a robot to deliver them food rewards. [57]

Dogs have been observed to learn to use public transport to arrive at a desired destination. In Moscow out of 500 dogs, 20 learned to commute. [58] [59] Eclipse, a black labrador in Seattle, would occasionally ride the bus ahead of its owner when eager to get to the dog park. [60] Ratty, a Jack Russell terrier in Yorkshire, England, traveled 5 mi (8.0 km) by bus to be fed at two pubs. [61]

Captive-raised dingoes (Canis dingo) can outperform domestic dogs in non-social problem-solving. [62] Another study indicated that after undergoing training to solve a simple manipulation task, dogs faced with an unsolvable version of the same problem look at the human, whereas socialized wolves do not. [31] [63] Modern domestic dogs use humans to solve their problems for them. [3] [64] Sex-specific dynamics are an important contributor to individual differences in cognitive performance of pet dogs in repeated problem-solving tasks. [65]

Learning by inference

Dogs have been shown to learn by making inferences in a similar way to children. [66] [3] :170–180Dogs have the ability to train themselves and learn behaviors through watching and interacting with other dogs. [67] In one study dogs were first introduced to a setting with two bowls, one with a reward. After four demonstrations to show dogs there was at least one reward in one of the bowls, the empty bowl was lifted and shown then, laid down, 33% of dogs correctly picked the reward bowl more often than the empty bowl. [68]

Theory of mind

Theory of mind is the ability to attribute mental states—beliefs, intents, desires, pretending, knowledge, etc.—to oneself and others and to understand that others have beliefs, desires, intentions, and perspectives that are different from one's own. [69] There is some evidence that dogs demonstrate a theory of mind by engaging in deception. For example, one observer reported that a dog hid a stolen treat by sitting on it until the rightful owner of the treat left the room. [23] Although this could have been accidental, it suggests that the thief understood that the treat's owner would be unable to find the treat if it were out of view. [23] [28] A study found that dogs are able to discriminate an object that a human partner is looking for based on its relevance for the partner and they are more keen on indicating an object that is relevant to the partner compared to an irrelevant one; this suggests that dogs might have a rudimentary version of some of the skills necessary for theory of mind. [70] Dogs can figure what a human is seeing. [71]

Tool use

Dogs have been trained to follow commands to drive cars. [72] [73] [74] [75]

See also

Related Research Articles

Comparative psychology is the scientific study of the behavior and mental processes of non-human animals, especially as these relate to the phylogenetic history, adaptive significance, and development of behavior. The phrase comparative psychology may be employed in either a narrow or a broad meaning. In its narrow meaning, it refers to the study of the similarities and differences in the psychology and behavior of different species. In a broader meaning, comparative psychology includes comparisons between different biological and socio-cultural groups, such as species, sexes, developmental stages, ages, and ethnicities. Research in this area addresses many different issues, uses many different methods and explores the behavior of many different species, from insects to primates.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Animal cognition</span> Intelligence of non-human animals

Animal cognition encompasses the mental capacities of non-human animals, including insect cognition. The study of animal conditioning and learning used in this field was developed from comparative psychology. It has also been strongly influenced by research in ethology, behavioral ecology, and evolutionary psychology; the alternative name cognitive ethology is sometimes used. Many behaviors associated with the term animal intelligence are also subsumed within animal cognition.

In psychology and philosophy, theory of mind refers to the capacity to understand other people by ascribing mental states to them. A theory of mind includes the understanding that others' beliefs, desires, intentions, emotions, and thoughts may be different from one's own. Possessing a functional theory of mind is crucial for success in everyday human social interactions. People utilize a theory of mind when analyzing, judging, and inferring others' behaviors.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Dog training</span> Practice of teaching behaviors to dogs

Dog training is a kind of animal training, the application of behavior analysis which uses the environmental events of antecedents and consequences to modify the dog behavior, either for it to assist in specific activities or undertake particular tasks, or for it to participate effectively in contemporary domestic life. While training dogs for specific roles dates back to Roman times at least, the training of dogs to be compatible household pets developed with suburbanization in the 1950s.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Metacognition</span> Self-awareness about thinking, higher-order thinking skills

Metacognition is an awareness of one's thought processes and an understanding of the patterns behind them. The term comes from the root word meta, meaning "beyond", or "on top of". Metacognition can take many forms, such as reflecting on one's ways of thinking, and knowing when and how oneself and others use particular strategies for problem-solving. There are generally two components of metacognition: (1) cognitive conceptions and (2) cognitive regulation system. Research has shown that both components of metacognition play key roles in metaconceptual knowledge and learning. Metamemory, defined as knowing about memory and mnemonic strategies, is an important aspect of metacognition.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Dog</span> Domesticated species of canid

The dog is a domesticated descendant of the wolf. Also called the domestic dog, it was selectively bred from an extinct population of wolves during the Late Pleistocene by hunter-gatherers. The dog was the first species to be domesticated by humans, over 14,000 years ago and before the development of agriculture. Experts estimate that due to their long association with humans, dogs have gained the ability to thrive on a starch-rich diet that would be inadequate for other canids.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Emotion in animals</span> Research into similarities between non-human and human emotions

Emotion is defined as any mental experience with high intensity and high hedonic content. The existence and nature of emotions in non-human animals are believed to be correlated with those of humans and to have evolved from the same mechanisms. Charles Darwin was one of the first scientists to write about the subject, and his observational approach has since developed into a more robust, hypothesis-driven, scientific approach. Cognitive bias tests and learned helplessness models have shown feelings of optimism and pessimism in a wide range of species, including rats, dogs, cats, rhesus macaques, sheep, chicks, starlings, pigs, and honeybees. Jaak Panksepp played a large role in the study of animal emotion, basing his research on the neurological aspect. Mentioning seven core emotional feelings reflected through a variety of neuro-dynamic limbic emotional action systems, including seeking, fear, rage, lust, care, panic and play. Through brain stimulation and pharmacological challenges, such emotional responses can be effectively monitored.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Behavioral enrichment</span> Techniques for stimulating captive animals

Behavioral enrichment is an animal husbandry principle that seeks to enhance the quality of captive animal care by identifying and providing the environmental stimuli necessary for optimal psychological and physiological well-being. Enrichment can either be active or passive, depending on whether it requires direct contact between the animal and the enrichment. A variety of enrichment techniques are used to create desired outcomes similar to an animal's individual and species' history. Each of the techniques used is intended to stimulate the animal's senses similarly to how they would be activated in the wild. Provided enrichment may be seen in the form of auditory, olfactory, habitat factors, food, research projects, training, and objects.

Cat intelligence is the capacity of the domesticated cat to solve problems and adapt to its environment. Research has shown that feline intelligence includes the ability to acquire new behavior that applies knowledge to new situations, communicating needs and desires within a social group and responding to training.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Dog behavior</span> Internally coordinated responses of dogs to internal and external stimuli

Dog behavior is the internally coordinated responses of individuals or groups of domestic dogs to internal and external stimuli. It has been shaped by millennia of contact with humans and their lifestyles. As a result of this physical and social evolution, dogs have acquired the ability to understand and communicate with humans. Behavioral scientists have uncovered a wide range of social-cognitive abilities in domestic dogs.

Comparative cognition is the comparative study of the mechanisms and origins of cognition in various species, and is sometimes seen as more general than, or similar to, comparative psychology. From a biological point of view, work is being done on the brains of fruit flies that should yield techniques precise enough to allow an understanding of the workings of the human brain on a scale appreciative of individual groups of neurons rather than the more regional scale previously used. Similarly, gene activity in the human brain is better understood through examination of the brains of mice by the Seattle-based Allen Institute for Brain Science, yielding the freely available Allen Brain Atlas. This type of study is related to comparative cognition, but better classified as one of comparative genomics. Increasing emphasis in psychology and ethology on the biological aspects of perception and behavior is bridging the gap between genomics and behavioral analysis.

Socialization of animals is the process of training animals so that they can be kept in close relationship to humans.

<i>The Intelligence of Dogs</i> Book by Stanley Coren

The Intelligence of Dogs: A Guide to the Thoughts, Emotions, and Inner Lives of Our Canine Companions is a 1994 book on dog intelligence by Stanley Coren, a professor of canine psychology at the University of British Columbia. The book explains Coren's theories about the differences in intelligence between various breeds of dogs. Coren published a second edition in 2006.

Perspective-taking is the act of perceiving a situation or understanding a concept from an alternative point of view, such as that of another individual.

Sex differences in cognition are widely studied in the current scientific literature. Biological and genetic differences in combination with environment and culture have resulted in the cognitive differences among males and females. Among biological factors, hormones such as testosterone and estrogen may play some role mediating these differences. Among differences of diverse mental and cognitive abilities, the largest or most well known are those relating to spatial abilities, social cognition and verbal skills and abilities.

Theory of mind in animals is an extension to non-human animals of the philosophical and psychological concept of theory of mind (ToM), sometimes known as mentalisation or mind-reading. It involves an inquiry into whether non-human animals have the ability to attribute mental states to themselves and others, including recognition that others have mental states that are different from their own. To investigate this issue experimentally, researchers place non-human animals in situations where their resulting behavior can be interpreted as supporting ToM or not.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Pointing</span> Gesture

Pointing is a gesture specifying a direction from a person's body, usually indicating a location, person, event, thing or idea. It typically is formed by extending the arm, hand, and index finger, although it may be functionally similar to other hand gestures. Types of pointing may be subdivided according to the intention of the person, as well as by the linguistic function it serves.

The evolution of cognition is the process by which life on Earth has gone from organisms with little to no cognitive function to a greatly varying display of cognitive function that we see in organisms today. Animal cognition is largely studied by observing behavior, which makes studying extinct species difficult. The definition of cognition varies by discipline; psychologists tend define cognition by human behaviors, while ethologists have widely varying definitions. Ethological definitions of cognition range from only considering cognition in animals to be behaviors exhibited in humans, while others consider anything action involving a nervous system to be cognitive.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Insect cognition</span> Mental capacity of insects

Insect cognition describes the mental capacities and study of those capacities in insects. The field developed from comparative psychology where early studies focused more on animal behavior. Researchers have examined insect cognition in bees, fruit flies, and wasps. 

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Clive Wynne</span> British-Australian ethologist

Clive D. L. Wynne is a British-Australian ethologist specializing in the behavior of dogs and their wild relatives. He has worked in the United States, Australia, and Europe, and is currently based at Arizona State University in Tempe, AZ. He was born and raised on the Isle of Wight, off the south coast of England, studied at University College London, and got his Ph.D. at Edinburgh University. He has studied the behavior of many species - ranging from pigeons to dunnarts, but starting around 2006 melded his childhood love of dogs with his professional training and now studies and teaches about the behavior of dogs and their wild relatives.

References

  1. Humphreys, L. G. (1979). "The construct of general intelligence". Intelligence. 3 (2): 105–120. doi:10.1016/0160-2896(79)90009-6.
  2. Tomasello, M.; Kaminski, J. (2009). "Like Infant, Like Dog". Science. 325 (5945): 1213–4. doi:10.1126/science.1179670. PMID   19729645. S2CID   206522649.
  3. 1 2 3 4 Hare, Brian; Woods, Venessa (2013). The Genius of Dogs. Penguin Publishing Group.
  4. Hare, B; Tomasello, M (2005). "Human-like social skills in dogs?". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 9 (9): 439–44. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.07.003. PMID   16061417. S2CID   9311402.
  5. Shipman P (2011) The Animal Connection. A New Perspective on What Makes Us Human. New York: W.W. Norton and Co
  6. Bradshaw J (2011) Dog Sense. How the New Science of Dog Behavior Can Make You a Better Friend. New York: Basic Books
  7. Topál, J.; Kis, A.; Oláh, K. (2014). "Dogs' Sensitivity to Human Ostensive Cues". The Social Dog. pp. 319–346. doi:10.1016/b978-0-12-407818-5.00011-5. ISBN   9780124078185.
  8. 1 2 Hare, B. (2002). "The Domestication of Social Cognition in Dogs" (PDF). Science. 298 (5598): 1634–6. Bibcode:2002Sci...298.1634H. doi:10.1126/science.1072702. PMID   12446914. S2CID   13369396.
  9. 1 2 Hare, Brian; Tomasello, Michael (September 2005). "Human-like social skills in dogs?". Trends in Cognitive Sciences . 9 (9): 439–444. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.07.003. PMID   16061417. S2CID   9311402.
  10. Téglás, Ernő; Gergely, Anna; Kupán, Krisztina; Miklósi, Ádám; Topál, József (February 2012). "Dogs' Gaze Following Is Tuned to Human Communicative Signals". Current Biology. 22 (3): 209–212. Bibcode:2012CBio...22..209T. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.12.018 . PMID   22226744.
  11. 1 2 Kaminski, J.; Call, J.; Fischer, J. (2004). "Word learning in a domestic dog: Evidence for "fast mapping"". Science. 304 (5677): 1682–1683. Bibcode:2004Sci...304.1682K. doi:10.1126/science.1097859. PMID   15192233. S2CID   31901162.
  12. Mendl, M.; Brooks, J.; Basse, C.; Burman, O.; Paul, E.; Blackwell, E.; Casey, R. (2010). "Dogs showing separation-related behaviour exhibit a 'pessimistic' cognitive bias". Current Biology . 20 (19): R839–R840. Bibcode:2010CBio...20.R839M. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.08.030 . PMID   20937467. S2CID   8638509.
  13. Bekoff, Marc (January 2007). The Emotional Lives of Animals: A Leading Scientist Explores Animal Joy, Sorrow, and Empathy — and Why They Matter . New World Library. ISBN   978-1-57731-502-5.
  14. Crawford, Amy. "Why Dogs are More Like Humans Than Wolves".
  15. Li, Y.; Wang, G.-D.; Wang, M.-S.; Irwin, D. M.; Wu, D.-D.; Zhang, Y.-P. (2014). "Domestication of the Dog from the Wolf Was Promoted by Enhanced Excitatory Synaptic Plasticity: A Hypothesis". Genome Biology and Evolution. 6 (11): 3115–3121. doi:10.1093/gbe/evu245. PMC   4255776 . PMID   25377939.
  16. Udell, M.A.R.; Dorey, N.R.; Wynne, C.D.L. (2010). "What did domestication do to dogs? A new account of dogs' sensitivity to human actions". Biological Reviews. 85 (2): 327–345. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.483.3002 . doi:10.1111/j.1469-185X.2009.00104.x. PMID   19961472. S2CID   11627064.
  17. Head, E.; Mehta, R.; Hartley, J.; Kameka, M.; Cummings, B.J.; Cotman, C.W.; Ruehl, W.W.; Milgram, N.W. (1995). "Spatial learning and memory as a function of age in the dog". Behavioral Neuroscience. 109 (5): 851–858. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.109.5.851. PMID   8554710.
  18. Coon, Kathy (1977). the dog intelligence test . Avon Books. ISBN   978-0-380-01903-8.
  19. Schacter, Daniel (2011). Psychology . Worth Publishers. ISBN   9781429237192.
  20. Horowitz, Alexandra (2016). "2-Smeller". Being a Dog: Following the Dog Into a World of Smell. Scribner New York. p. 29.
  21. Dahl, Melissa (2016). "Apparently Dogs Can Tell Time With Their Noses". The Science of Us - NYMag.
  22. Bensky, Miles K.; Gosling, Samuel D.; Sinn, David L. (2013). "Chapter 5: The world from a dog's point of view: A review and synthesis of dog cognition research". In Brockmann, H. Jane; et al. (eds.). Advances in the Study of Behavior. Vol. 45. Elsevier. pp. 209–406. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-407186-5.00005-7. ISBN   9780124071865.
  23. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Coren, Stanley (1995). The Intelligence of Dogs: A Guide to the Thoughts, Emotions, and Inner Lives of Our Canine Companions. Bantam Books. ISBN   978-0-553-37452-0.
  24. Coren, Stanley (2013-10-08). "Do Dogs Know The Difference Between Dogs and Other Animals?". Psychology Today. Retrieved 10 April 2015.
  25. "Dogs' brain scans reveal vocal responses". BBC News. 21 February 2014. Retrieved 9 August 2015.
  26. Gibeault, Stephanie. "Do Dogs Have Self-Awareness?". American Kennel Club. Retrieved 2024-02-25.
  27. "How Dogs Learn". National Animal Interest Alliance. Retrieved 4 July 2014.
  28. 1 2 Horowitz, Alexandra (2009). "Attention to attention in domestic dog (Canis familiaris) dyadic play". Animal Cognition. 12 (1): 107–18. doi:10.1007/s10071-008-0175-y. PMID   18679727. S2CID   207050813.
  29. Adler, Leonore Loeb; Adler, Helmut E. (2004). "Ontogeny of observational learning in the dog (Canis familiaris)". Developmental Psychobiology. 10 (3): 267–271. doi:10.1002/dev.420100310. PMID   863122.
  30. Pongrácz, Péter; Bánhegyi, Petra; Miklósi, Ádám (2012). "When rank counts—dominant dogs learn better from a human demonstrator in a two-action test". Behaviour . 149 (1): 111–132. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.456.670 . doi:10.1163/156853912X629148.
  31. 1 2 Miklósi, Adam; et al. (29 April 2003). "A simple reason for a big difference: wolves do not look back at humans, but dogs do". Current Biology . 13 (9): 763–766. Bibcode:2003CBio...13..763M. doi: 10.1016/S0960-9822(03)00263-X . PMID   12725735. S2CID   10200094.
  32. Gjersoe, Nathalia (Sep 23, 2013). "Dogs: an uncomplicated relationship". The Guardian .
  33. Kubinyi, E.; Topal, J. & Miklosi, A. (2003). "Dogs (canis familiaris) learn their owners via observation in a manipulation task". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 117 (2): 156–165. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.117.2.156. PMID   12856786.
  34. Hare, B.; Brown, M.; Williamson, C. & Tomasello, M. (Nov 2002). "The domestication of social cognition in dogs" (PDF). Science. 298 (5598): 1634–6. Bibcode:2002Sci...298.1634H. doi:10.1126/science.1072702. ISSN   0036-8075. PMID   12446914. S2CID   13369396.
  35. Udell, M. A. R; Spencer, J. M.; Dorey, N. R.; Wynne, C. D. L. (2012). "Human-Socialized Wolves Follow Diverse Human Gestures... And They May Not Be Alone". International Journal of Comparative Psychology. 25 (2): 97–117. doi: 10.46867/IJCP.2012.25.02.04 .
  36. Kaminski, Juliane; Nitzschner, Marie (2013). "Do dogs get the point? A review of dog–human communication ability". Learning and Motivation. 44 (4): 294–302. doi:10.1016/j.lmot.2013.05.001.
  37. Kaminski, Juliane; Piotti, Patrizia (2016). "Current Trends in Dog-Human Communication". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 25 (5): 322–326. doi:10.1177/0963721416661318. hdl: 2434/727094 . S2CID   151423286.
  38. Udell, M.A.R. (2014). "A Dog's-Eye View of Canine Cognition". In A. Horowitz (ed.). Domestic Dog Cognition and Behavior. Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg. pp. 221–240. doi:10.1007/978-3-642-53994-7_10. ISBN   978-3-642-53993-0.
  39. d'Aniello, Biagio; Scandurra, Anna; Alterisio, Alessandra; Valsecchi, Paola; Prato-Previde, Emanuela (2016). "The importance of gestural communication: A study of human–dog communication using incongruent information". Animal Cognition. 19 (6): 1231–1235. doi:10.1007/s10071-016-1010-5. PMID   27338818. S2CID   3913013.
  40. Fugazza, Claudia; Pogány, Ákos; Miklósi, Ádám (2016). "Recall of Others' Actions after Incidental Encoding Reveals Episodic-like Memory in Dogs". Current Biology. 26 (23): 3209–3213. Bibcode:2016CBio...26.3209F. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.09.057 . PMID   27889264.
  41. Horschler, Daniel J.; Hare, Brian; Call, Josep; Kaminski, Juliane; Miklósi, Ádám; MacLean, Evan L. (2019). "Absolute brain size predicts dog breed differences in executive function". Animal Cognition. 22 (2): 187–198. doi:10.1007/s10071-018-01234-1. hdl: 10150/631852 . PMID   30607673. S2CID   57428663.
  42. "Why your dog is not as clever as you thought". The Telegraph. 1 October 2018. Retrieved 12 May 2020.
  43. Morell, Virginia (March 2008). "Minds of their Own". National Geographic. Archived from the original on February 20, 2008. Retrieved 2008-10-13.
  44. McKinley, Sue; Young, Robert J (2003). "The efficacy of the model-rival method when compared to operant conditioning for training domestic dogs to perform a retrieval-selection task". AABS. 81 (4): 357–365. doi:10.1016/S0168-1591(02)00277-0.
  45. Pilley, John (2013). Chaser: Unlocking the genius of the dog who knows a thousand words. Houghton Mifflin Harcourt. ISBN   9780544102576.
  46. Magyari, L., Zs Huszár, A. Turzó, and A. Andics. "Event-related potentials reveal limited readiness to access phonetic details during word processing in dogs." Royal Society Open Science 7, no. 12 (2020): 200851.
  47. Cuaya, Laura V.; Hernández-Pérez, Raúl; Boros, Marianna; Deme, Andrea; Andics, Attila (2021-12-12). "Speech naturalness detection and language representation in the dog brain". NeuroImage. 248: 118811. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118811 . hdl: 10831/83069 . ISSN   1053-8119. PMID   34906714. S2CID   245022566.
  48. "Test reveals dogs' jealous side". BBC News. 2008-12-08. Retrieved 2010-05-01.
  49. Berns, Gregory S.; Brooks, Andrew M.; Spivak, Mark (2012-05-11). "Functional MRI in Awake Unrestrained Dogs". PLOS ONE. 7 (5): e38027. Bibcode:2012PLoSO...738027B. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0038027 . PMC   3350478 . PMID   22606363.
  50. de Waal, Frans (1996). Good Natured. Harvard University Press. pp.  107–108. ISBN   978-0-674-35660-3.
  51. Mendl, Michael; Brooks, Julie; Basse, Christine; Burman, Oliver; Paul, Elizabeth; Blackwell, Emily; Casey, Rachel (2010). "Dogs showing separation-related behaviour exhibit a 'pessimistic' cognitive bias". Current Biology. 20 (19): R839–R840. Bibcode:2010CBio...20.R839M. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.08.030 . PMID   20937467. S2CID   8638509.
  52. Karagiannis, Christos I.; Burman, Oliver HP; Mills, Daniel S. (2015-03-28). "Dogs with separation-related problems show a "less pessimistic" cognitive bias during treatment with fluoxetine (Reconcile™) and a behaviour modification plan". BMC Veterinary Research. 11: 80. doi: 10.1186/s12917-015-0373-1 . ISSN   1746-6148. PMC   4393593 . PMID   25889323.
  53. 1 2 Kis, Anna; Hernádi, Anna; Kanizsár, Orsolya; Gácsi, Márta; Topál, József (2015). "Oxytocin induces positive expectations about ambivalent stimuli (cognitive bias) in dogs" (PDF). Hormones and Behavior. 69: 1–7. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2014.12.004. PMID   25530486. S2CID   3723849.
  54. Piotti, P (2017). "Positive emotions and quality of life in dogs" (PDF). Animal Sentience. 2 (14): 17. doi: 10.51291/2377-7478.1281 .
  55. Müller, Corsin A.; Schmitt, Kira; Barber, Anjuli L.A.; Huber, Ludwig (2015). "Dogs can discriminate the emotional expressions of human faces". Current Biology. 25 (5): 601–605. Bibcode:2015CBio...25..601M. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.12.055 . PMID   25683806.
  56. K Guo; C Hall; S Hall; K Meints; D Mills (2007). "Left gaze bias in human infants, rhesus monkeys, and domestic dogs". Perception. 36 ECVP. Archived from the original on 15 July 2011. Retrieved 24 June 2010.
  57. Byrne, Ceara; Logas, Jacob; Freil, Larry; Allen, Courtney; Baltrusaitis, Melissa; Nguyen, Vi; Saad, Christopher; Jackson, Melody Moore (2020-01-10). "Dog Driven Robot: Towards Quantifying Problem-Solving Abilities in Dogs". Proceedings of the Sixth International Conference on Animal-Computer Interaction. ACI '19. New York, NY, USA: Association for Computing Machinery. pp. 1–5. doi:10.1145/3371049.3371063. ISBN   978-1-4503-7693-8.
  58. Sternthal, Susanne (January 16, 2010). "Moscow's stray dogs". Financial Times. Archived from the original on December 16, 2011. Retrieved December 16, 2011.{{cite web}}: CS1 maint: unfit URL (link)
  59. "Russia: Stray Dogs Master Complex Moscow Subway System - ABC News". Abcnews.go.com. 2010-03-19. Retrieved 2015-07-02.
  60. "Seattle Dog Figures Out Buses, Starts Riding Solo To The Dog Park". Huffingtonpost.com. 2015-01-13. Retrieved 2015-07-02.
  61. Stead, Mark (6 April 2010). "Ratty, the York bus-travelling dog who made headlines all over world dies". York Press. Retrieved 9 February 2017.
  62. Smith, B.; Litchfield, C. (2010). "How well do dingoes (Canis dingo) perform on the detour task". Animal Behaviour. 80: 155–162. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.04.017. S2CID   53153703.
  63. "Why dogs are more like humans than wolves". Smithsonian Magazine.
  64. "The brilliance of the dog mind". Scientific American.
  65. Duranton, Charlotte; Rödel, Heiko G.; Bedossa, Thierry; Belkhir, Séverine (2015). "Inverse sex effects on performance of domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) in a repeated problem-solving task". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 129 (1): 84–7. doi:10.1037/a0037825. PMID   25181448.
  66. Tiffany O'Callaghan (2013). "Survival of the Friendliest". New Scientist.
  67. Stanley., Coren, (2004). How dogs think : understanding the canine mind. New York: Free Press. ISBN   0743222326. OCLC 55588158.
  68. Junttila, Saara; Valros, Anna; Mäki, Katariina; Väätäjä, Heli; Reunanen, Elisa; Tiira, Katriina (2022-12-29). "Breed differences in social cognition, inhibitory control, and spatial problem-solving ability in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris)". Scientific Reports. 12 (1): 22529. Bibcode:2022NatSR..1222529J. doi:10.1038/s41598-022-26991-5. ISSN   2045-2322. PMC   9800387 . PMID   36581704.
  69. Premack, D. G.; Woodruff, G. (1978). "Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind?". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1 (4): 515–526. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x00076512 .
  70. Piotti, Patrizia; Kaminski, Juliane (2016-08-10). "Do Dogs Provide Information Helpfully?". PLOS ONE. 11 (8): e0159797. Bibcode:2016PLoSO..1159797P. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0159797 . ISSN   1932-6203. PMC   4980001 . PMID   27508932.
  71. Bräuer, Juliane; Kaminski, Juliane (2021). What Dogs Know. pp. 57–59. doi:10.1007/978-3-030-89533-4. ISBN   978-3-030-89532-7.
  72. "Why this dog is learning to drive". BBC News. Retrieved 2024-09-01.
  73. Eliot, Lance. "Those Three Clever Dogs Trained To Drive A Car Provide Valuable Lessons For AI Self-Driving Cars". Forbes. Retrieved 2024-09-01.
  74. "Rescue Dogs Taught How To Drive A Car". Sky News. Retrieved 2024-09-01.
  75. "New Zealand's 'driving dogs' pass their test". ABC News. 2012-12-11. Retrieved 2024-09-01.

Further reading