FOXP2

Last updated

FOXP2
FOXP2 (2as5).png
Available structures
PDB Human UniProt search: PDBe RCSB
Identifiers
Aliases FOXP2 , CAGH44, SPCH1, TNRC10, forkhead box P2
External IDs OMIM: 605317 HomoloGene: 33482 GeneCards: FOXP2
Orthologs
SpeciesHumanMouse
Entrez
Ensembl
UniProt
RefSeq (mRNA)

n/a

RefSeq (protein)

n/a

Location (UCSC) Chr 7: 114.09 – 114.69 Mb n/a
PubMed search [2] n/a
Wikidata
View/Edit Human

Forkhead box protein P2 (FOXP2) is a protein that, in humans, is encoded by the FOXP2 gene. FOXP2 is a member of the forkhead box family of transcription factors, proteins that regulate gene expression by binding to DNA. It is expressed in the brain, heart, lungs and digestive system. [3] [4]

Contents

FOXP2 is found in many vertebrates, where it plays an important role in mimicry in birds (such as birdsong) and echolocation in bats. FOXP2 is also required for the proper development of speech and language in humans. [5] In humans, mutations in FOXP2 cause the severe speech and language disorder developmental verbal dyspraxia. [5] [6] Studies of the gene in mice and songbirds indicate that it is necessary for vocal imitation and the related motor learning. [7] [8] [9] Outside the brain, FOXP2 has also been implicated in development of other tissues such as the lung and digestive system. [10]

Initially identified in 1998 as the genetic cause of a speech disorder in a British family designated the KE family, FOXP2 was the first gene discovered to be associated with speech and language [11] and was subsequently dubbed "the language gene". [12] However, other genes are necessary for human language development, and a 2018 analysis confirmed that there was no evidence of recent positive evolutionary selection of FOXP2 in humans. [13] [14]

Structure and function

Foxp2 is expressed in the developing cerebellum and the hindbrain of the embryonic day 13.5 mouse. Allen Brain Atlases Foxp2, ISH, E13.5 mouse, cerebellum-hindbrain.jpg
Foxp2 is expressed in the developing cerebellum and the hindbrain of the embryonic day 13.5 mouse. Allen Brain Atlases

As a FOX protein, FOXP2 contains a forkhead-box domain. In addition, it contains a polyglutamine tract, a zinc finger and a leucine zipper. The protein attaches to the DNA of other proteins and controls their activity through the forkhead-box domain. Only a few targeted genes have been identified, however researchers believe that there could be up to hundreds of other genes targeted by the FOXP2 gene. The forkhead box P2 protein is active in the brain and other tissues before and after birth, many studies show that it is paramount for the growth of nerve cells and transmission between them. The FOXP2 gene is also involved in synaptic plasticity, making it imperative for learning and memory. [15]

FOXP2 is required for proper brain and lung development. Knockout mice with only one functional copy of the FOXP2 gene have significantly reduced vocalizations as pups. [16] Knockout mice with no functional copies of FOXP2 are runted, display abnormalities in brain regions such as the Purkinje layer, and die an average of 21 days after birth from inadequate lung development. [10]

FOXP2 is expressed in many areas of the brain, [17] including the basal ganglia and inferior frontal cortex, where it is essential for brain maturation and speech and language development. [18] In mice, the gene was found to be twice as highly expressed in male pups than female pups, which correlated with an almost double increase in the number of vocalisations the male pups made when separated from mothers. Conversely, in human children aged 4–5, the gene was found to be 30% more expressed in the Broca's areas of female children. The researchers suggested that the gene is more active in "the more communicative sex". [19] [20]

The expression of FOXP2 is subject to post-transcriptional regulation, particularly microRNA (miRNA), causing the repression of the FOXP2 3' untranslated region. [21]

Three amino acid substitutions distinguish the human FOXP2 protein from that found in mice, while two amino acid substitutions distinguish the human FOXP2 protein from that found in chimpanzees, [17] but only one of these changes is unique to humans. [10] Evidence from genetically manipulated mice [22] and human neuronal cell models [23] suggests that these changes affect the neural functions of FOXP2.

Clinical significance

The FOXP2 gene has been implicated in several cognitive functions including; general brain development, language, and synaptic plasticity. The FOXP2 gene region acts as a transcription factor for the forkhead box P2 protein. Transcription factors affect other regions, and the forkhead box P2 protein has been suggested to also act as a transcription factor for hundreds of genes. This prolific involvement opens the possibility that the FOXP2 gene is much more extensive than originally thought. [15] Other targets of transcription have been researched without correlation to FOXP2. Specifically, FOXP2 has been investigated in correlation with autism and dyslexia, however with no mutation was discovered as the cause. [24] [6] One well identified target is language. [25] Although some research disagrees with this correlation, [26] the majority of research shows that a mutated FOXP2 causes the observed production deficiency. [15] [25] [27] [24] [28] [29]

There is some evidence that the linguistic impairments associated with a mutation of the FOXP2 gene are not simply the result of a fundamental deficit in motor control. Brain imaging of affected individuals indicates functional abnormalities in language-related cortical and basal ganglia regions, demonstrating that the problems extend beyond the motor system. [30]

Mutations in FOXP2 are among several (26 genes plus 2 intergenic) loci which correlate to ADHD diagnosis in adults – clinical ADHD is an umbrella label for a heterogeneous group of genetic and neurological phenomena which may result from FOXP2 mutations or other causes. [31]

A 2020 genome-wide association study (GWAS) implicates single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) of FOXP2 in susceptibility to cannabis use disorder. [32]

Language disorder

It is theorized that the translocation of the 7q31.2 region of the FOXP2 gene causes a severe language impairment called developmental verbal dyspraxia (DVD) [25] or childhood apraxia of speech (CAS) [33] So far this type of mutation has only been discovered in three families across the world including the original KE family. [29] A missense mutation causing an arginine-to-histidine substitution (R553H) in the DNA-binding domain is thought to be the abnormality in KE. [34] This would cause a normally basic residue to be fairly acidic and highly reactive at the body's pH. A heterozygous nonsense mutation, R328X variant, produces a truncated protein involved in speech and language difficulties in one KE individual and two of their close family members. R553H and R328X mutations also affected nuclear localization, DNA-binding, and the transactivation (increased gene expression) properties of FOXP2. [6]

These individuals present with deletions, translocations, and missense mutations. When tasked with repetition and verb generation, these individuals with DVD/CAS had decreased activation in the putamen and Broca's area in fMRI studies. These areas are commonly known as areas of language function. [35] This is one of the primary reasons that FOXP2 is known as a language gene. They have delayed onset of speech, difficulty with articulation including slurred speech, stuttering, and poor pronunciation, as well as dyspraxia. [29] It is believed that a major part of this speech deficit comes from an inability to coordinate the movements necessary to produce normal speech including mouth and tongue shaping. [25] Additionally, there are more general impairments with the processing of the grammatical and linguistic aspects of speech. [6] These findings suggest that the effects of FOXP2 are not limited to motor control, as they include comprehension among other cognitive language functions. General mild motor and cognitive deficits are noted across the board. [27] Clinically these patients can also have difficulty coughing, sneezing, or clearing their throats. [25]

While FOXP2 has been proposed to play a critical role in the development of speech and language, this view has been challenged by the fact that the gene is also expressed in other mammals as well as birds and fish that do not speak. [36] It has also been proposed that the FOXP2 transcription-factor is not so much a hypothetical 'language gene' but rather part of a regulatory machinery related to externalization of speech. [37]

Evolution

Human FOXP2 gene and evolutionary conservation is shown in a multiple alignment (at bottom of figure) in this image from the UCSC Genome Browser. Note that conservation tends to cluster around coding regions (exons). BrowserFoxp2.jpg
Human FOXP2 gene and evolutionary conservation is shown in a multiple alignment (at bottom of figure) in this image from the UCSC Genome Browser. Note that conservation tends to cluster around coding regions (exons).

The FOXP2 gene is highly conserved in mammals. [17] The human gene differs from that in non-human primates by the substitution of two amino acids, a threonine to asparagine substitution at position 303 (T303N) and an asparagine to serine substitution at position 325 (N325S). [34] In mice it differs from that of humans by three substitutions, and in zebra finch by seven amino acids. [17] [38] [39] One of the two amino acid differences between human and chimps also arose independently in carnivores and bats. [10] [40] Similar FOXP2 proteins can be found in songbirds, fish, and reptiles such as alligators. [41] [42]

DNA sampling from Homo neanderthalensis bones indicates that their FOXP2 gene is a little different though largely similar to those of Homo sapiens (i.e. humans). [43] [44] Previous genetic analysis had suggested that the H. sapiens FOXP2 gene became fixed in the population around 125,000 years ago. [45] Some researchers consider the Neanderthal findings to indicate that the gene instead swept through the population over 260,000 years ago, before our most recent common ancestor with the Neanderthals. [45] Other researchers offer alternative explanations for how the H. sapiens version would have appeared in Neanderthals living 43,000 years ago. [45]

According to a 2002 study, the FOXP2 gene showed indications of recent positive selection. [17] [46] Some researchers have speculated that positive selection is crucial for the evolution of language in humans. [17] Others, however, were unable to find a clear association between species with learned vocalizations and similar mutations in FOXP2. [41] [42] A 2018 analysis of a large sample of globally distributed genomes confirmed there was no evidence of positive selection, suggesting that the original signal of positive selection may be driven by sample composition. [13] [14] Insertion of both human mutations into mice, whose version of FOXP2 otherwise differs from the human and chimpanzee versions in only one additional base pair, causes changes in vocalizations as well as other behavioral changes, such as a reduction in exploratory tendencies, and a decrease in maze learning time. A reduction in dopamine levels and changes in the morphology of certain nerve cells are also observed. [22]

Interactions

FOXP2 is known to regulate CNTNAP2 , CTBP1 , [47] SRPX2 and SCN3A . [48] [18] [49]

FOXP2 downregulates CNTNAP2, a member of the neurexin family found in neurons. CNTNAP2 is associated with common forms of language impairment. [50]

FOXP2 also downregulates SRPX2, the 'Sushi Repeat-containing Protein X-linked 2'. [51] [52] It directly reduces its expression, by binding to its gene's promoter. SRPX2 is involved in glutamatergic synapse formation in the cerebral cortex and is more highly expressed in childhood. SRPX2 appears to specifically increase the number of glutamatergic synapses in the brain, while leaving inhibitory GABAergic synapses unchanged and not affecting dendritic spine length or shape. On the other hand, FOXP2's activity does reduce dendritic spine length and shape, in addition to number, indicating it has other regulatory roles in dendritic morphology. [51]

In other animals

Chimpanzees

In chimpanzees, FOXP2 differs from the human version by two amino acids. [53] A study in Germany sequenced FOXP2's complementary DNA in chimps and other species to compare it with human complementary DNA in order to find the specific changes in the sequence. [17] FOXP2 was found to be functionally different in humans compared to chimps. Since FOXP2 was also found to have an effect on other genes, its effects on other genes is also being studied. [54] Researchers deduced that there could also be further clinical applications in the direction of these studies in regards to illnesses that show effects on human language ability. [23]

Mice

In a mouse FOXP2 gene knockouts, loss of both copies of the gene causes severe motor impairment related to cerebellar abnormalities and lack of ultrasonic vocalisations normally elicited when pups are removed from their mothers. [16] These vocalizations have important communicative roles in mother–offspring interactions. Loss of one copy was associated with impairment of ultrasonic vocalisations and a modest developmental delay. Male mice on encountering female mice produce complex ultrasonic vocalisations that have characteristics of song. [55] Mice that have the R552H point mutation carried by the KE family show cerebellar reduction and abnormal synaptic plasticity in striatal and cerebellar circuits. [7]

Humanized FOXP2 mice display altered cortico-basal ganglia circuits. The human allele of the FOXP2 gene was transferred into the mouse embryos through homologous recombination to create humanized FOXP2 mice. The human variant of FOXP2 also had an effect on the exploratory behavior of the mice. In comparison to knockout mice with one non-functional copy of FOXP2, the humanized mouse model showed opposite effects when testing its effect on the levels of dopamine, plasticity of synapses, patterns of expression in the striatum and behavior that was exploratory in nature. [22]

When FOXP2 expression was altered in mice, it affected many different processes including the learning motor skills and the plasticity of synapses. Additionally, FOXP2 is found more in the sixth layer of the cortex than in the fifth, and this is consistent with it having greater roles in sensory integration. FOXP2 was also found in the medial geniculate nucleus of the mouse brain, which is the processing area that auditory inputs must go through in the thalamus. It was found that its mutations play a role in delaying the development of language learning. It was also found to be highly expressed in the Purkinje cells and cerebellar nuclei of the cortico-cerebellar circuits. High FOXP2 expression has also been shown in the spiny neurons that express type 1 dopamine receptors in the striatum, substantia nigra, subthalamic nucleus and ventral tegmental area. The negative effects of the mutations of FOXP2 in these brain regions on motor abilities were shown in mice through tasks in lab studies. When analyzing the brain circuitry in these cases, scientists found greater levels of dopamine and decreased lengths of dendrites, which caused defects in long-term depression, which is implicated in motor function learning and maintenance. Through EEG studies, it was also found that these mice had increased levels of activity in their striatum, which contributed to these results. There is further evidence for mutations of targets of the FOXP2 gene shown to have roles in schizophrenia, epilepsy, autism, bipolar disorder and intellectual disabilities. [56]

Bats

FOXP2 has implications in the development of bat echolocation. [34] [40] [57] Contrary to apes and mice, FOXP2 is extremely diverse in echolocating bats. [40] Twenty-two sequences of non-bat eutherian mammals revealed a total number of 20 nonsynonymous mutations in contrast to half that number of bat sequences, which showed 44 nonsynonymous mutations. [40] All cetaceans share three amino acid substitutions, but no differences were found between echolocating toothed whales and non-echolocating baleen cetaceans. [40] Within bats, however, amino acid variation correlated with different echolocating types. [40]

Birds

In songbirds, FOXP2 most likely regulates genes involved in neuroplasticity. [8] [58] Gene knockdown of FOXP2 in area X of the basal ganglia in songbirds results in incomplete and inaccurate song imitation. [8] Overexpression of FOXP2 was accomplished through injection of adeno-associated virus serotype 1 (AAV1) into area X of the brain. This overexpression produced similar effects to that of knockdown; juvenile zebra finch birds were unable to accurately imitate their tutors. [59] Similarly, in adult canaries, higher FOXP2 levels also correlate with song changes. [39]

Levels of FOXP2 in adult zebra finches are significantly higher when males direct their song to females than when they sing song in other contexts. [58] "Directed" singing refers to when a male is singing to a female usually for a courtship display. "Undirected" singing occurs when for example, a male sings when other males are present or is alone. [60] Studies have found that FoxP2 levels vary depending on the social context. When the birds were singing undirected song, there was a decrease of FoxP2 expression in Area X. This downregulation was not observed and FoxP2 levels remained stable in birds singing directed song. [58]

Differences between song-learning and non-song-learning birds have been shown to be caused by differences in FOXP2 gene expression, rather than differences in the amino acid sequence of the FOXP2 protein.

Zebrafish

In zebrafish, FOXP2 is expressed in the ventral and dorsal thalamus, telencephalon, diencephalon where it likely plays a role in nervous system development. The zebrafish FOXP2 gene has an 85% similarity to the human FOX2P ortholog. [61]

History

FOXP2 and its gene were discovered as a result of investigations on an English family known as the KE family, half of whom (15 individuals across three generations) had a speech and language disorder called developmental verbal dyspraxia. Their case was studied at the Institute of Child Health of University College London. [62] In 1990, Myrna Gopnik, Professor of Linguistics at McGill University, reported that the disorder-affected KE family had severe speech impediment with incomprehensible talk, largely characterized by grammatical deficits. [63] She hypothesized that the basis was not of learning or cognitive disability, but due to genetic factors affecting mainly grammatical ability. [64] (Her hypothesis led to a popularised existence of "grammar gene" and a controversial notion of grammar-specific disorder. [65] [66] ) In 1995, the University of Oxford and the Institute of Child Health researchers found that the disorder was purely genetic. [67] Remarkably, the inheritance of the disorder from one generation to the next was consistent with autosomal dominant inheritance, i.e., mutation of only a single gene on an autosome (non-sex chromosome) acting in a dominant fashion. This is one of the few known examples of Mendelian (monogenic) inheritance for a disorder affecting speech and language skills, which typically have a complex basis involving multiple genetic risk factors. [68]

The FOXP2 gene is located on the long (q) arm of chromosome 7, at position 31. FOXP2 location.png
The FOXP2 gene is located on the long (q) arm of chromosome 7, at position 31.

In 1998, Oxford University geneticists Simon Fisher, Anthony Monaco, Cecilia S. L. Lai, Jane A. Hurst, and Faraneh Vargha-Khadem identified an autosomal dominant monogenic inheritance that is localized on a small region of chromosome 7 from DNA samples taken from the affected and unaffected members. [3] The chromosomal region (locus) contained 70 genes. [69] The locus was given the official name "SPCH1" (for speech-and-language-disorder-1) by the Human Genome Nomenclature committee. Mapping and sequencing of the chromosomal region was performed with the aid of bacterial artificial chromosome clones. [4] Around this time, the researchers identified an individual who was unrelated to the KE family but had a similar type of speech and language disorder. In this case, the child, known as CS, carried a chromosomal rearrangement (a translocation) in which part of chromosome 7 had become exchanged with part of chromosome 5. The site of breakage of chromosome 7 was located within the SPCH1 region. [4]

In 2001, the team identified in CS that the mutation is in the middle of a protein-coding gene. [5] Using a combination of bioinformatics and RNA analyses, they discovered that the gene codes for a novel protein belonging to the forkhead-box (FOX) group of transcription factors. As such, it was assigned with the official name of FOXP2. When the researchers sequenced the FOXP2 gene in the KE family, they found a heterozygous point mutation shared by all the affected individuals, but not in unaffected members of the family and other people. [5] This mutation is due to an amino-acid substitution that inhibits the DNA-binding domain of the FOXP2 protein. [70] Further screening of the gene identified multiple additional cases of FOXP2 disruption, including different point mutations [6] and chromosomal rearrangements, [71] providing evidence that damage to one copy of this gene is sufficient to derail speech and language development.

See also

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Rett syndrome</span> Genetic brain disorder

Rett syndrome (RTT) is a genetic disorder that typically becomes apparent after 6–18 months of age and almost exclusively in females. Symptoms include impairments in language and coordination, and repetitive movements. Those affected often have slower growth, difficulty walking, and a smaller head size. Complications of Rett syndrome can include seizures, scoliosis, and sleeping problems. The severity of the condition is variable.

FOXproteins are a family of transcription factors that play important roles in regulating the expression of genes involved in cell growth, proliferation, differentiation, and longevity. Many FOX proteins are important to embryonic development. FOX proteins also have pioneering transcription activity by being able to bind condensed chromatin during cell differentiation processes.

Expressive language disorder is one of the "specific developmental disorders of speech and language" recognised by the tenth edition of the International Classification of Diseases (ICD-10). As of the eleventh edition, it is considered to be covered by the various categories of developmental language disorder. Transition to the ICD-11 will take place at a different time in different countries.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Chimpanzee genome project</span> Effort to determine the DNA sequence of the chimpanzee genome

The Chimpanzee Genome Project was an effort to determine the DNA sequence of the chimpanzee genome. Sequencing began in 2005 and by 2013 twenty-four individual chimpanzees had been sequenced. This project was folded into the Great Ape Genome Project.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">22q13 deletion syndrome</span> Rare genetic syndrome

22q13 deletion syndrome, also known as Phelan–McDermid syndrome (PMS), is a genetic disorder caused by deletions or rearrangements on the q terminal end of chromosome 22. Any abnormal genetic variation in the q13 region that presents with significant manifestations (phenotype) typical of a terminal deletion may be diagnosed as 22q13 deletion syndrome. There is disagreement among researchers as to the exact definition of 22q13 deletion syndrome. The Developmental Synaptopathies Consortium defines PMS as being caused by SHANK3 mutations, a definition that appears to exclude terminal deletions. The requirement to include SHANK3 in the definition is supported by many but not by those who first described 22q13 deletion syndrome.

The KE family is a medical name designated for a British family, about half of whom exhibit a severe speech disorder called developmental verbal dyspraxia. It is the first family with speech disorder to be investigated using genetic analyses, by which the speech impairment is discovered to be due to genetic mutation, and from which the gene FOXP2, often dubbed the "language gene", was discovered. Their condition is also the first human speech and language disorder known to exhibit strict Mendelian inheritance.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">FOXP1</span> Protein-coding gene in the species Homo sapiens

Forkhead box protein P1 is a protein that in humans is encoded by the FOXP1 gene. FOXP1 is necessary for the proper development of the brain, heart, and lung in mammals. It is a member of the large FOX family of transcription factors.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Forkhead box C1</span> Protein-coding gene in the species Homo sapiens

Forkhead box C1, also known as FOXC1, is a protein which in humans is encoded by the FOXC1 gene.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">FOXL2</span> Transcription factor gene of the FOX family

Forkhead box protein L2 is a protein that in humans is encoded by the FOXL2 gene.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">FOXG1</span> Protein-coding gene in the species Homo sapiens

Forkhead box protein G1 is a protein that in humans is encoded by the FOXG1 gene.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">FOXN1</span> Protein-coding gene in the species Homo sapiens

Forkhead box protein N1 is a protein that in humans is encoded by the FOXN1 gene.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">FRAS1</span> Protein-coding gene in the species Homo sapiens

Extracellular matrix protein FRAS1 is a protein that in humans is encoded by the FRAS1 gene. This gene encodes an extracellular matrix protein that appears to function in the regulation of epidermal-basement membrane adhesion and organogenesis during development.

Myrna Lee Gopnik is a Canadian linguist. She is a professor emerita of linguistics at McGill University. She is known for her research on the KE family, an English family with several members affected by specific language impairment.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">FOXE3</span> Protein-coding gene in the species Homo sapiens

Forkhead box protein E3 (FOXE3) also known as forkhead-related transcription factor 8 (FREAC-8) is a protein that in humans is encoded by the FOXE3 gene located on the short arm of chromosome 1.

Language has a long evolutionary history and is closely related to the brain, but what makes the human brain uniquely adapted to language is unclear. The regions of the brain that are involved in language in humans have similar analogues in apes and monkeys, and yet they do not use language. There may also be a genetic component: mutations in the FOXP2 gene prevent humans from constructing complete sentences.

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) refers to a variety of conditions typically identified by challenges with social skills, communication, speech, and repetitive sensory-motor behaviors. The 11th International Classification of Diseases (ICD-11), released in January 2021, characterizes ASD by the associated deficits in the ability to initiate and sustain two-way social communication and restricted or repetitive behavior unusual for the individual's age or situation. Although linked with early childhood, the symptoms can appear later as well. Symptoms can be detected before the age of two and experienced practitioners can give a reliable diagnosis by that age. However, official diagnosis may not occur until much older, even well into adulthood. There is a large degree of variation in how much support a person with ASD needs in day-to-day life. This can be classified by a further diagnosis of ASD level 1, level 2, or level 3. Of these, ASD level 3 describes people requiring very substantial support and who experience more severe symptoms. ASD-related deficits in nonverbal and verbal social skills can result in impediments in personal, family, social, educational, and occupational situations. This disorder tends to have a strong correlation with genetics along with other factors. More research is identifying ways in which epigenetics is linked to autism. Epigenetics generally refers to the ways in which chromatin structure is altered to affect gene expression. Mechanisms such as cytosine regulation and post-translational modifications of histones. Of the 215 genes contributing, to some extent in ASD, 42 have been found to be involved in epigenetic modification of gene expression. Some examples of ASD signs are specific or repeated behaviors, enhanced sensitivity to materials, being upset by changes in routine, appearing to show reduced interest in others, avoiding eye contact and limitations in social situations, as well as verbal communication. When social interaction becomes more important, some whose condition might have been overlooked suffer social and other exclusion and are more likely to have coexisting mental and physical conditions. Long-term problems include difficulties in daily living such as managing schedules, hypersensitivities, initiating and sustaining relationships, and maintaining jobs.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Simon Fisher</span> British geneticist and neuroscientist (born 1970)

Simon E. Fisher is a British geneticist and neuroscientist who has pioneered research into the genetic basis of human speech and language. He is a director of the Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics and Professor of language and genetics at the Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour in Nijmegen, The Netherlands.

Evolutionary psychology of language is the study of the evolutionary history of language as a psychological faculty within the discipline of evolutionary psychology. It makes the assumption that language is the result of a Darwinian adaptation.

Developmental verbal dyspraxia (DVD), also known as childhood apraxia of speech (CAS) and developmental apraxia of speech (DAS), is a condition in which an individual has problems saying sounds, syllables and words. This is not because of muscle weakness or paralysis. The brain has problems planning to move the body parts needed for speech. The individual knows what they want to say, but their brain has difficulty coordinating the muscle movements necessary to say those words.

The development of an animal model of autism is one approach researchers use to study potential causes of autism. Given the complexity of autism and its etiology, researchers often focus only on single features of autism when using animal models.

References

  1. 1 2 3 GRCh38: Ensembl release 89: ENSG00000128573 - Ensembl, May 2017
  2. "Human PubMed Reference:". National Center for Biotechnology Information, U.S. National Library of Medicine.
  3. 1 2 Fisher SE, Vargha-Khadem F, Watkins KE, Monaco AP, Pembrey ME (February 1998). "Localisation of a gene implicated in a severe speech and language disorder". Nature Genetics. 18 (2): 168–70. doi:10.1038/ng0298-168. hdl: 11858/00-001M-0000-0012-CBD9-5 . PMID   9462748. S2CID   3190318.
  4. 1 2 3 Lai CS, Fisher SE, Hurst JA, Levy ER, Hodgson S, Fox M, et al. (August 2000). "The SPCH1 region on human 7q31: genomic characterization of the critical interval and localization of translocations associated with speech and language disorder". American Journal of Human Genetics. 67 (2): 357–68. doi:10.1086/303011. PMC   1287211 . PMID   10880297.
  5. 1 2 3 4 Lai CS, Fisher SE, Hurst JA, Vargha-Khadem F, Monaco AP (October 2001). "A forkhead-domain gene is mutated in a severe speech and language disorder". Nature. 413 (6855): 519–23. Bibcode:2001Natur.413..519L. doi:10.1038/35097076. hdl: 11858/00-001M-0000-0012-CB9C-F . PMID   11586359. S2CID   4421562.
  6. 1 2 3 4 5 MacDermot KD, Bonora E, Sykes N, Coupe AM, Lai CS, Vernes SC, et al. (June 2005). "Identification of FOXP2 truncation as a novel cause of developmental speech and language deficits". American Journal of Human Genetics. 76 (6): 1074–80. doi:10.1086/430841. PMC   1196445 . PMID   15877281.
  7. 1 2 Groszer M, Keays DA, Deacon RM, de Bono JP, Prasad-Mulcare S, Gaub S, et al. (March 2008). "Impaired synaptic plasticity and motor learning in mice with a point mutation implicated in human speech deficits". Current Biology. 18 (5): 354–62. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2008.01.060. PMC   2917768 . PMID   18328704.
  8. 1 2 3 Haesler S, Rochefort C, Georgi B, Licznerski P, Osten P, Scharff C (December 2007). "Incomplete and inaccurate vocal imitation after knockdown of FoxP2 in songbird basal ganglia nucleus Area X". PLOS Biology. 5 (12): e321. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0050321 . PMC   2100148 . PMID   18052609.
  9. Fisher SE, Scharff C (April 2009). "FOXP2 as a molecular window into speech and language". Trends in Genetics. 25 (4): 166–77. doi:10.1016/j.tig.2009.03.002. hdl: 11858/00-001M-0000-0012-CA31-7 . PMID   19304338.
  10. 1 2 3 4 Shu W, Lu MM, Zhang Y, Tucker PW, Zhou D, Morrisey EE (May 2007). "Foxp2 and Foxp1 cooperatively regulate lung and esophagus development". Development. 134 (10): 1991–2000. doi:10.1242/dev.02846. PMID   17428829. S2CID   22896384.
  11. Nudel R, Newbury DF (September 2013). "FOXP2". Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews. Cognitive Science. 4 (5): 547–560. doi:10.1002/wcs.1247. PMC   3992897 . PMID   24765219.
  12. Harpaz Y. "Language gene found". human-brain.org. Archived from the original on 25 October 2014. Retrieved 31 October 2014.
  13. 1 2 Atkinson EG, Audesse AJ, Palacios JA, Bobo DM, Webb AE, Ramachandran S, Henn BM (September 2018). "No Evidence for Recent Selection at FOXP2 among Diverse Human Populations". Cell. 174 (6): 1424–1435.e15. doi:10.1016/j.cell.2018.06.048. PMC   6128738 . PMID   30078708.
  14. 1 2 "Language Gene Dethroned". The Scientist Magazine®. Retrieved 28 January 2020.
  15. 1 2 3 "FOXP2 Gene". Genetics Home Reference. U.S. National Library of Medicine, National Institutes of Health. September 2016.
  16. 1 2 Shu W, Cho JY, Jiang Y, Zhang M, Weisz D, Elder GA, et al. (July 2005). "Altered ultrasonic vocalization in mice with a disruption in the Foxp2 gene". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 102 (27): 9643–8. Bibcode:2005PNAS..102.9643S. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0503739102 . PMC   1160518 . PMID   15983371.
  17. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Enard W, Przeworski M, Fisher SE, Lai CS, Wiebe V, Kitano T, et al. (August 2002). "Molecular evolution of FOXP2, a gene involved in speech and language" (PDF). Nature. 418 (6900): 869–72. Bibcode:2002Natur.418..869E. doi:10.1038/nature01025. hdl: 11858/00-001M-0000-0012-CB89-A . PMID   12192408. S2CID   4416233. Archived from the original (PDF) on 30 August 2006.
  18. 1 2 Spiteri E, Konopka G, Coppola G, Bomar J, Oldham M, Ou J, et al. (December 2007). "Identification of the transcriptional targets of FOXP2, a gene linked to speech and language, in developing human brain". American Journal of Human Genetics. 81 (6): 1144–57. doi:10.1086/522237. PMC   2276350 . PMID   17999357.
  19. Balter M (February 2013). "'Language Gene' More Active in Young Girls Than Boy". Science: 360.
  20. Bowers JM, Perez-Pouchoulen M, Edwards NS, McCarthy MM (February 2013). "Foxp2 mediates sex differences in ultrasonic vocalization by rat pups and directs order of maternal retrieval". The Journal of Neuroscience. 33 (8): 3276–83. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0425-12.2013. PMC   3727442 . PMID   23426656.
  21. Clovis YM, Enard W, Marinaro F, Huttner WB, De Pietri Tonelli D (September 2012). "Convergent repression of Foxp2 3'UTR by miR-9 and miR-132 in embryonic mouse neocortex: implications for radial migration of neurons". Development. 139 (18): 3332–42. doi: 10.1242/dev.078063 . PMID   22874921.
  22. 1 2 3 Enard W, Gehre S, Hammerschmidt K, Hölter SM, Blass T, Somel M, et al. (May 2009). "A humanized version of Foxp2 affects cortico-basal ganglia circuits in mice". Cell. 137 (5): 961–71. doi:10.1016/j.cell.2009.03.041. hdl: 11858/00-001M-0000-000F-F8C5-2 . PMID   19490899. S2CID   667723.
  23. 1 2 Konopka G, Bomar JM, Winden K, Coppola G, Jonsson ZO, Gao F, et al. (November 2009). "Human-specific transcriptional regulation of CNS development genes by FOXP2". Nature. 462 (7270): 213–7. Bibcode:2009Natur.462..213K. doi:10.1038/nature08549. PMC   2778075 . PMID   19907493.
  24. 1 2 Gauthier J, Joober R, Mottron L, Laurent S, Fuchs M, De Kimpe V, Rouleau GA (April 2003). "Mutation screening of FOXP2 in individuals diagnosed with autistic disorder". American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part A. 118A (2): 172–5. doi:10.1002/ajmg.a.10105. PMID   12655497. S2CID   39762074.
  25. 1 2 3 4 5 "FOXP2-related speech and language disorder". Genetics Home Reference. U.S. National Library of Medicine, National Institutes of Health. Retrieved 26 February 2019.
  26. Newbury DF, Bonora E, Lamb JA, Fisher SE, Lai CS, Baird G, et al. (May 2002). "FOXP2 is not a major susceptibility gene for autism or specific language impairment". American Journal of Human Genetics. 70 (5): 1318–27. doi:10.1086/339931. PMC   447606 . PMID   11894222.
  27. 1 2 Lennon PA, Cooper ML, Peiffer DA, Gunderson KL, Patel A, Peters S, et al. (April 2007). "Deletion of 7q31.1 supports involvement of FOXP2 in language impairment: clinical report and review". American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part A. 143A (8): 791–8. doi:10.1002/ajmg.a.31632. PMID   17330859. S2CID   22021740.
  28. Rossell S, Tan E, Bozaoglu K, Neill E, Sumner P, Carruthers S, Van Rheenen T, Thomas E, Gurvich C (2017). "Is language Impairment in Schizophrenia related to Language Genes?". European Neuropsychopharmacology. 27: S459–S460. doi:10.1016/j.euroneuro.2016.09.532. S2CID   54316143.
  29. 1 2 3 Reuter MS, Riess A, Moog U, Briggs TA, Chandler KE, Rauch A, et al. (January 2017). "FOXP2 variants in 14 individuals with developmental speech and language disorders broaden the mutational and clinical spectrum". Journal of Medical Genetics. 54 (1): 64–72. doi:10.1136/jmedgenet-2016-104094. PMID   27572252. S2CID   24589445.
  30. Liégeois F, Baldeweg T, Connelly A, Gadian DG, Mishkin M, Vargha-Khadem F (November 2003). "Language fMRI abnormalities associated with FOXP2 gene mutation". Nature Neuroscience. 6 (11): 1230–7. doi:10.1038/nn1138. PMID   14555953. S2CID   31003547.
  31. Demontis D, Walters RK, Martin J, Mattheisen M, Als TD, Agerbo E, et al. (January 2019). "Discovery of the first genome-wide significant risk loci for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder". Nature Genetics. 51 (1): 63–75. doi:10.1038/s41588-018-0269-7. PMC   6481311 . PMID   30478444.
  32. Johnson EC, Demontis D, Thorgeirsson TE, Walters RK, Polimanti R, Hatoum AS, et al. (December 2020). "A large-scale genome-wide association study meta-analysis of cannabis use disorder". The Lancet. Psychiatry. 7 (12): 1032–1045. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(20)30339-4 . PMC   7674631 . PMID   33096046.
  33. Morgan A, Fisher SE, Scheffer I, Hildebrand M (23 June 2016). Adam MP, Ardinger HH, Pagon RA, Wallace SE, Bean LJ, Stephens K, Amemiya A (eds.). "FOXP2-Related Speech and Language Disorders". GenReviews. University of Washington. PMID   27336128.
  34. 1 2 3 Preuss TM (June 2012). "Human brain evolution: from gene discovery to phenotype discovery". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 109 (Suppl 1): 10709–16. Bibcode:2012PNAS..10910709P. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1201894109 . PMC   3386880 . PMID   22723367.
  35. Vargha-Khadem F, Gadian DG, Copp A, Mishkin M (February 2005). "FOXP2 and the neuroanatomy of speech and language". Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 6 (2): 131–8. doi:10.1038/nrn1605. PMID   15685218. S2CID   2504002.
  36. Friederici AD (2016). 'Language in the Brain. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. p. 222. ISBN   978-0-262-03692-4.
  37. Berwick RC, Chomsky N (2016). Why Only Us?. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. p. 76. ISBN   978-0-262-53349-2.
  38. Teramitsu I, Kudo LC, London SE, Geschwind DH, White SA (March 2004). "Parallel FoxP1 and FoxP2 expression in songbird and human brain predicts functional interaction". The Journal of Neuroscience. 24 (13): 3152–63. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5589-03.2004. PMC   6730014 . PMID   15056695.
  39. 1 2 Haesler S, Wada K, Nshdejan A, Morrisey EE, Lints T, Jarvis ED, Scharff C (March 2004). "FoxP2 expression in avian vocal learners and non-learners". The Journal of Neuroscience. 24 (13): 3164–75. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4369-03.2004. PMC   6730012 . PMID   15056696.
  40. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Li G, Wang J, Rossiter SJ, Jones G, Zhang S (September 2007). Ellegren H (ed.). "Accelerated FoxP2 evolution in echolocating bats". PLOS ONE. 2 (9): e900. Bibcode:2007PLoSO...2..900L. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0000900 . PMC   1976393 . PMID   17878935.
  41. 1 2 Webb DM, Zhang J (2005). "FoxP2 in song-learning birds and vocal-learning mammals". The Journal of Heredity. 96 (3): 212–6. doi:10.1093/jhered/esi025. PMID   15618302.
  42. 1 2 Scharff C, Haesler S (December 2005). "An evolutionary perspective on FoxP2: strictly for the birds?". Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 15 (6): 694–703. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2005.10.004. PMID   16266802. S2CID   11350165.
  43. Zimmer C (17 March 2016). "Humans Interbred With Hominins on Multiple Occasions, Study Finds". The New York Times . Retrieved 17 March 2016.
  44. Krause J, Lalueza-Fox C, Orlando L, Enard W, Green RE, Burbano HA, et al. (November 2007). "The derived FOXP2 variant of modern humans was shared with Neandertals". Current Biology. 17 (21): 1908–12. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2007.10.008. hdl: 11858/00-001M-0000-000F-FED3-1 . PMID   17949978. S2CID   9518208. See also Benítez-Burraco A, Longa VM, Lorenzo G, Uriagereka J (November 2008). "Also sprach Neanderthalis... Or Did She?". Biolinguistics. 2 (2–3): 225–232. doi: 10.5964/bioling.8643 . S2CID   60864520.
  45. 1 2 3 Benítez-Burraco A, Longa VM, Lorenzo G, Uriagereka J (November 2008). "Also sprach Neanderthalis... Or Did She?". Biolinguistics. 2 (2–3): 225–232. doi: 10.5964/bioling.8643 . S2CID   60864520.
  46. Toda M, Okubo S, Ikigai H, Suzuki T, Suzuki Y, Hara Y, Shimamura T (1992). "The protective activity of tea catechins against experimental infection by Vibrio cholerae O1". Microbiology and Immunology. 36 (9): 999–1001. doi: 10.1111/j.1348-0421.1992.tb02103.x . PMID   1461156. S2CID   34400234.
  47. Li S, Weidenfeld J, Morrisey EE (January 2004). "Transcriptional and DNA binding activity of the Foxp1/2/4 family is modulated by heterotypic and homotypic protein interactions". Molecular and Cellular Biology. 24 (2): 809–22. doi:10.1128/MCB.24.2.809-822.2004. PMC   343786 . PMID   14701752.
  48. Smith RS, Kenny CJ, Ganesh V, Jang A, Borges-Monroy R, Partlow JN, et al. (September 2018). "Sodium Channel SCN3A (NaV1.3) Regulation of Human Cerebral Cortical Folding and Oral Motor Development". Neuron. 99 (5): 905–913.e7. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2018.07.052. PMC   6226006 . PMID   30146301.
  49. Vernes SC, Spiteri E, Nicod J, Groszer M, Taylor JM, Davies KE, et al. (December 2007). "High-throughput analysis of promoter occupancy reveals direct neural targets of FOXP2, a gene mutated in speech and language disorders". American Journal of Human Genetics. 81 (6): 1232–50. doi:10.1086/522238. PMC   2276341 . PMID   17999362.
  50. Vernes SC, Newbury DF, Abrahams BS, Winchester L, Nicod J, Groszer M, et al. (November 2008). "A functional genetic link between distinct developmental language disorders". The New England Journal of Medicine. 359 (22): 2337–45. doi:10.1056/NEJMoa0802828. PMC   2756409 . PMID   18987363.
  51. 1 2 Sia GM, Clem RL, Huganir RL (November 2013). "The human language-associated gene SRPX2 regulates synapse formation and vocalization in mice". Science. 342 (6161): 987–91. Bibcode:2013Sci...342..987S. doi:10.1126/science.1245079. PMC   3903157 . PMID   24179158.
  52. Pennisi E (31 October 2013). "'Language Gene' Has a Partner". Science. Retrieved 30 October 2014.
  53. Smith K (11 November 2009). "Evolution of a single gene linked to language". Nature. doi:10.1038/news.2009.1079. ISSN   1744-7933.
  54. "Why can't chimps talk? It's more than just genes". Reuters. 11 November 2009. Retrieved 21 February 2019.
  55. Holy TE, Guo Z (December 2005). "Ultrasonic songs of male mice". PLOS Biology. 3 (12): e386. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030386 . PMC   1275525 . PMID   16248680.
  56. French CA, Fisher SE (October 2014). "What can mice tell us about Foxp2 function?". Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 28: 72–9. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2014.07.003. hdl: 11858/00-001M-0000-0019-F62D-4 . PMID   25048596. S2CID   17848265.
  57. Wilbrecht L, Nottebohm F (2003). "Vocal learning in birds and humans". Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews. 9 (3): 135–48. doi:10.1002/mrdd.10073. PMID   12953292.
  58. 1 2 3 Teramitsu I, White SA (July 2006). "FoxP2 regulation during undirected singing in adult songbirds". The Journal of Neuroscience. 26 (28): 7390–4. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1662-06.2006. PMC   2683919 . PMID   16837586.
  59. Heston JB, White SA (February 2015). "Behavior-linked FoxP2 regulation enables zebra finch vocal learning". The Journal of Neuroscience. 35 (7): 2885–94. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3715-14.2015. PMC   4331621 . PMID   25698728.
  60. Jarvis ED, Scharff C, Grossman MR, Ramos JA, Nottebohm F (October 1998). "For whom the bird sings: context-dependent gene expression". Neuron. 21 (4): 775–88. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80594-2 . PMID   9808464. S2CID   13893471.
  61. Bonkowsky JL, Chien CB (November 2005). "Molecular cloning and developmental expression of foxP2 in zebrafish". Developmental Dynamics. 234 (3): 740–6. doi: 10.1002/dvdy.20504 . PMID   16028276. S2CID   24771138.
  62. Hurst JA, Baraitser M, Auger E, Graham F, Norell S (April 1990). "An extended family with a dominantly inherited speech disorder". Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. 32 (4): 352–5. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.1990.tb16948.x. PMID   2332125. S2CID   2654363.
  63. Gopnik M (September 1990). "Genetic basis of grammar defect". Nature. 347 (6288): 26. Bibcode:1990Natur.347...26G. doi: 10.1038/347026a0 . PMID   2395458. S2CID   4323390.
  64. Gopnik M (April 1990). "Feature-blind grammar and dysphagia". Nature. 344 (6268): 715. Bibcode:1990Natur.344..715G. doi: 10.1038/344715a0 . PMID   2330028. S2CID   4360334.
  65. Cowie F (1999). What's Within?: Nativism Reconsidered. New York, US: Oxford University Press. pp. 290–291. ISBN   978-0-1951-5978-3.
  66. Jenkins L (2000). Biolinguistics: Exploring the Biology of Language (Revised ed.). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. pp. 98–99. ISBN   978-0-5210-0391-9.
  67. Vargha-Khadem F, Watkins K, Alcock K, Fletcher P, Passingham R (January 1995). "Praxic and nonverbal cognitive deficits in a large family with a genetically transmitted speech and language disorder". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 92 (3): 930–3. Bibcode:1995PNAS...92..930V. doi: 10.1073/pnas.92.3.930 . PMC   42734 . PMID   7846081.
  68. Fisher SE, Lai CS, Monaco AP (2003). "Deciphering the genetic basis of speech and language disorders". Annual Review of Neuroscience. 26: 57–80. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.26.041002.131144. hdl: 11858/00-001M-0000-0012-CB7E-4 . PMID   12524432. S2CID   1276712.
  69. "Genes that are essential for speech". The Brain from Top to Bottom. Retrieved 31 October 2014.
  70. Vernes SC, Nicod J, Elahi FM, Coventry JA, Kenny N, Coupe AM, et al. (November 2006). "Functional genetic analysis of mutations implicated in a human speech and language disorder". Human Molecular Genetics. 15 (21): 3154–67. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddl392 . PMID   16984964.
  71. Feuk L, Kalervo A, Lipsanen-Nyman M, Skaug J, Nakabayashi K, Finucane B, et al. (November 2006). "Absence of a paternally inherited FOXP2 gene in developmental verbal dyspraxia". American Journal of Human Genetics. 79 (5): 965–72. doi:10.1086/508902. PMC   1698557 . PMID   17033973.