Neuroscience and intelligence

Last updated

Neuroscience and intelligence refers to the various neurological factors that are partly responsible for the variation of intelligence within species or between different species. A large amount of research in this area has been focused on the neural basis of human intelligence. Historic approaches to studying the neuroscience of intelligence consisted of correlating external head parameters, for example head circumference, to intelligence. [1] Post-mortem measures of brain weight and brain volume have also been used. [1] More recent methodologies focus on examining correlates of intelligence within the living brain using techniques such as magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), functional MRI (fMRI), electroencephalography (EEG), positron emission tomography and other non-invasive measures of brain structure and activity. [1]

Contents

Researchers have been able to identify correlates of intelligence within the brain and its functioning. These include overall brain volume, [2] grey matter volume, [3] white matter volume, [4] white matter integrity, [5] cortical thickness [3] and neural efficiency. [6]

Analyses of the parameters of intellectual systems, patterns of their emergence and evolution, distinctive features, and the constants and limits of their structures and functions made it possible to measure and compare the capacity of communications (~100 m/s), to quantify the number of components in intellectual systems (~1011 neurons), and to calculate the number of successful links responsible for cooperation (~1014 synapses). [7]

Although the evidence base for our understanding of the neural basis of human intelligence has increased greatly over the past 30 years, even more research is needed to fully understand it. [1]

The neural basis of intelligence has also been examined in animals such as primates, cetaceans, and rodents. [8]

Humans

Brain volume

One of the main methods used to establish a relationship between intelligence and the brain is to use measures of brain volume. [1] The earliest attempts at estimating brain volume were done using measures of external head parameters, such as head circumference as a proxy for brain size. [1] More recent methodologies employed to study this relationship include post-mortem measures of brain weight and volume. These have their own limitations and strengths. [9] The advent of MRI as a non-invasive highly-accurate measure of living brain structure and function (using fMRI) made this the pre-dominant and preferred method for measuring brain volume. [1]

Overall, larger brain size and volume is associated with better cognitive functioning and higher intelligence. [1] The specific regions that show the most robust correlation between volume and intelligence are the frontal, temporal and parietal lobes of the brain. [10] [11] [12] A large number of studies have been conducted with uniformly positive correlations, leading to the generally safe conclusion that larger brains predict greater intelligence. [13] [14] In healthy adults, the correlation of total brain volume and IQ is approximately 0.4 when high-quality tests are used. [15] A large scale study (n = 29k) using the UK Biobank found a correlation of .275. The strength of this relationship did not depend on sex, contradicting some earlier studies. [16] A study using a sibling-design in two medium sized samples found evidence of causality with an effect size of .19. [17] This study design rules out confounders that vary between families, but not those that vary within families.

Less is known about variation on scales less than total brain volume. A meta-analytic review by McDaniel found that the correlation between intelligence and in vivo brain size was larger for females (0.40) than for males (0.25). [18] The same study also found that the correlation between brain size and Intelligence increased with age, with children showing smaller correlations. [18] It has been suggested that the link between larger brain volumes and higher intelligence is related to variation in specific brain regions: a whole-brain measure would under-estimate these links. [10] For functions more specific than general intelligence, regional effects may be more important. For instance evidence suggests that in adolescents learning new words, vocabulary growth is associated with gray matter density in bilateral posterior supramarginal gyri. [19] Small studies have shown transient changes in gray-matter associated with developing a new physical skill (juggling) occipito-temporal cortex [20]

Brain volume is not a perfect account of intelligence: the relationship explains a modest amount of variance in intelligence – 12% to 36% of the variance. [9] [10] The amount of variance explained by brain volume may also depend on the type of intelligence measured. [9] Up to 36% of variance in verbal intelligence can be explained by brain volume, while only approximately 10% of variance in visuospatial intelligence can be explained by brain volume. [9] A 2015 study by researcher Stuart J. Ritchie found that brain size explained 12% of the variance in intelligence among individuals. [21] These caveats imply that there are other major factors influencing how intelligent an individual is apart from brain size. [1] In a large meta-analysis consisting of 88 studies Pietschnig et al. (2015) estimated the correlation between brain volume and intelligence to be about correlation coefficient of 0.24 which equates to 6% variance. [22] Taking into account measurement quality, and sample type and IQ-range, the meta-analytic association of brain volume in appears to be ~ .4 in normal adults. [15] Researcher Jakob Pietschnig argued that the strength of the positive association of brain volume and IQ remains robust, but has been overestimated in the literature. He has stated that "It is tempting to interpret this association in the context of human cognitive evolution and species differences in brain size and cognitive ability, we show that it is not warranted to interpret brain size as an isomorphic proxy of human intelligence differences". [22]

Grey matter

Grey matter has been examined as a potential biological foundation for differences in intelligence. Similarly to brain volume, global grey matter volume is positively associated with intelligence. [1] More specifically, higher intelligence has been associated with larger cortical grey matter in the prefrontal and posterior temporal cortex in adults. [3] Furthermore, both verbal and nonverbal intelligence have been shown to be positively correlated with grey matter volume across the parietal, temporal and occipital lobes in young healthy adults, implying that intelligence is associated with a wide variety of structures within the brain. [23]

There appear to be sex differences between the relationship of grey matter to intelligence between men and women. [24] Men appear to show more intelligence to grey matter correlations in the frontal and parietal lobes, while the strongest correlations between intelligence and grey matter in women can be found in the frontal lobes and Broca's area. [24] However, these differences do not seem to impact overall Intelligence, implying that the same cognitive ability levels can be attained in different ways. [24]

One specific methodology used to study grey matter correlates of intelligence in areas of the brain is known as voxel-based morphometry (VBM). VBM allows researchers to specify areas of interest with great spatial resolution, allowing the examination of grey matter areas correlated with intelligence with greater special resolution. VBM has been used to correlate grey matter positively with intelligence in the frontal, temporal, parietal, and occipital lobes in healthy adults. [25] VBM has also been used to show that grey matter volume in the medial region of the prefrontal cortex and the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex correlate positively with intelligence in a group of 55 healthy adults. [26] VBM has also been successfully used to establish a positive correlation between grey matter volumes in the anterior cingulate and intelligence in children aged 5 to 18 years old. [27]

Grey matter has also been shown to positively correlate with intelligence in children. [27] [28] [29] Reis and colleagues [29] have found that grey matter in the prefrontal cortex contributes most robustly to variance in Intelligence in children between 5 and 17, while subcortical grey matter is related to intelligence to a lesser extent. Frangou and colleagues [28] examined the relationship between grey matter and intelligence in children and young adults aged between 12 and 21, and found that grey matter in the orbitofrontal cortex, cingulate gyrus, cerebellum and thalamus was positively correlated to intelligence, while grey matter in the caudate nucleus is negatively correlated with intelligence. However, the relationship between grey matter volume and intelligence only develops over time, as no significant positive relationship can be found between grey matter volume and intelligence in children under 11. [27]

An underlying caveat to research into the relationship of grey matter volume and intelligence is demonstrated by the hypothesis of neural efficiency. [6] [30] The findings that more intelligent individuals are more efficient at using their neurons might indicate that the correlation of grey matter to intelligence reflects selective elimination of unused synapses, and thus a better brain circuitry. [31]

White matter

Similar to grey matter, white matter has been shown to correlate positively with intelligence in humans. [1] [4] White matter consists mainly of myelinated neuronal axons, responsible for delivering signals between neurons. The pinkish-white color of white matter is actually a result of these myelin sheaths that electrically insulate neurons that are transmitting signals to other neurons. White matter connects different regions of grey matter in the cerebrum together. These interconnections make transport more seamless and allow us to perform tasks easier. Significant correlations between intelligence and the corpus callosum have been found, as larger callosal areas have been positively correlated with cognitive performance. [1] However, there appear to be differences in importance for white matter between verbal and nonverbal intelligence, as although both verbal and nonverbal measures of intelligence correlate positively with the size of the corpus callosum, the correlation for intelligence and corpus callosum size was larger (.47) for nonverbal measures than that for verbal measures (.18). [32] Anatomical mesh-based geometrical modelling [33] [34] [35] has also shown positive correlations between the thickness of the corpus callosum and Intelligence in healthy adults. [36]

White matter integrity has also been found to be related to intelligence. [5] White matter tract integrity is important for information processing speed, and therefore reduced white matter integrity is related to lower intelligence. [5] The effect of white matter integrity is mediate entirely through information processing speed. [5] These findings indicate that the brain is structurally interconnected and that axonal fibres are integrally important for fast information process, and thus general intelligence. [5]

Contradicting the findings described above, VBM failed to find a relationship between the corpus callosum and intelligence in healthy adults. [25] This contradiction can be viewed to signify that the relationship between white matter volume and intelligence is not as robust as that of grey matter and intelligence. [1]

Cortical thickness

Cortical thickness has also been found to correlate positively with intelligence in humans. [3] However, the rate of growth of cortical thickness is also related to intelligence. [31] In early childhood, cortical thickness displays a negative correlation with intelligence, while by late childhood this correlation has shifted to a positive one. [31] More intelligent children were found to develop cortical thickness more steadily and over longer periods of time than less bright children. [31] Studies have found cortical thickness to explain 5% in the variance of intelligence among individuals. [21] In a study conducted to find associations between cortical thickness and general intelligence between different groups of people, sex did not play a role in intelligence. [37] Although it is hard to pin intelligence on age based on cortical thickness due to different socioeconomic circumstances and education levels, older subjects (17 - 24) tended to have less variances in terms of intelligence than when compared to younger subjects (19 - 17). [37] [ dubious discuss ]

Cortical convolution

Cortical convolution has increased the folding of the brain’s surface over the course of human evolution. It has been hypothesized that the high degree of cortical convolution may be a neurological substrate that supports some of the human brain's most distinctive cognitive abilities. Consequently, individual intelligence within the human species might be modulated by the degree of cortical convolution. [38]

An analysis published in 2019 found the contours of 677 children and adolescent (mean age 12.72 years) brains had a genetic correlation of almost 1 between IQ and surface area of the supramarginal gyrus on the left side of the brain. [39] [40]

Neural efficiency

The neural efficiency hypothesis postulates that more intelligent individuals display less activation in the brain during cognitive tasks, as measured by Glucose metabolism. [6] A small sample of participants (N=8) displayed negative correlations between intelligence and absolute regional metabolic rates ranging from -0.48 to -0.84, as measured by PET scans, indicating that brighter individuals were more effective processors of information, as they use less energy. [6] According to an extensive review by Neubauer & Fink [41] a large number of studies (N=27) have confirmed this finding using methods such as PET scans, [42] EEG [43] and fMRI. [44]

fMRI and EEG studies have revealed that task difficulty is an important factor affecting neural efficiency. [41] More intelligent individuals display neural efficiency only when faced with tasks of subjectively easy to moderate difficulty, while no neural efficiency can be found during difficult tasks. [45] In fact, more able individuals appear to invest more cortical resources in tasks of high difficulty. [41] This appears to be especially true for the Prefrontal Cortex, as individuals with higher intelligence displayed increased activation of this area during difficult tasks compared to individuals with lower intelligence. [46] [47] It has been proposed that the main reason for the neural efficiency phenomenon could be that individuals with high intelligence are better at blocking out interfering information than individuals with low intelligence. [48]

Further research

Some scientists prefer to look at more qualitative variables to relate to the size of measurable regions of known function, for example relating the size of the primary visual cortex to its corresponding functions, that of visual performance. [49] [50]

In a study of the head growth of 633 term-born children from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children cohort, it was shown that prenatal growth and growth during infancy were associated with subsequent IQ. The study’s conclusion was that the brain volume a child achieves by the age of 1 year helps determine later intelligence. Growth in brain volume after infancy may not compensate for poorer earlier growth. [51]

There is an association between IQ and myopia. One suggested explanation is that one or several pleiotropic gene(s) affect the size of the neocortex part of the brain and eyes simultaneously. [52]

Parieto-frontal integration theory

In 2007, Behavioral and Brain Sciences published a target article that put forth a biological model of intelligence based on 37 peer-reviewed neuroimaging studies (Jung & Haier, 2007). Their review of a wealth of data from functional imaging (functional magnetic resonance imaging and positron emission tomography) and structural imaging (diffusion MRI, voxel-based morphometry, in vivo magnetic resonance spectroscopy) argues that human intelligence arises from a distributed and integrated neural network comprising brain regions in the frontal and parietal lobes. [53]

A recent lesion mapping study conducted by Barbey and colleagues provides evidence to support the P-FIT theory of intelligence. [54] [55] [56]

Brain injuries at an early age isolated to one side of the brain typically results in relatively spared intellectual function and with IQ in the normal range. [57]

Primates

Brain size

Another theory of brain size in vertebrates is that it may relate to social rather than mechanical skills. Cortical size relates directly to pair-bonding lifestyle and among primates, cerebral cortex size varies directly with the demands of living in a large complex social network. Compared to other mammals, primates have significantly larger brain sizes. Additionally, most primates are found to be polygynandrous, having many social relationships with others. Although inconclusive, some studies have shown that this polygynandrous statue correlates to brain size. [58]

Intelligence in chimpanzees has been found to be related to brain size, grey matter volume, and cortical thickness, as in humans. [59]

Health

Several environmental factors related to health can lead to significant cognitive impairment, particularly if they occur during pregnancy and childhood when the brain is growing and the blood–brain barrier is less effective. Developed nations have implemented several health policies regarding nutrients and toxins known to influence cognitive function. These include laws requiring fortification of certain food products and laws establishing safe levels of pollutants (e.g. lead, mercury, and organochlorides). Comprehensive policy recommendations targeting reduction of cognitive impairment in children have been proposed. [60]

See also

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Grey matter</span> Areas of neuronal cell bodies in the brain

Grey matter, or brain matter in American English, is a major component of the central nervous system, consisting of neuronal cell bodies, neuropil, glial cells, synapses, and capillaries. Grey matter is distinguished from white matter in that it contains numerous cell bodies and relatively few myelinated axons, while white matter contains relatively few cell bodies and is composed chiefly of long-range myelinated axons. The colour difference arises mainly from the whiteness of myelin. In living tissue, grey matter actually has a very light grey colour with yellowish or pinkish hues, which come from capillary blood vessels and neuronal cell bodies.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Cerebral cortex</span> Outer layer of the cerebrum of the mammalian brain

The cerebral cortex, also known as the cerebral mantle, is the outer layer of neural tissue of the cerebrum of the brain in humans and other mammals. It is the largest site of neural integration in the central nervous system, and plays a key role in attention, perception, awareness, thought, memory, language, and consciousness. The cerebral cortex is the part of the brain responsible for cognition.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Brodmann area 9</span> Part of the frontal cortex in the brain of humans and other primates

Brodmann area 9, or BA9, refers to a cytoarchitecturally defined portion of the frontal cortex in the brain of humans and other primates. Its cytoarchitecture is referred to as granular due to the concentration of granule cells in layer IV. It contributes to the dorsolateral and medial prefrontal cortex.

Encephalization quotient (EQ), encephalization level (EL), or just encephalization is a relative brain size measure that is defined as the ratio between observed and predicted brain mass for an animal of a given size, based on nonlinear regression on a range of reference species. It has been used as a proxy for intelligence and thus as a possible way of comparing the intelligence levels of different species. For this purpose, it is a more refined measurement than the raw brain-to-body mass ratio, as it takes into account allometric effects. Expressed as a formula, the relationship has been developed for mammals and may not yield relevant results when applied outside this group.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Insular cortex</span> Portion of the mammalian cerebral cortex

The insular cortex is a portion of the cerebral cortex folded deep within the lateral sulcus within each hemisphere of the mammalian brain.

Neuroplasticity, also known as neural plasticity or brain plasticity, is the ability of neural networks in the brain to change through growth and reorganization. It is when the brain is rewired to function in some way that differs from how it previously functioned. These changes range from individual neuron pathways making new connections, to systematic adjustments like cortical remapping or neural oscillation. Other forms of neuroplasticity include homologous area adaptation, cross modal reassignment, map expansion, and compensatory masquerade. Examples of neuroplasticity include circuit and network changes that result from learning a new ability, information acquisition, environmental influences, pregnancy, caloric intake, practice/training, and psychological stress.

The size of the brain is a frequent topic of study within the fields of anatomy, biological anthropology, animal science and evolution. Measuring brain size and cranial capacity is relevant both to humans and other animals, and can be done by weight or volume via MRI scans, by skull volume, or by neuroimaging intelligence testing.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Posterior cingulate cortex</span> Caudal part of the cingulate cortex of the brain

The posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) is the caudal part of the cingulate cortex, located posterior to the anterior cingulate cortex. This is the upper part of the "limbic lobe". The cingulate cortex is made up of an area around the midline of the brain. Surrounding areas include the retrosplenial cortex and the precuneus.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex</span> Area of the prefrontal cortex of primates

The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex is an area in the prefrontal cortex of the primate brain. It is one of the most recently derived parts of the human brain. It undergoes a prolonged period of maturation which lasts into adulthood. The DLPFC is not an anatomical structure, but rather a functional one. It lies in the middle frontal gyrus of humans. In macaque monkeys, it is around the principal sulcus. Other sources consider that DLPFC is attributed anatomically to BA 9 and 46 and BA 8, 9 and 10.

Gyrification is the process of forming the characteristic folds of the cerebral cortex.

Malleability of intelligence describes the processes by which intelligence can increase or decrease over time and is not static. These changes may come as a result of genetics, pharmacological factors, psychological factors, behavior, or environmental conditions. Malleable intelligence may refer to changes in cognitive skills, memory, reasoning, or muscle memory related motor skills. In general, the majority of changes in human intelligence occur at either the onset of development, during the critical period, or during old age.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Neuroscience of sex differences</span> Characteristics of the brain that differentiate the male brain and the female brain

The neuroscience of sex differences is the study of characteristics that separate brains of different sexes. Psychological sex differences are thought by some to reflect the interaction of genes, hormones, and social learning on brain development throughout the lifespan. A 2021 meta-synthesis led by Lise Eliot found that sex accounted for 1% of the brain's structure or laterality, finding large group-level differences only in total brain volume. A subsequent 2021 led by Camille Michèle Williams contradicted Eliot's conclusions, finding that sex differences in total brain volume are not accounted for merely by sex differences in height and weight, and that once global brain size is taken into account, there remain numerous regional sex differences in both directions. A 2022 follow-up meta-analysis led by Alex DeCasien analyzed the studies from both Eliot and Williams, concluding that "The human brain shows highly reproducible sex differences in regional brain anatomy above and beyond sex differences in overall brain size" and that these differences are of a "small-moderate effect size." A review from 2006 and a meta-analysis from 2014 found that some evidence from brain morphology and function studies indicates that male and female brains cannot always be assumed to be identical from either a structural or functional perspective, and some brain structures are sexually dimorphic.

Neuroimaging intelligence testing concerns the use of neuroimaging techniques to evaluate human intelligence. Neuroimaging technology has advanced such that scientists hope to use neuroimaging increasingly for investigations of brain function related to IQ.

The biological basis of personality is a collection of brain systems and mechanisms that underlie human personality. Human neurobiology, especially as it relates to complex traits and behaviors, is not well understood, but research into the neuroanatomical and functional underpinnings of personality are an active field of research. Animal models of behavior, molecular biology, and brain imaging techniques have provided some insight into human personality, especially trait theories.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Mechanisms of mindfulness meditation</span>

Mindfulness has been defined in modern psychological terms as "paying attention to relevant aspects of experience in a nonjudgmental manner", and maintaining attention on present moment experience with an attitude of openness and acceptance. Meditation is a platform used to achieve mindfulness. Both practices, mindfulness and meditation, have been "directly inspired from the Buddhist tradition" and have been widely promoted by Jon Kabat-Zinn. Mindfulness meditation has been shown to have a positive impact on several psychiatric problems such as depression and therefore has formed the basis of mindfulness programs such as mindfulness-based cognitive therapy, mindfulness-based stress reduction and mindfulness-based pain management. The applications of mindfulness meditation are well established, however the mechanisms that underlie this practice are yet to be fully understood. Many tests and studies on soldiers with PTSD have shown tremendous positive results in decreasing stress levels and being able to cope with problems of the past, paving the way for more tests and studies to normalize and accept mindful based meditation and research, not only for soldiers with PTSD, but numerous mental inabilities or disabilities.

Andreas Karl Engel is a German neuroscientist. He is the director of the Department of Neurophysiology and Pathophysiology at the University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf (UKE).

The parieto-frontal integration theory (P-FIT) considers intelligence to relate to how well different brain regions integrate to form intelligent behaviors. The theory proposes that large scale brain networks connect brain regions, including regions within frontal, parietal, temporal, and cingulate cortices, underlie the biological basis of human intelligence. These regions, which overlap significantly with the task-positive network, allow the brain to communicate and exchange information efficiently with one another. Support for this theory is primarily based on neuroimaging evidence, with support from lesion studies. The P-FIT is influential in that it explains the majority of current neuroimaging findings, as well as increasing empirical support for cognition being the result of large-scale brain networks, rather than numerous domain-specific processes or modules. A 2010 review of the neuroscience of intelligence described P-FIT as "the best available answer to the question of where in the brain intelligence resides".

The neural efficiency hypothesis proposes that while performing a cognitive task, individuals with higher intelligence levels exhibit lower brain activation in comparison to individuals with lower intelligence levels. This hypothesis suggests that individual differences in cognitive abilities are due to differences in the efficiency of neural processing. Essentially, individuals with higher cognitive abilities utilize fewer neural resources to perform a given task than those with lower cognitive abilities.

James Van Loan Haxby is an American neuroscientist. He currently is a professor in the Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences at Dartmouth College and was the Director for the Dartmouth Center for Cognitive Neuroscience from 2008 to 2021. He is best known for his work on face perception and applications of machine learning in functional neuroimaging.

The term posterior cortical hot zone was coined by Christof Koch and colleagues to describe the part of the neocortex closely associated with the minimal neural substrate essential for conscious perception. The posterior cortical hot zone includes sensory cortical areas in the parietal, temporal, and occipital lobes. It is the “sensory” cortex, much as the frontal cortex is the “action” cortex.

References

  1. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 Luders, E.; Narr, K. L.; Thompson, P. M.; Toga, A. W. (2009). "Neuroanatomical correlates of intelligence". Intelligence. 37 (2): 156–163. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2008.07.002. PMC   2770698 . PMID   20160919.
  2. Pietschnig J, Penke L, Wicherts JM, Zeiler M, Voracek M (2015). "Meta-analysis of associations between human brain volume and intelligence differences: How strong are they and what do they mean?". Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 57: 411–32. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.09.017. PMID   26449760. S2CID   23180321.
  3. 1 2 3 4 Narr, K. L.; Woods, R. P.; Thompson, P. M.; Szeszko, P.; Robinson, D.; Dimtcheva, T.; Bilder, R. M. (2007). "Relationships between IQ and regional cortical gray matter thickness in healthy adults". Cerebral Cortex. 17 (9): 2163–2171. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl125 . PMID   17118969.
  4. 1 2 Gur, R. C.; Turetsky, B. I.; Matsui, M.; Yan, M.; Bilker, W.; Hughett, P.; Gur, R. E. (1999). "Sex differences in brain gray and white matter in healthy young adults: correlations with cognitive performance". Journal of Neuroscience. 19 (10): 4065–4072. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-10-04065.1999. PMC   6782697 . PMID   10234034.
  5. 1 2 3 4 5 Penke, L.; Maniega, S. M.; Bastin, M. E.; Hernandez, M. V.; Murray, C.; Royle, N. A.; Deary, I. J. (2012). "Brain white matter tract integrity as a neural foundation for general intelligence". Molecular Psychiatry. 17 (10): 1026–1030. doi:10.1038/mp.2012.66. PMID   22614288. S2CID   2334558.
  6. 1 2 3 4 Haier, R. J.; Siegel, B. V.; Nuechterlein, K. H.; Hazlett, E.; Wu, J. C.; Paek, J.; Buchsbaum, M. S. (1988). "Cortical glucose metabolic rate correlates of abstract reasoning and attention studied with positron emission tomography". Intelligence. 12 (2): 199–217. doi:10.1016/0160-2896(88)90016-5.
  7. Eryomin, A. L. (2022). "Biophysics of Evolution of Intellectual Systems". Biophysics. 67 (2): 320–326. doi:10.1134/S0006350922020051. PMC   9244026 . PMID   35789557.
  8. Dunbar, R. I.; Shultz, S. (2007). "Evolution in the social brain". Science. 317 (5843): 1344–1347. Bibcode:2007Sci...317.1344D. doi:10.1126/science.1145463. PMID   17823343. S2CID   1516792.
  9. 1 2 3 4 Witelson, S. F.; Beresh, H.; Kigar, D. L. (2006). "Intelligence and brain size in 100 postmortem brains: sex, lateralization and age factors". Brain. 129 (2): 386–398. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh696 . PMID   16339797.
  10. 1 2 3 Andreasen, N. C.; Flaum, M.; Victor Swayze, I. I.; O'Leary, D. S.; Alliger, R.; Cohen, G. (1993). "Intelligence and brain structure in normal individuals". Am J Psychiatry. 150 (1): 130–4. doi:10.1176/ajp.150.1.130. PMID   8417555.
  11. Flashman, L. A.; Andreasen, N. C.; Flaum, M.; Swayze, V. W. (1997). "Intelligence and regional brain volumes in normal controls". Intelligence. 25 (3): 149–160. doi:10.1016/s0160-2896(97)90039-8.
  12. MacLullich, A. M. J.; Ferguson, K. J.; Deary, I. J.; Seckl, J. R.; Starr, J. M.; Wardlaw, J. M. (2002). "Intracranial capacity and brain volumes are associated with cognition in healthy elderly men". Neurology. 59 (2): 169–174. doi:10.1212/wnl.59.2.169. PMID   12136052. S2CID   46043963.
  13. Gray, J. R.; Thompson, P. M. (2004). "Neurobiology of intelligence: science and ethics". Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 5 (6): 471–482. doi:10.1038/nrn1405. PMID   15152197. S2CID   2430677.
  14. Toga, A. W.; Thompson, P. M. (2005). "Genetics of brain structure and intelligence". Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 28: 1–23. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135655. PMID   15651931. S2CID   780961.
  15. 1 2 Gignac, Gilles E.; Bates, Timothy C. (2017). "Brain volume and intelligence: The moderating role of intelligence measurement quality" (PDF). Intelligence. 64: 18–29. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2017.06.004. hdl: 20.500.11820/a61135a7-6389-4f5c-9a4e-24403ba7e873 . S2CID   84839916.
  16. Cox, S. R.; Ritchie, S. J.; Fawns-Ritchie, C.; Tucker-Drob, E. M.; Deary, I. J. (2019-09-01). "Structural brain imaging correlates of general intelligence in UK Biobank". Intelligence. 76: 101376. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2019.101376. ISSN   0160-2896. PMC   6876667 . PMID   31787788.
  17. Lee, James J.; McGue, Matt; Iacono, William G.; Michael, Andrew M.; Chabris, Christopher F. (July 2019). "The causal influence of brain size on human intelligence: Evidence from within-family phenotypic associations and GWAS modeling". Intelligence. 75: 48–58. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2019.01.011. PMC   7440690 . PMID   32831433.
  18. 1 2 McDaniel, M. A. (2005). "Big-brained people are smarter: A meta-analysis of the relationship between in vivo brain volume and intelligence". Intelligence. 33 (4): 337–346. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2004.11.005.
  19. Lee, H.; Devlin, J. T.; Shakeshaft, C.; Stewart, L. H.; Brennan, A.; Glensman, J.; Price, C. J. (2007). "Anatomical traces of vocabulary acquisition in the adolescent brain" (PDF). The Journal of Neuroscience. 27 (5): 1184–1189. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.4442-06.2007. PMC   6673201 . PMID   17267574.
  20. Driemeyer, J.; Boyke, J.; Gaser, C.; Büchel, C.; May, A. (2008). "Changes in gray matter induced by learning—revisited". PLOS ONE. 3 (7): e2669. Bibcode:2008PLoSO...3.2669D. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002669 . PMC   2447176 . PMID   18648501.
  21. 1 2 Ritchie, Stuart J.; Booth, Tom; Valdés Hernández, Maria del C.; Corley, Janie; Maniega, Susana Muñoz; Gow, Alan J.; Royle, Natalie A.; Pattie, Alison; Karama, Sherif (2015-01-01). "Beyond a bigger brain: Multivariable structural brain imaging and intelligence". Intelligence. 51: 47–56. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2015.05.001. ISSN   0160-2896. PMC   4518535 . PMID   26240470.
  22. 1 2 Pietschnig, Jakob; Penke, Lars; Wicherts, Jelte M.; Zeiler, Michael; Voracek, Martin (2015-10-01). "Meta-analysis of associations between human brain volume and intelligence differences: How strong are they and what do they mean?". Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 57: 411–432. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.09.017. PMID   26449760. S2CID   23180321.
  23. Colom, R.; Jung, R. E.; Haier, R. J. (2006). "Distributed brain sites for the g-factor of intelligence". NeuroImage. 31 (3): 1359–1365. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.006. PMID   16513370. S2CID   33222604.
  24. 1 2 3 Haier, R. J.; Jung, R. E.; Yeo, R. A.; Head, K.; Alkire, M. T. (2005). "The neuroanatomy of general intelligence: sex matters". NeuroImage. 25 (1): 320–327. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.11.019. PMID   15734366. S2CID   4127512.
  25. 1 2 Haier, R. J.; Jung, R. E.; Yeo, R. A.; Head, K.; Alkire, M. T. (2004). "Structural brain variation and general intelligence". NeuroImage. 23 (1): 425–433. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.04.025. PMID   15325390. S2CID   29426973.
  26. Gong, Q. Y.; Sluming, V.; Mayes, A.; Keller, S.; Barrick, T.; Cezayirli, E.; Roberts, N. (2005). "Voxel-based morphometry and stereology provide convergent evidence of the importance of medial prefrontal cortex for fluid intelligence in healthy adults". NeuroImage. 25 (4): 1175–1186. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.044. PMID   15850735. S2CID   6986485.
  27. 1 2 3 Wilke, M.; Sohn, J. H.; Byars, A. W.; Holland, S. K. (2003). "Bright spots: correlations of gray matter volume with IQ in a normal pediatric population". NeuroImage. 20 (1): 202–215. doi:10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00199-x. PMID   14527581. S2CID   14583968.
  28. 1 2 Frangou, S.; Chitins, X.; Williams, S. C. (2004). "Mapping IQ and gray matter density in healthy young people". NeuroImage. 23 (3): 800–805. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.05.027. PMID   15528081. S2CID   16808023.
  29. 1 2 Reiss, A. L.; Abrams, M. T.; Singer, H. S.; Ross, J. L.; Denckla, M. B. (1996). "Brain development, gender and IQ in children A volumetric imaging study". Brain. 119 (5): 1763–1774. doi: 10.1093/brain/119.5.1763 . PMID   8931596.
  30. Haier, R. J.; Siegel, B.; Tang, C.; Abel, L.; Buchsbaum, M. S. (1992). "Intelligence and changes in regional cerebral glucose metabolic rate following learning". Intelligence. 16 (3): 415–426. doi:10.1016/0160-2896(92)90018-m.
  31. 1 2 3 4 Shaw, P.; Greenstein, D.; Lerch, J.; Clasen, L.; Lenroot, R.; Gogtay, N.; Giedd, J. (2006). "Intellectual ability and cortical development in children and adolescents". Nature. 440 (7084): 676–679. Bibcode:2006Natur.440..676S. doi:10.1038/nature04513. PMID   16572172. S2CID   3079565.
  32. Fletcher, J. M.; Bohan, T. P.; Brandt, M. E.; Brookshire, B. L.; Beaver, S. R.; Francis, D. J.; Miner, M. E. (1992). "Cerebral white matter and cognition in hydrocephalic children". Archives of Neurology. 49 (8): 818–824. doi:10.1001/archneur.1992.00530320042010. PMID   1524514.
  33. Thompson, P. M.; Schwartz, C.; Lin, R. T.; Khan, A. A.; Toga, A. W. (1996). "Three-dimensional statistical analysis of sulcal variability in the human brain". The Journal of Neuroscience. 16 (13): 4261–4274. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-13-04261.1996 . PMC   6578992 . PMID   8753887.
  34. Thompson, P. M.; Schwartz, C.; Toga, A. W. (1996). "High-resolution random mesh algorithms for creating a probabilistic 3D surface atlas of the human brain". NeuroImage. 3 (1): 19–34. doi:10.1006/nimg.1996.0003. PMID   9345472. S2CID   15940105.
  35. Thompson, P. M.; MacDonald, D.; Mega, M. S.; Holmes, C. J.; Evans, A. C.; Toga, A. W. (1997). "Detection and mapping of abnormal brain structure with a probabilistic atlas of cortical surfaces". Journal of Computer Assisted Tomography. 21 (4): 567–581. doi:10.1097/00004728-199707000-00008. PMID   9216760.
  36. Luders, E.; Narr, K. L.; Bilder, R. M.; Thompson, P. M.; Szeszko, P. R.; Hamilton, L.; Toga, A. W. (2007). "Positive correlations between corpus callosum thickness and intelligence". NeuroImage. 37 (4): 1457–1464. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.06.028. PMC   2754582 . PMID   17689267.
  37. 1 2 Menary, Kyle; Collins, Paul F.; Porter, James N.; Muetzel, Ryan; Olson, Elizabeth A.; Kumar, Vipin; Steinbach, Michael; Lim, Kelvin O.; Luciana, Monica (2013-01-01). "Associations between cortical thickness and general intelligence in children, adolescents and young adults". Intelligence. 41 (5): 597–606. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2013.07.010. ISSN   0160-2896. PMC   3985090 . PMID   24744452.
  38. Luders, Eileen; Narr, Katherine L.; Thompson, Paul M.; Toga, Arthur W. (2009-03-01). "Neuroanatomical Correlates of Intelligence". Intelligence. 37 (2): 156–163. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2008.07.002. ISSN   0160-2896. PMC   2770698 . PMID   20160919.
  39. "Brain Surface Area Reveals Overlap in Genes, Intelligence, Evolution". The Scientist Magazine®. Retrieved 2019-07-17.
  40. Raznahan, Armin; Giedd, Jay N.; Lee, Nancy Raitano; Wallace, Gregory L.; Chu, Alan; Pritikin, Joshua N.; Seidlitz, Jakob; Liu, Siyuan; Clasen, Liv S. (2019-04-17). "A Comprehensive Quantitative Genetic Analysis of Cerebral Surface Area in Youth". Journal of Neuroscience. 39 (16): 3028–3040. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2248-18.2019. ISSN   0270-6474. PMC   6468099 . PMID   30833512.
  41. 1 2 3 Neubauer, A. C., & Fink, A. (2009). Intelligence and neural efficiency. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 33(7), 1004-1023.
  42. Andreasen, N.C.; O'Leary, D.S.; Arndt, S.; Cizadlo, T.; Rezai, K.; Watkins, G.L.; Boles Ponto, L.L.; Hichwa, R.D. (1995). "PET studies of memory: novel and practiced free recall of complex narratives". NeuroImage. 2 (4): 284–295. doi:10.1006/nimg.1995.1036. PMID   9343613. S2CID   19745366.
  43. Doppelmayr, M.; Klimesch, W.; Schwaiger, J.; Auinger, P.; Winkler, T. (1998). "Theta synchronization in the human EEG and episodic retrieval". Neurosci. Lett. 257 (1): 41–44. doi:10.1016/s0304-3940(98)00805-2. PMID   9857961. S2CID   11307011.
  44. Rypma, B.; D'Esposito, M. (1999). "The roles of prefrontal brain regions in components of working memory: effects of memory load and individual differences". PNAS. 96 (11): 6558–6563. Bibcode:1999PNAS...96.6558R. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.11.6558 . PMC   26921 . PMID   10339627.
  45. Neubauer, A.C., Sange, G., Pfurtscheller, G., 1999. Psychometric intelligence and event-related desynchronisation during performance of a letter matching task. In: Pfurtscheller, G., Lopes da Silva, F.H. (Eds.), Event-Related Desynchronization (ERD) and Related Oscillatory EEG-Phenomena of the Awake Brain. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp. 219–231.
  46. Callicott, J. H.; Mattay, V. S.; Bertolino, A.; Finn, K.; Coppola, R.; Frank, J. A. (1999). "Physiological characteristics of capacity constraints in working memory as revealed by functional MRI". Cerebral Cortex. 9 (1): 20–26. doi: 10.1093/cercor/9.1.20 . PMID   10022492.
  47. Rypma, B., Berger, J. S., Prabhakaran, V., Bly, B. M., Kimberg, D. Y., & Biswal, B. B. (2006). Neural correlates of cognitive efficiency. NeuroImage, 33(3), 969 –979.
  48. Gray, J. R.; Chabris, C. F.; Braver, T. S. (2003). "Neural mechanisms of general fluid intelligence". Nature Neuroscience. 6 (3): 316–322. doi:10.1038/nn1014. PMID   12592404. S2CID   10492067.
  49. Schoenemann, PT; Budinger, TF; Sarich, VM; Wang, WS (April 2000). "Brain size does not predict cognitive abilities within families". PNAS. 97 (9): 4932–4937. Bibcode:2000PNAS...97.4932S. doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.9.4932 . PMC   18335 . PMID   10781101.
  50. Brain size and intelligence
  51. Catharine R. Gale, Finbar J. O'Callaghan, Maria Bredow, MBChB, Christopher N. Martyn, DPhil and the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children Study Team (October 4, 2006). "The Influence of Head Growth in Fetal Life, Infancy, and Childhood on Intelligence at the Ages of 4 and 8 Years". Pediatrics. 118 (4): 1486–1492. doi:10.1542/peds.2005-2629. PMID   17015539. S2CID   12447118 . Retrieved August 6, 2006.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  52. Czepita, D.; Lodygowska, E.; Czepita, M. (2008). "Are children with myopia more intelligent? A literature review". Annales Academiae Medicae Stetinensis. 54 (1): 13–16, discussion 16. PMID   19127804.
  53. Richard Haier & Rex Jung (July 26, 2007). "The Parieto-Frontal Integration Theory (P-FIT) of intelligence: Converging neuroimaging evidence". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 30 (2). Cambridge University Press: 135–154. doi:10.1017/S0140525X07001185. PMID   17655784. S2CID   14699011 . Retrieved September 28, 2009.
  54. Barbey, Aron K.; Colom, Roberto; Solomon, Jeffrey; Krueger, Frank; Forbes, Chad; Grafman, Jordan (2012). "An integrative architecture for general intelligence and executive function revealed by lesion mapping". Brain. 135 (4): 1154–1164. doi:10.1093/brain/aws021. PMC   3326251 . PMID   22396393.
  55. HealthDay (2012-04-13). "Researchers Map Brain Regions Linked to Intelligence". U.S. News & World Report.
  56. Yates, Diana. "Researchers Use Brain-Injury Data to Map Intelligence in the Brain". University of Illinois News Bureau. University of Illinois.
  57. Bava, Sunita; Ballantyne, Angela O; Trauner, Doris A (2005). "Disparity of Verbal and Performance IQ Following Early Bilateral Brain Damage". Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology. 18 (3): 163–70. doi:10.1097/01.wnn.0000178228.61938.3e. PMID   16175020. S2CID   30150030.
  58. Dunbar RI, Shultz S; Shultz (2007-09-07). "Evolution in the social brain". Science. 317 (5843): 1344–1347. Bibcode:2007Sci...317.1344D. doi:10.1126/science.1145463. PMID   17823343. S2CID   1516792.
  59. Hopkins, William D.; Li, Xiang; Roberts, Neil (November 2018). "More intelligent chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) have larger brains and increased cortical thickness". Intelligence. 74: 18–24. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2018.11.002. S2CID   150309083.
  60. Olness, K. (2003). "Effects on brain development leading to cognitive impairment: a worldwide epidemic". Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. 24 (2): 120–30. doi:10.1097/00004703-200304000-00009. PMID   12692458. S2CID   31999992.