Elephant communication

Last updated
Asian elephants greeting each other by inter-twining their trunks Three elephant's curly kisses.jpg
Asian elephants greeting each other by inter-twining their trunks

Elephant communication includes touching, visual displays, vocalisations, seismic vibrations, and semiochemicals.

Contents

Tactile

Elephant (Loxodonta africana) mating ritual composite, Addo Elephant Park, South Africa Elephant (Loxodonta africana) mating ritual composite.jpg
Elephant ( Loxodonta africana ) mating ritual composite, Addo Elephant Park, South Africa

Individual elephants greet each other by stroking or wrapping their trunks; the latter also occurs during mild competition. Older elephants use trunk-slaps, kicks, and shoves to discipline younger ones. Individuals of any age and sex will touch each other's mouths, temporal glands, and genitals, particularly during meetings or when excited. This allows individuals to pick up chemical cues. Touching is especially important for mother–calf communication. When moving, elephant mothers will touch their calves with their trunks or feet when side-by-side or with their tails if the calf is behind them. If a calf wants to rest, it will press against its mother's front legs and when it wants to suckle, it will touch her breast or leg. [1]

Visual

Visual displays mostly occur in agonistically behavioural situations. Elephants will try to appear more threatening by raising their heads and spreading their ears. They may add to the display by shaking their heads and snapping their ears, as well as throwing dust and vegetation. They are usually bluffing when performing these actions. Excited elephants may raise their trunks. Submissive ones will lower their heads and trunks, as well as flatten their ears against their necks, while those that accept a challenge will position their ears in a V shape. [1]

Acoustic

Elephants produce several sounds, usually through the larynx, though some may be modified by the trunk. [1] [2] [3] [4] [5] Perhaps the most well-known call is the trumpet which is made by blowing through the trunk. Trumpeting is made during excitement, distress or aggression. [6] [7] [8] [9] Fighting elephants may roar or squeal, and wounded ones may bellow. [10] [11]

Asian elephants have been recorded to make three basic sounds: growls, squeaks, and snorts. Growls in their basic form are used for short-distance communication. During mild arousal, growls resonate in the trunk and become rumbles while for long-distance communication, they escalate into roars. Low-frequency growls are infrasonic and made in many contexts. [4] Squeaks come in two forms; chirpings and trumpets. Chirping consists of multiple short squeaks and signal conflict and nervousness. Trumpets are longer squeaks with increased loudness and produced during extreme arousal. Snorts signal changes in activity and increase in loudness during mild or strong arousal. During the latter case, when an elephant bounces the tip of the trunk it creates booms which serve as threat displays. [12] :142

Using machine learning, it was postulated in 2023, that elephants use personal names. [13]

Infrasound

Elephants can produce infrasonic calls which occur at frequencies less than 20 Hz. [14] Infrasonic calls are important, particularly for long-distance communication, [1] in both Asian and African elephants. For Asian elephants, these calls have a frequency of 14–24  Hz, with sound pressure levels of 85–90  dB and last 10–15 seconds. [15] For African elephants, calls range from 15 to 35 Hz with sound pressure levels as high as 117 dB, allowing communication for many kilometres, with a possible maximum range of around 10 km (6 mi). [16]

Low frequency rumble visualised with acoustic camera.

At Amboseli National Park several different infrasonic calls have been identified: [12] :145

Names and recognition

In 2024, research published in Nature Ecology and Evolution demonstrated that elephants call each other by name and respond when they hear others call their name. Researchers analyzed hundreds of elephant calls recorded over more than a year in Kenya, utilizing machine learning to identify specific sounds made by elephants when calling each other. When recorded calls were played back, elephants responded to the sound of their friends or family members calling their name by either calling back or moving toward the speaker. This behavior suggests that elephants may be capable of abstract thought and possess a vocabulary that extends beyond names. It is suggested that this research could potentially enable humans to communicate directly with elephants in the future, possibly warning them about dangers such as poachers. [17]

Anatomy of the vocal tract

The larynx of the elephant is the largest known among mammals. The vocal folds are long and are attached close to the epiglottis base. When comparing an elephant's vocal folds to those of a human, an elephant's are longer, thicker, and have a larger cross-sectional area. In addition, they are tilted at 45 degrees and positioned more anteriorly than a human's vocal folds. [18] From various experiments, the elephant larynx is shown to produce various and complex vibratory phenomena. During in vivo situations, these phenomena could be triggered when the vocal folds and vocal tract interact to raise or lower the fundamental frequency. [14]

One of the vibratory phenomena that occurred inside the larynx is alternating A-P (anterior-posterior) and P-A traveling waves, which happened due to the unusual larynx layout. This can be characterized by its unique glottal opening/closing pattern. When the trachea is at a pressure of approximately 6 kPa, phonation begins in the larynx and the laryngeal tissue starts to vibrate at approximately 15 kPa. Vocal production mechanisms at certain frequencies are similar to that of humans and other mammals and the laryngeal tissues are subjected to self-maintained oscillations. Two biomechanical features can trigger these traveling wave patterns, which are a low fundamental frequency and in the vocal folds, increasing longitudinal tension. [18]

Seismics

Elephants are known to communicate with seismics, vibrations produced by impacts on the earth's surface or acoustical waves that travel through it. They appear to rely on their leg and shoulder bones to transmit the signals to the middle ear. When detecting seismic signals, the animals lean forward and put more weight on their larger front feet; this is known as the "freezing behaviour". Elephants possess several adaptations suited for seismic communication. The cushion pads of the feet contain cartilaginous nodes and have similarities to the acoustic fat found in marine mammals such as toothed whales and sirenians. A unique sphincter-like muscle around the ear canal constricts the passageway, thereby dampening acoustic signals and allowing the animal to hear more seismic signals. [19]

Elephants appear to use seismics for a number of purposes. An individual running or mock charging can create seismic signals that can be heard at great distances. [20] When detecting the seismics of an alarm call signalling danger from predators, elephants enter a defensive posture and family groups will pack together. Seismic waveforms produced by locomotion appear to travel distances of up to 32 km (20 mi) while those from vocalisations travel 16 km (10 mi). [21]

Semiochemicals

Elephants can also communicate through olfaction and semiochemicals. [22] [23] Secretion of semiochemicals can occur through feces and urine [24] as well as the temporal gland, a structure that is derived from sweat glands and located on both sides of the head of male and female elephants. [22] [23] The substance secreted by male elephants from their temporal glands during musth contains many chemicals and seems to be of interest to females. [22] Elephants may investigate and detected semiochemicals through the vomeronasal organ (VNO). [23] Elephants may go through several steps of investigating the smell of a surface with their trunk before inserting its tip into their mouth to touch the anterior part of their hard palate and thus transfer semiochemicals to the VNO. [23]

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Elephant</span> Largest living land animals

Elephants are the largest living land animals. Three living species are currently recognised: the African bush elephant, the African forest elephant, and the Asian elephant. They are the only surviving members of the family Elephantidae and the order Proboscidea; extinct relatives include mammoths and mastodons. Distinctive features of elephants include a long proboscis called a trunk, tusks, large ear flaps, pillar-like legs, and tough but sensitive grey skin. The trunk is prehensile, bringing food and water to the mouth and grasping objects. Tusks, which are derived from the incisor teeth, serve both as weapons and as tools for moving objects and digging. The large ear flaps assist in maintaining a constant body temperature as well as in communication. African elephants have larger ears and concave backs, whereas Asian elephants have smaller ears and convex or level backs.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Infrasound</span> Vibrations with frequencies lower than 20 hertz

Infrasound, sometimes referred to as low frequency sound, describes sound waves with a frequency below the lower limit of human audibility. Hearing becomes gradually less sensitive as frequency decreases, so for humans to perceive infrasound, the sound pressure must be sufficiently high. Although the ear is the primary organ for sensing low sound, at higher intensities it is possible to feel infrasound vibrations in various parts of the body.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">African forest elephant</span> African elephant species

The African forest elephant is one of the two living species of African elephant. It is native to humid tropical forests in West Africa and the Congo Basin. It is the smallest of the three living elephant species, reaching a shoulder height of 2.4 m. As with other African elephants, both sexes have straight, down-pointing tusks, which begin to grow once the animals reach 1–3 years old. The forest elephant lives in highly sociable family groups of up to 20 individuals. Since they forage primarily on leaves, seeds, fruit, and tree bark, they have often been referred to as the 'megagardener of the forest'; the species is one of many that contributes significantly to maintaining the composition, diversity and structure of the Guinean Forests of West Africa and the Congolese rainforests. Seeds of various plants will go through the elephant's digestive tract and eventually pass through in the animal's droppings, thus helping to maintain the spread and biodiversity of the forests.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Asian elephant</span> Second largest elephant species

The Asian elephant, also known as the Asiatic elephant, is a species of elephant distributed throughout the Indian subcontinent and Southeast Asia, from India in the west to Borneo in the east, and Nepal in the north to Sumatra in the south. Three subspecies are recognised—E. m. maximus, E. m. indicus and E. m. sumatranus. The Asian elephant is characterised by its long trunk with a single finger-like processing; large tusks in males; laterally folded large ears but smaller in contrast to African elephants; and wrinkled grey skin. The skin is smoother than African elephants and may be depigmented on the trunk, ears or neck. Adult males average 4 t in weight, and females 2.7 t.

A purr or whirr is a tonal fluttering sound made by some species of felids, including both larger, outdoor cats and the domestic cat, as well as two species of genets. It varies in loudness and tone among species and in the same animal. In smaller and domestic cats it is known as a purr, while in larger felids, such as the panther, it is called a whirr.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Whale vocalization</span> Sounds produced by whales

Whales use a variety of sounds for communication and sensation. The mechanisms used to produce sound vary from one family of cetaceans to another. Marine mammals, including whales, dolphins, and porpoises, are much more dependent on sound than land mammals due to the limited effectiveness of other senses in water. Sight is less effective for marine mammals because of the way particulates in the ocean scatter light. Smell is also limited, as molecules diffuse more slowly in water than in air, which makes smelling less effective. However, the speed of sound is roughly four times greater in water than in the atmosphere at sea level. As sea mammals are so dependent on hearing to communicate and feed, environmentalists and cetologists are concerned that they are being harmed by the increased ambient noise in the world's oceans caused by ships, sonar and marine seismic surveys.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Rut (mammalian reproduction)</span> Mating season of ruminant mammals

The rut is the mating season of certain mammals, which includes ruminants such as deer, sheep, camels, goats, pronghorns, bison, giraffes and antelopes, and extends to others such as skunks and elephants. The rut is characterized in males by an increase in testosterone, exaggerated sexual dimorphisms, increased aggression, and increased interest in females. The males of the species may mark themselves with mud, undergo physiological changes or perform characteristic displays in order to make themselves more visually appealing to the females. Males also use olfaction to entice females to mate using secretions from glands and soaking in their own urine. Deer will also leave their own personal scent marking around by urinating down their own legs with the urine soaking the hair that covers their tarsal glands. Male deer do these most often during breeding season.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">African elephant</span> Genus comprising two living elephant species

African elephants are members of the genus Loxodonta comprising two living elephant species, the African bush elephant and the smaller African forest elephant. Both are social herbivores with grey skin, but differ in the size and colour of their tusks and in the shape and size of their ears and skulls.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Syrinx (bird anatomy)</span> The vocal organ of birds

The syrinx is the vocal organ of birds. Located at the base of a bird's trachea, it produces sounds without the vocal folds of mammals. The sound is produced by vibrations of some or all of the membrana tympaniformis and the pessulus, caused by air flowing through the syrinx. This sets up a self-oscillating system that modulates the airflow creating the sound. The muscles modulate the sound shape by changing the tension of the membranes and the bronchial openings. The syrinx enables some species of birds to mimic human speech.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Hearing range</span> Range of frequencies that can be heard by humans or other animals

Hearing range describes the frequency range that can be heard by humans or other animals, though it can also refer to the range of levels. The human range is commonly given as 20 to 20,000 Hz, although there is considerable variation between individuals, especially at high frequencies, and a gradual loss of sensitivity to higher frequencies with age is considered normal. Sensitivity also varies with frequency, as shown by equal-loudness contours. Routine investigation for hearing loss usually involves an audiogram which shows threshold levels relative to a normal.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Sri Lankan elephant</span> Subspecies of the Asian elephant

The Sri Lankan elephant is native to Sri Lanka and one of three recognised subspecies of the Asian elephant. It is the type subspecies of the Asian elephant and was first described by Carl Linnaeus under the binomial Elephas maximus in 1758. The Sri Lankan elephant population is now largely restricted to the dry zone in the north, east and southeast of Sri Lanka. Elephants are present in Udawalawe National Park, Yala National Park, Lunugamvehera National Park, Wilpattu National Park and Minneriya National Park but also live outside protected areas. It is estimated that Sri Lanka has the highest density of elephants in Asia. Human-elephant conflict is increasing due to conversion of elephant habitat to settlements and permanent cultivation.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Katy Payne</span> Expert on animals communication

Katharine Boynton "Katy" Payne is an American zoologist and researcher in the Bioacoustics Research Program at the Laboratory of Ornithology at Cornell University. Payne studied music and biology in college and after a decade doing research in the savanna elephant country in Kenya, Zimbabwe, and Namibia, she founded Cornell's Elephant Listening Project in 1999.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Prusten</span>

Prusten is a form of communicative behaviour exhibited by some members of the family Felidae. Prusten is also referred to as chuffing or chuffle. It is described as a short, low intensity, non-threatening vocalization. In order to vocalize a chuff, the animal's mouth is closed and air is blown through the nostrils, producing a breathy snort. It is typically accompanied by a head bobbing movement. It is often used between two cats as a greeting, during courtship, or by a mother comforting her cubs. The vocalization is produced by tigers, jaguars, snow leopards, clouded leopards and even polar bears. Prusten has significance in both the fields of evolution and conservation.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Roar (vocalization)</span> Deep resonating sound produced by animals

A roar is a type of animal vocalization that is loud, deep and resonating. Many mammals have evolved to produce roars and other roar-like vocals for purposes such as long-distance communication and intimidation. These include various species of big cats, bears, pinnipeds, deer, bovids, elephants and simians.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">African bush elephant</span> Species of mammal

The African bush elephant, also known as the African savanna elephant, is one of two extant African elephant species and one of three extant elephant species. It is the largest living terrestrial animal, with bulls reaching an average shoulder height of 3.04–3.36 metres (10.0–11.0 ft) and a body mass of 5.2–6.9 tonnes (11,500–15,200 lb), with the largest recorded specimen having a shoulder height of 3.96 metres (13.0 ft) and a body mass of 10.4 tonnes (22,900 lb).

Rayleigh waves are a type of surface acoustic wave that travel along the surface of solids. They can be produced in materials in many ways, such as by a localized impact or by piezo-electric transduction, and are frequently used in non-destructive testing for detecting defects. Rayleigh waves are part of the seismic waves that are produced on the Earth by earthquakes. When guided in layers they are referred to as Lamb waves, Rayleigh–Lamb waves, or generalized Rayleigh waves.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Seismic communication</span>

Seismic or vibrational communication is a process of conveying information through mechanical (seismic) vibrations of the substrate. The substrate may be the earth, a plant stem or leaf, the surface of a body of water, a spider's web, a honeycomb, or any of the myriad types of soil substrates. Seismic cues are generally conveyed by surface Rayleigh or bending waves generated through vibrations on the substrate, or acoustical waves that couple with the substrate. Vibrational communication is an ancient sensory modality and it is widespread in the animal kingdom where it has evolved several times independently. It has been reported in mammals, birds, reptiles, amphibians, insects, arachnids, crustaceans and nematode worms. Vibrations and other communication channels are not necessarily mutually exclusive, but can be used in multi-modal communication.

Infrasound is sound at frequencies lower than the low frequency end of human hearing threshold at 20 Hz. It is known, however, that humans can perceive sounds below this frequency at very high pressure levels. Infrasound can come from many natural as well as man-made sources, including weather patterns, topographic features, ocean wave activity, thunderstorms, geomagnetic storms, earthquakes, jet streams, mountain ranges, and rocket launchings. Infrasounds are also present in the vocalizations of some animals. Low frequency sounds can travel for long distances with very little attenuation and can be detected hundreds of miles away from their sources.

Joyce Hatheway Poole is a biologist, ethologist, conservationist, and co-founder/scientific director of ElephantVoices. She is a world authority on elephant reproductive, communicative, and cognitive behavior.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Communication in aquatic animals</span>

Communication occurs when an animal produces a signal and uses it to influences the behaviour of another animal. A signal can be any behavioural, structural or physiological trait that has evolved specifically to carry information about the sender and/or the external environment and to stimulate the sensory system of the receiver to change their behaviour. A signal is different from a cue in that cues are informational traits that have not been selected for communication purposes. For example, if an alerted bird gives a warning call to a predator and causes the predator to give up the hunt, the bird is using the sound as a signal to communicate its awareness to the predator. On the other hand, if a rat forages in the leaves and makes a sound that attracts a predator, the sound itself is a cue and the interaction is not considered a communication attempt.

References

  1. 1 2 3 4 Payne, K. B.; Langauer, W. B (2000). "Elephant Communication". In Shoshani, J. (ed.). Elephants: Majestic Creatures of the Wild. Rodale Press. pp. 116–23. ISBN   978-0-87596-143-9. OCLC   475147472.
  2. Stoeger, Angela S.; Baotic, Anton; Heilmann, Gunnar (August 2021). "Vocal Creativity in Elephant Sound Production". Biology. 10 (8): 750. doi: 10.3390/biology10080750 . ISSN   2079-7737. PMC   8389636 . PMID   34439982.
  3. Beeck, Veronika C.; Heilmann, Gunnar; Kerscher, Michael; Stoeger, Angela S. (2021-06-17). "A novel theory of Asian elephant high-frequency squeak production". BMC Biology. 19 (1): 121. doi: 10.1186/s12915-021-01026-z . ISSN   1741-7007. PMC   8210382 . PMID   34134675.
  4. 1 2 "Mystery of elephant infrasounds revealed". ScienceDaily. Retrieved 2023-02-15.
  5. Joyce, Hoope (January 2011). "The behavioral context of African elephant acoustic communication". In C. J. Moss; H. J. Croze; P. C. Lee (eds.). The Amboseli Elephants: A Long-Term Perspective on a Long-Lived Mammal. University of Chicago Press.
  6. Estes, R. (1991). The behavior guide to African mammals: including hoofed mammals, carnivores, primates . University of California Press. pp.  263–66. ISBN   978-0-520-08085-0.
  7. Fuchs, Evelyn; Beeck, Veronika C.; Baotic, Anton; Stoeger, Angela S. (2021-11-23). "Acoustic structure and information content of trumpets in female Asian elephants (Elephas maximus)". PLOS ONE. 16 (11): e0260284. Bibcode:2021PLoSO..1660284F. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0260284 . ISSN   1932-6203. PMC   8610244 . PMID   34813615.
  8. "What Elephant Calls Mean: A User's Guide". National Geographic . 2014-05-02. Archived from the original on February 18, 2021. Retrieved 2023-02-15.
  9. "World Elephant Day: 25 wild animal facts". CBS News. 12 August 2015. Retrieved 2023-02-15.
  10. Kingdon, p. 63.
  11. "Everything you ever wanted to know about Elephants". WildTrails. 2016-04-09. Retrieved 2023-02-15.
  12. 1 2 Sukumar, R. (11 September 2003). The Living Elephants: Evolutionary Ecology, Behaviour, and Conservation . Oxford University Press, USA. ISBN   978-0-19-510778-4. OCLC   935260783.
  13. Pardo, Michael A.; Fristrup, Kurt; Lolchuragi, David S.; Poole, Joyce H.; Granli, Petter; Moss, Cynthia; Douglas-Hamilton, Iain; Wittemyer, George (2024-06-10). "African elephants address one another with individually specific name-like calls". Nature Ecology & Evolution: 1–12. doi:10.1038/s41559-024-02420-w. ISSN   2397-334X.
  14. 1 2 Herbest, C. T.; Stoeger, A.; Frey, R.; Lohscheller, J.; Titze, I. R.; Gumpenberger, M.; Fitch, W. T. (2012). "How Low Can You Go? Physical Production Mechanism of Elephant Infrasonic Vocalizations". Science. 337 (6094): 595–599. Bibcode:2012Sci...337..595H. doi:10.1126/science.1219712. PMID   22859490. S2CID   32792564.
  15. Payne, K.B.; Langbauer, W.R.; Thomas, E.M. (1986). "Infrasonic calls of the Asian elephant (Elephas maximus)". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 18 (4): 297–301. doi:10.1007/BF00300007. S2CID   1480496.
  16. Larom, D.; Garstang, M.; Payne, K.; Raspet, R.; Lindeque, M. (1997). "The influence of surface atmospheric conditions on the range and area reached by animal vocalizations" (PDF). Journal of Experimental Biology. 200 (Pt 3): 421–31. doi:10.1242/jeb.200.3.421. PMID   9057305.
  17. "Elephants Have Names for Each Other, Study Finds". Yale E360. Retrieved 2024-06-14.
  18. 1 2 Herbest, C. T.; Švec, J. G.; Lohscheller, J.; Frey, R.; Gumpenberger, M.; Stoeger, A.; Fitch, W. T. (2013). "Complex Vibratory Patterns in an Elephant Larynx". Journal of Experimental Biology. 216 (21): 4054–4064. doi: 10.1242/jeb.091009 . PMID   24133151.
  19. O'Connell-Rodwell, E.O. (2007). "Keeping an "ear" to the ground: seismic communication in elephants". Physiology. 22 (4): 287–94. doi:10.1152/physiol.00008.2007. PMID   17699882.
  20. O'Connell-Rodwell C. E.; Arnason, B.; Hart, L. A. (2000). "Seismic properties of Asian elephant (Elephas maximus) vocalizations and locomotion". Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 108 (6): 3066–72. Bibcode:2000ASAJ..108.3066O. doi:10.1121/1.1323460. PMID   11144599.
  21. O'Connell-Rodwell, C. E.; Wood, J. D.; Rodwell, T. C.; Puria, S.; Partan, S. R.; Keefe, R.; Shriver, D.; Arnason, B. T.; Hart, L. A. (1 February 2006). "Wild elephant (Loxodonta africana) breeding herds respond to artificially transmitted seismic stimuli" (PDF). Behavioural and Ecological Sociobiology. 59 (6): 842–50. doi:10.1007/s00265-005-0136-2. S2CID   33221888. Archived (PDF) from the original on 3 December 2013.
  22. 1 2 3 S., Albone, Eric (1984). Mammalian semiochemistry : the investigation of chemical signals between mammals. Wiley. ISBN   0-471-10253-9. OCLC   476223784.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  23. 1 2 3 4 Rasmussen, L.E.L.; Schulte, B.A. (1998). "Chemical signals in the reproduction of Asian (Elephas maximus) and African (Loxodonta africana) elephants". Animal Reproduction Science. 53 (1–4): 19–34. doi:10.1016/s0378-4320(98)00124-9. ISSN   0378-4320. PMID   9835364.
  24. Von Dürckheim, Katharina (2021). Olfaction and scent discrimination in African elephants (Loxodonta africana) (Thesis). Stellenbosch University.