Seismic communication

Last updated
Vibrational communication
DispersionRayleighWave.jpg
Dispersion of Rayleigh waves in a thin gold film on glass
Golden mole dipping its head into sand to detect seismic waves

Seismic or vibrational communication is a process of conveying information through mechanical (seismic) vibrations of the substrate. The substrate may be the earth, a plant stem or leaf, the surface of a body of water, a spider's web, a honeycomb, or any of the myriad types of soil substrates. Seismic cues are generally conveyed by surface Rayleigh or bending waves generated through vibrations on the substrate, or acoustical waves that couple with the substrate. Vibrational communication is an ancient sensory modality and it is widespread in the animal kingdom where it has evolved several times independently. It has been reported in mammals, birds, reptiles, amphibians, insects, arachnids, crustaceans and nematode worms. [1] Vibrations and other communication channels are not necessarily mutually exclusive, but can be used in multi-modal communication.

Contents

Functions

Communication requires a sender, a message, and a recipient, although neither the sender or receiver need be present or aware of the other's intent to communicate at the time of communication.

Intra-specific communication

Vibrations can provide cues to conspecifics about specific behaviours being performed, predator warning and avoidance, herd or group maintenance, and courtship. The Middle East blind mole-rat (Spalax ehrenbergi) was the first mammal for which vibrational communication was documented. These fossorial rodents bang their head against the walls of their tunnels, which was initially interpreted as part of their tunnel building behaviour. It was eventually realised they generate temporally patterned vibrational signals for long-distance communication with neighbouring mole-rats. Footdrumming is used widely as a predator warning or defensive action. It is used primarily by fossorial or semi-fossorial rodents, but has also been recorded for spotted skunks (Spilogale putorius), deer (e.g. white-tailed deer Odocoileus virginianus), marsupials (e.g. tammar wallabies Macropus eugenii), rabbits (e.g. European rabbits Oryctolagus cuniculus) and elephant shrews (Macroscelididae). [2] Banner-tailed kangaroo rats ( Dipodomys spectabilis ) footdrum in the presence of snakes as a form of individual defense and parental care. [3] [4] Several studies have indicated intentional use of ground vibrations as a means of intra-specific communication during courtship among the Cape mole-rat (Georychus capensis). [5] Footdrumming has been reported to be involved in male-male competition where the dominant male indicates its resource holding potential by drumming, thus minimising physical contact with potential rivals. The Asian elephant (Elephas maximus) uses seismic communication in herd or group maintenance [6] and many social insects use seismic vibrations to coordinate the behaviour of group members, for example in cooperative foraging. [7] Other insects use vibrational communication to search for and attract mates, like North American treehoppers, Enchenopa binotata. Males of this species use their abdomen to send vibrations through their host plant's stem. Females perceive these signals and respond to them to initiate a duet. [8] [9] [10]

Inter-specific communication

The banner-tailed kangaroo rat, (Dipodomys spectabilis), produces several complex footdrumming patterns in a number of different contexts, one of which is when it encounters a snake. The footdrumming may alert nearby offspring but most likely conveys that the rat is too alert for a successful attack, thus preventing the snake's predatory pursuit. [7] Vibrations caused by stampeding animals may be sensed by other species to alert them to danger, thereby increasing the size of the stampede and reducing the risk of danger to an individual.

Eavesdropping

Some animals use eavesdropping to either catch their prey or to avoid being caught by predators. Some snakes are able to perceive and react to substrate-borne vibrations. The vibrations are transmitted through the lower jaw, which is often rested on the ground and is connected with the inner ear. They also detect vibrations directly with receptors on their body surface. Studies on horned desert vipers ( Cerastes cerastes ) showed they strongly rely on vibrational cues for capturing prey. Localisation of the prey is probably aided by the two halves of the lower jaw being independent. [7]

Vibrational cues can even indicate the life stage of prey thereby aiding optimal prey selection by predators, e.g. larval vibrations can be distinguished from those generated by pupae, or, adults from juveniles. [11] Although some species can conceal or mask their movements, substrate-borne vibrations are generally more difficult to avoid producing than airborne vibrations. [12] The common angle moth ( Semiothisa aemulataria ) caterpillar escapes predation by lowering itself to safety by a silk thread in response to substrate-borne vibrations produced by approaching predators. [13]

Mimicry

Several animals have learnt to capture prey species by mimicking the vibrational cues of their predators. Wood turtles (Clemmys insculpta), [14] European herring gulls (Larus argentatus), [15] and humans [16] have learnt to vibrate the ground causing earthworms to rise to the surface where they can be easily caught. It is believed that deliberately produced surface vibrations mimic the seismic cues of moles moving through the ground to prey on the worms; the worms respond to these naturally produced vibrations by emerging from their burrows and fleeing across the surface. [16]

Other animals mimic the vibrational cues of prey, only to ambush the predator when it is lured towards the mimic. Assassin bugs (Stenolemus bituberus) hunt web-building spiders by invading the web and plucking the silk to generate vibrations that mimic prey of the spider. This lures the resident spider into striking range of the bug. [17] Spiders from at least five different families routinely invade the webs of other spiders and lure them as prey with vibratory signals (e.g. Pholcus or ‘daddy long-leg’ spiders; salticid ‘jumping’ spiders from the genera Portia , Brettus , Cyrba and Gelotia ). [17]

Portia fimbriata jumping spiders lure female Euryattus species by mimicking male courtship vibrations. [18]

Habitat sensing

The wandering spider ( Cupiennius salei ) can discriminate vibrations created by rain, wind, prey, and potential mates. The creeping grasshopper can escape predation by this spider if it produces vibrations similar enough to those of wind. [19] Thunderstorms and earthquakes produce vibrational cues; these may be used by elephants and birds to attract them to water or avoid earthquakes. Mole rats use reflected, self-generated seismic waves to detect and bypass underground obstacles – a form of "seismic echolocation". [3]

However, this type of use is not considered communication in the strictest sense.

Production of vibrational cues

Vibrational cues can be produced in three ways, through percussion (drumming) on the substrate, vibrations of the body or appendages transmitted to the substrate, or, acoustical waves that couple with the substrate. [20] The strength of these cues depends mostly on the size and muscular power of the animal producing the signal. [21]

Percussion

Percussion, or drumming, can produce both short-and long-distance vibrational cues. Direct percussion of the substrate can yield a much stronger signal than an airborne vocalization that couples with the substrate, however, the strength of the percussive cue is related directly to the mass of the animal producing the vibration. Large size is often associated with greater source amplitudes, leading to a greater propagation range. A wide range of vertebrates perform drumming with some part of their body either on the surface or within burrows. Individuals bang heads, rap trunks or tails, stamp or drum with front feet, hind feet or teeth, thump a gular pouch, and basically employ available appendages to create vibrations on the substrates where they live. [1] [2] Insects use percussion by drumming (or scraping) with the head, hind legs, fore legs, mid legs, wings, abdomen, gaster, antennae or maxillary palps. [22]

Tremulation

Tremulation is performed by a range of insects. This process involves rocking of the entire body with the subsequent vibrations being transferred through the legs to the substrate on which the insect is walking or standing. [23]

Stridulation

Insects and other arthropods stridulate by rubbing together two parts of the body.

A stridulating cricket.

These are referred to generically as the stridulatory organs. Vibrations are transmitted to the substrate through the legs or body.

Tymbal vibrations

Insects possess tymbals which are regions of the exoskeleton modified to form a complex membrane with thin, membranous portions and thickened "ribs". These membranes vibrate rapidly, producing audible sound and vibrations that are transmitted to the substrate.

Acoustically coupled

Elephants produce low-frequency vocalizations at high amplitudes such that they couple with the ground and travel along the surface of the earth. [6] Direct percussion can produce a much stronger signal than airborne vocalizations that couple with the ground, as shown in the Cape mole rat and the Asian elephant. [24] However, the power that an animal can couple into the ground at low frequencies is related directly to its mass. Animals of low mass cannot generate low-frequency vibrational surface waves; thus the mole rat could not produce a vibrational signal at 10–20 Hz like the elephant. Some invertebrates e.g. prairie mole cricket ( Gryllotalpa major ), [25] bushcricket (Tettigoniidae), [26] and cicada [27] produce acoustic communications and substrate vibrations that may be due to acoustic coupling. [22]

For acoustic coupling, low-frequency, high-amplitude vocalizations are necessary for long-distance transmission. It has been suggested that other large mammals such as the lion and rhinoceros may produce acoustically coupled vibrational cues similar to elephants. [20]

Reception of vibrational cues

The star-nose mole Condylura.jpg
The star-nose mole

Vibrational cues are detected by various body parts. Snakes receive signals by sensors in the lower jaw or body, invertebrates by sensors in the legs or body (earthworms), birds by sensors in the legs (pigeons) or bill-tip (shorebirds, kiwis and ibises), mammals by sensors in the feet or lower jaw (mole rats) and kangaroos by sensors in the legs. [28] The star-nosed mole (Condylura cristata), has evolved an elaborate nose structure which may detect seismic waves. [29]

The sensory organs are generically known as somatosensory mechanoreceptors. In insects these sensors are known as campaniform sensillae located near the joints, the subgenual organ in the tibia and Johnston's organ located in the antennae. Arachnids use slit sense organ. In vertebrate animals the sensors are Pacinian corpuscles in placental mammals, similar lamellated corpuscles in marsupials, Herbst corpuscles in birds and a variety of encapsulated or naked nerve endings in other animals. [30]

These sensory receivers detect vibrations in the skin and joints, from which they are typically transmitted as nerve impulses (action potentials) to and through spinal nerves to the spinal cord and then the brain; in snakes, the nerve impulses could be carried through cranial nerves. Alternatively, the sensory receivers may be centralized in the cochlea of the inner ear. Vibrations are transmitted from the substrate to the cochlea through the body (bones, fluids, cartilage, etc.) in an ‘extra-tympanic’ pathway that bypasses the eardrum, and sometimes, even the middle ear. Vibrations then project to the brain along with cues from airborne sound received by the eardrum. [12]

Propagation of vibrational cues

Documented cases of vibrational communication are almost exclusively restricted to Rayleigh waves or bending waves. [12] Seismic energy in the form of Rayleigh waves transmits most efficiently between 10 and 40 Hz. This is the range in which elephants may communicate seismically. [6] [31] In areas with little to no human-generated seismic noise, frequencies around 20 Hz are relatively noise-free, other than vibrations associated with thunder or earth tremors, making it a reasonably quiet communication channel. Both airborne and vibrational waves are subject to interference and alteration from environmental factors. Factors such as wind and temperature influence airborne sound propagation, whereas propagation of seismic signals are affected by the substrate type and heterogeneity. Airborne sound waves spread spherically rather than cylindrically, attenuate more rapidly (losing 6 dB for every doubling of distance) than ground surface waves such as Rayleigh waves (3 dB loss for every doubling of distance), and thus ground surface waves maintain integrity longer. [24] Vibrational signals are probably not very costly to produce for small animals, whereas the generation of air-borne sound is limited by body size.

Benefits and costs of vibrational communication to the signaler are dependent on the function of the signal. For social signaling, daylight and line-of-sight are not required for seismic communication as they are for visual signaling. Likewise, flightless individuals may spend less time locating a potential mate by following the most direct route defined by substrate-borne vibrations, rather than by following sound or chemicals deposited on the path. [12]

Most insects are herbivorous and usually live on plants, therefore the majority of vibrational signals are transmitted through plant stems. Here, communication typically ranges from 0.3 m-2.0 m. It has been suggested that vibrational signals might be adapted to transmit through particular plants. [32]

Insect Mating

Many insects use vibrational communication in mating. In the case of Macrolophus pygmaeus , the males produce two different vibrational sounds intentionally while the females produce none. [33] The males produce two different vibrations, a yelp and a roar. The yelps are produced before copulation. Trialeurodes vaporariorum males also use vibrations to communicate during mating. They produce two types of vibrations, a “chirp”, and a “pulse” and occur in different stages throughout the mating ritual. [34] The occurrence  of the male signals can change due to male rivalry as well. The frequencies and quality of the vibrational calls can change, for example, a higher quality with lower frequency characterizes an aggressive call. The frequencies can also change to avoid signal overlapping which can lower male responsiveness. [33]

Insect Feeding

The use of vibrational communication for feeding is separated in different areas. Depending on the insect, for example a C. pinguis the process in finding new feeding grounds is quite a process. The little treehoppers have a dance which indicates the need to find a new feeding area, and will communicate through vibrations to send a scout to find the new area. [35] [36] Once this scout has been sent it sends vibrations back to the main group, which then moves over to the new area. There is other evidence that it is seemingly instinctual for insects to follow a path that has vibrations, even if they are artificially created in a lab. [35] Caterpillar larvae also send vibrations that help attract others to their feeding locations. This is done at the same time that they are eating. This type of signal was made by them scraping little hairs on the hind side, which is called anal scraping. Caterpillars do this while eating as they can multitask and share their location with other larvae. [37]

Male-Male Interactions in Insects

Vibrational communication is also used in competition. Macrolophus pygmaeus produces a vibrational sound called a “yelp” that is associated with male-male interactions. The yelp is also associated with physical contact between the two males, and then the males running away while emitting yelps. The duration of the signal as well can affect the female’s response, and it was shown that females typically prefer longer calls. [38] Sometimes insects can tell the fitness of a potential mate by their vibrational signals. The stonefly Pteronarcella badia uses vibrational communication in mating. The female stonefly can understand the fitness of males that are “duetting” her vibrational signals by timing how long it takes for the male to find her location. [39]

Examples

American alligator

During courtship, male American alligators use their near-infrasound capabilities to bellow to females, assuming a "reverse-arch" posture at the water's surface (head and tail slightly elevated, midsection barely breaking the surface) using near-infrasound to literally make the water's surface "sprinkle" [40] as they bellow, usually termed as their "water dance" [41] during the mating season.

White-lipped frog

European tree frog with distended gular pouch HylaArboreaCallingMale2.jpg
European tree frog with distended gular pouch

One of the earliest reports of vertebrate signaling using vibrational communication is the bimodal system of sexual advertisement of the white-lipped frog ( Leptodactylus albilabris ). Males on the ground sing airborne advertisement songs that target receptive females, but instead of supporting themselves on their front limbs as other frogs often do, they partially bury themselves in soft soil. As they inflate their vocal sacs to produce the airborne call, the gular pouch impacts the soil as a ‘thump’ that sets up Rayleigh waves which propagate 3–6 m through the substrate. Advertising males space themselves at distances of 1–2 m, thus, the nearest neighbour males are able to receive and respond to substrate-borne vibrations created by other males. [12] [42]

Namib Desert golden mole

Predators may use vibrational communication to detect and capture prey. The Namib Desert golden mole (Eremitalpa granti namibensis) is a blind mammal whose eyelids fuse early in development. The ear lacks a pinna, the reduced ear opening is hidden under fur and the organization of the middle ear indicates it would be sensitive to vibrational cues. The Namib Desert golden mole actively forages at night by dipping its head and shoulders into the sand in conjunction with ‘sand swimming’ as it navigates in search of termite prey producing head-banging alarms. [43] [44] [45] Experimental evidence supports the hypothesis that substrate-borne vibrations produced as wind blows through grassy hummocks influence these moles as they forage on termites associated with the grassy mounds, which are spaced at distances of 20–25 m. The exact mechanism of extracting directional information from the vibrations has not been confirmed. [12]

Elephants

Elephant producing low frequency rumbles

In the late 1990s, Caitlin O'Connell-Rodwell first argued that elephants communicate over long distances using low-pitched rumbles that are barely audible to humans.[ disputed ] [46] Further pioneering research in elephant infrasound communication was done by Katy Payne of the Elephant Listening Project [47] and detailed in her book Silent Thunder. This research is helping our understanding of behaviours such as how elephants can find distant potential mates and how social groups are able to coordinate their movements over extensive ranges. [48] Joyce Poole has also begun decoding elephant utterances that have been recorded over many years of observation, hoping to create a lexicon based on a systematic catalogue of elephant sounds. [49]

Seismic energy transmits most efficiently between 10–40  Hz, i.e. in the same range as the fundamental frequency and 2nd harmonic of an elephant rumble. [50] For Asian elephants, these calls have a frequency of 14–24 Hz, with sound pressure levels of 85–90 dB and last 10–15 seconds. [51] For African elephants, calls range from 15–35 Hz and can be as loud as 117 dB, allowing communication over many kilometers. [48] It seems that when an elephant rumbles, the infrasound that is produced couples with the surface of the earth and then propagates through the ground. In this way, elephants are able to use seismic vibrations at infrasound frequencies for communication. [52] These vibrations can be detected by the skin of an elephant's feet and trunk, which relay the resonant vibrations, similar to the skin on a drum. To listen attentively, individuals will lift one foreleg from the ground, possibly triangulating the source, and face the source of the sound. Occasionally, attentive elephants can be seen to lean forward, putting more weight on their front feet. These behaviours presumably increase the ground contact and sensitivity of the legs. Sometimes, the trunk will be laid on the ground.

Elephants possess several adaptations suited for vibratory communication. The cushion pads of the feet contain cartilaginous nodes and have similarities to the acoustic fat (melon) found in marine mammals like toothed whales and sirenians. In addition, the annular muscle surrounding the ear canal can constrict the passageway, thereby dampening acoustic signals and allowing the animal to hear more seismic signals. [24]

Elephants appear to use vibrational communication for a number of purposes. An elephant running or mock charging can create seismic signals that can be heard at great distances. [6] Vibrational waveforms produced by locomotion appear to travel at distances of up to 32 km (20 mi) while those from vocalizations travel 16 km (9.9 mi). When detecting the vibrational cues of an alarm call signaling danger from predators, elephants enter a defensive posture and family groups will congregate. [53] Vibrational cues are also thought to aid their navigation by use of external sources of infrasound. After the 2004 Boxing Day tsunami in Asia, there were reports that trained elephants in Thailand had become agitated and fled to higher ground before the devastating wave struck, thus saving their own lives and those of the tourists riding on their backs. Because earthquakes and tsunamis generate low-frequency waves, O'Connell-Rodwell and other elephant experts have begun to explore the possibility that the Thai elephants were responding to these events. [46]

Tree Hopper

Calloconophora pinguis use vibrational communication in order to help others of its species find food. They prefer newly growing leaves and need at least two or more new feeding areas while developing from larvae to adulthood. C. pinguis use vibrational communication which seems to be a form of running in place and bumping into others on the same stem. Once this occurs, a chain reaction follows until one finally leaves to scout another stem. This behavior is similar to honeybees, which have scouts for food locating. Once finding a suitable growing stem for the other C. pinguis larvae, it begins to send short vibrations to the old site. Once the group is together, a similar process occurs again when it runs out of food. [35] [36]

Pill Bug

Armadillidium officinalis, these isopods are able to use their legs in order to produce stridulations. This allows them to communicate with each other when feeding, looking for other food sources, as well as using their stridulations in means of a mating call. Another reason the pill bugs use their stridulations to communicate is to let others know that there are predators nearby. [54]

Honey Bee

Apis mellifera, use vibrational signals called tooting and quacking to communicate. This happens primarily on virgin queen cells when there are multiple queens in the hive. Vibrational signals performed on virgin queens after they emerged from the queen cells are connected to their success in the elimination period. [55]

Mole Cricket

Gryllotalpa orientalis make vibrational cues by scraping the forelegs, tapping the forelegs, pal-pal taps, and tremulation. The functions are unknown but it is known that they are not for mating or sexual selection. [56] Gryllotalpa major (prairie mole crickets), however, use underground vibrational cues as one part of their mating call. [57]

"Tok-Tok" Beetle

Psammodes striatus repeatedly taps its abdomen on the ground, sending vibrational cues to communicate with other beetles. [58] In male-female communication, the male typically initiates the tapping. The female responds by tapping as well, and as the communication continues, the female stays in place while the male tries to locate her. [59]

See also

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Infrasound</span> Vibrations with frequencies lower than 20 hertz

Infrasound, sometimes referred to as low frequency sound, describes sound waves with a frequency below the lower limit of human audibility. Hearing becomes gradually less sensitive as frequency decreases, so for humans to perceive infrasound, the sound pressure must be sufficiently high. Although the ear is the primary organ for sensing low sound, at higher intensities it is possible to feel infrasound vibrations in various parts of the body.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Golden mole</span> Monotypic family of mammals

Golden moles are small insectivorous burrowing mammals endemic to Sub-Saharan Africa. They comprise the family Chrysochloridae and as such they are taxonomically distinct from the true moles, family Talpidae, and other mole-like families, all of which, to various degrees, they resemble as a result of evolutionary convergence. There are 21 species. Some are relatively common, whereas others are rare and endangered.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Animal communication</span> Transfer of information from animal to animal

Animal communication is the transfer of information from one or a group of animals to one or more other animals that affects the current or future behavior of the receivers. Information may be sent intentionally, as in a courtship display, or unintentionally, as in the transfer of scent from predator to prey with kairomones. Information may be transferred to an "audience" of several receivers. Animal communication is a rapidly growing area of study in disciplines including animal behavior, sociology, neurology and animal cognition. Many aspects of animal behavior, such as symbolic name use, emotional expression, learning and sexual behavior, are being understood in new ways.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Bioacoustics</span> Study of sound relating to biology

Bioacoustics is a cross-disciplinary science that combines biology and acoustics. Usually it refers to the investigation of sound production, dispersion and reception in animals. This involves neurophysiological and anatomical basis of sound production and detection, and relation of acoustic signals to the medium they disperse through. The findings provide clues about the evolution of acoustic mechanisms, and from that, the evolution of animals that employ them.

<i>Leucorchestris arenicola</i> Species of spider

Leucorchestris arenicola, commonly called the dancing white lady spider, is a huntsman spider in the family Sparassidae and genus Leucorchestris. It is commonly found in the Namib desert of Namibia. It is often mistaken with the similarly named Carparachne aureoflava, or more commonly known as the wheel spider from the same location. L. arenicola relies on seismic vibrations, called drumming, for communication. It taps its foremost legs on the sand to send messages to other white lady spiders. Male L. arenicola will travel over 50 m in one night searching for a mate. If they find a mate, they must be extremely careful, for drumming the wrong message can be deadly. One of the major features that characterizes its nocturnal behavior is its specialized vision, using eight eyes in different orientations to capture a panoramic view of the surroundings. L. arenicola spiders use temporal summation in order to be able to see dim lighting during night-time wanderings. The species was first described by Reginald Frederick Lawrence in 1962, who described all the species in the genus Leucorchestris.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Alarm signal</span> Signal made by social animals to warn others of danger

In animal communication, an alarm signal is an antipredator adaptation in the form of signals emitted by social animals in response to danger. Many primates and birds have elaborate alarm calls for warning conspecifics of approaching predators. For example, the alarm call of the blackbird is a familiar sound in many gardens. Other animals, like fish and insects, may use non-auditory signals, such as chemical messages. Visual signs such as the white tail flashes of many deer have been suggested as alarm signals; they are less likely to be received by conspecifics, so have tended to be treated as a signal to the predator instead.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Display (zoology)</span> Set of ritualized behaviours in animals

Display behaviour is a set of ritualized behaviours that enable an animal to communicate to other animals about specific stimuli. Such ritualized behaviours can be visual, but many animals depend on a mixture of visual, audio, tactical and chemical signals. Evolution has tailored these stereotyped behaviours to allow animals to communicate both conspecifically and interspecifically which allows for a broader connection in different niches in an ecosystem. It is connected to sexual selection and survival of the species in various ways. Typically, display behaviour is used for courtship between two animals and to signal to the female that a viable male is ready to mate. In other instances, species may make territorial displays, in order to preserve a foraging or hunting territory for its family or group. A third form is exhibited by tournament species in which males will fight in order to gain the 'right' to breed. Animals from a broad range of evolutionary hierarchies avail of display behaviours - from invertebrates such as the simple jumping spider to the more complex vertebrates like the harbour seal.

<i>Zygiella x-notata</i> Species of spider

Zygiella x-notata, sometimes known as the missing sector orb weaver or the silver-sided sector spider, is a spider species in the family Araneidae. They are solitary spiders, residing in daily spun orb webs. Z. x-notata is a member of the genus Zygiella, the orb-weaving spiders. The adult female is easily recognized by the characteristic leaf-like mark on her posterior opisthosoma, caudal to the yellow-brown cephalothorax.

<i>Maratus volans</i> Species of spider

Maratus volans is a species in the jumping spider family (Salticidae), belonging to the genus Maratus. These spiders are native to certain areas in Australia and occupy a wide distribution of habitats. They have a specialized visual system that allows them to see the full visible spectrum as well as in the ultraviolet-range; this helps them detect and pursue prey. Males of this species are characterized by their colourful abdomen flaps that are used to attract females during courtship.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Aggressive mimicry</span> Deceptive mimicry of a harmless species by a predator

Aggressive mimicry is a form of mimicry in which predators, parasites, or parasitoids share similar signals, using a harmless model, allowing them to avoid being correctly identified by their prey or host. Zoologists have repeatedly compared this strategy to a wolf in sheep's clothing. In its broadest sense, aggressive mimicry could include various types of exploitation, as when an orchid exploits a male insect by mimicking a sexually receptive female, but will here be restricted to forms of exploitation involving feeding. For example, indigenous Australians who dress up as and imitate kangaroos when hunting would not be considered aggressive mimics, nor would a human angler, though they are undoubtedly practising self-decoration camouflage. Treated separately is molecular mimicry, which shares some similarity; for instance a virus may mimic the molecular properties of its host, allowing it access to its cells. An alternative term, Peckhamian mimicry, has been suggested, but it is seldom used.

Rayleigh waves are a type of surface acoustic wave that travel along the surface of solids. They can be produced in materials in many ways, such as by a localized impact or by piezo-electric transduction, and are frequently used in non-destructive testing for detecting defects. Rayleigh waves are part of the seismic waves that are produced on the Earth by earthquakes. When guided in layers they are referred to as Lamb waves, Rayleigh–Lamb waves, or generalized Rayleigh waves.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Six-spotted fishing spider</span> Species of spider

The six-spotted fishing spider is an arachnid from the nursery web spider family Pisauridae. This species is from the genus Dolomedes, or the fishing spiders. Found in wetland habitats throughout North America, these spiders are usually seen scampering along the surface of ponds and other bodies of water. They are also referred to as dock spiders because they can sometimes be witnessed quickly vanishing through the cracks of boat docks. D. triton gets its scientific name from the Greek mythological god Triton, who is the messenger of the big sea and the son of Poseidon.

<i>Phidippus clarus</i> Species of spider

Phidippus clarus, also known as the brilliant jumping spider, is a species of jumping spider found in old fields throughout eastern North America. It often waits upside down near the top of a plant, which may be useful for detecting prey, and then quickly jumps down before the prey can escape. The spider is one of 60 species in the genus Phidippus, and one of about 5,000 in the Salticidae, a family that accounts for about 10% of all spider species. P. clarus is a predator, mostly consuming insects, other spiders, and other terrestrial arthropods.

<i>Portia fimbriata</i> Species of spider

Portia fimbriata, sometimes called the fringed jumping spider, is a jumping spider found in Australia and Southeast Asia. Adult females have bodies 6.8 to 10.5 millimetres long, while those of adult males are 5.2 to 6.5 millimetres long. Both sexes have a generally dark brown carapace, reddish brown chelicerae ("fangs"), a brown underside, dark brown palps with white hairs, and dark brown abdomens with white spots on the upper side. Both sexes have fine, faint markings and soft fringes of hair, and the legs are spindly and fringed. However, specimens from New Guinea and Indonesia have orange-brown carapaces and yellowish abdomens. In all species of the genus Portia, the abdomen distends when the spider is well fed or producing eggs.

Lizards are among the most diverse groups of reptiles with more than 5,600 species. With such diversity in physical and behavioral characteristics, lizards have evolved many different ways to communicate. Lizards communicate to gain information about the individuals around them by paying attention to various characteristics exhibited by individuals and using various physical and behavioral traits to communicate. These traits differ based on the mode of communication being used.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Biotremology</span>

Biotremology is the study of production, dispersion and reception of mechanical vibrations by organisms, and their effect on behavior. This involves neurophysiological and anatomical basis of vibration production and detection, and relation of vibrations to the medium they disperse through. Vibrations can represent either signals used in vibrational (seismic) communication or inadvertent cues used, for example, in locating prey. In almost all known cases, they are transmitted as surface waves along the boundary of a medium, i.e. Rayleigh waves or bending waves. While most attention is directed towards the role of vibrations in animal behavior, plants actively respond to sounds and vibrations as well, so this subject is shared with plant bioacoustics. Other groups of organisms are also postulated to either actively produce or at least use vibrations to sense their environment, but those are currently far less studied.

<i>Habronattus pyrrithrix</i> Species of spider

Habronattus pyrrithrix is a species of jumping spider in the family Salticidae. It is found in the southwestern United States and western Mexico.

<i>Pardosa milvina</i> Species of arachnid

Pardosa milvina, the shore spider, is a species in the wolf spider family. They are mainly found near rivers and in agricultural areas in eastern North America. P. milvina feed on a large variety of small insects and spiders. Ground beetles such as Scarites quadriceps and large wolf spiders such as Tigrosa helluo are predators of P. milvina. P. milvina are smaller spiders with thin, long legs. This species captures prey such as arthropods with their legs and then kills them with their venom. Their predators are larger wolf spiders and beetles. P. milvina are able to detect these predators from chemotactile and vibratory cues. These spiders lose limbs when escaping from predators and they can change their preferred location in order to avoid predators. P. milvina also use chemical cues in order to mate. During their mating ritual, the male raises his legs and shakes his body. Both males and females can use silk, a chemotactile cue, for sexual communication. Additionally, female shore spiders heavily invest in their offspring, keeping them in egg sacs and carrying them for a few weeks after they are born.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Communication in aquatic animals</span>

Communication occurs when an animal produces a signal and uses it to influences the behaviour of another animal. A signal can be any behavioural, structural or physiological trait that has evolved specifically to carry information about the sender and/or the external environment and to stimulate the sensory system of the receiver to change their behaviour. A signal is different from a cue in that cues are informational traits that have not been selected for communication purposes. For example, if an alerted bird gives a warning call to a predator and causes the predator to give up the hunt, the bird is using the sound as a signal to communicate its awareness to the predator. On the other hand, if a rat forages in the leaves and makes a sound that attracts a predator, the sound itself is a cue and the interaction is not considered a communication attempt.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Elephant communication</span> Communication between elephants

Elephants communicate with each other in various ways, including touching, visual displays, vocalisations, seismic vibrations, and semiochemicals.

References

  1. 1 2 Hill, P.S.M., (2008). Vibrational Communication in Animals. Harvard, Cambridge, London
  2. 1 2 Randall, J.A., (2001). Evolution and function of drumming as communication in mammals. American Zoologist, 41: 1143–1156
  3. 1 2 "Vibrational communication in mammals". Map of Life. Retrieved 8 December 2012.
  4. Randall, M.D. and Matocq, J.A., (1997). Why do kangaroo rats (Dipodomys spectabilis) footdrum at snakes? Behavioural Ecology, 8: 404–413
  5. Narins, P.M., Reichman, O.J., Jarvis, J.U.M. and Lewis, E.R., (1992). Seismic signal transmission between burrows of the Cape mole-rat Georychus capensis. Journal of Comparative Physiology [A], 170: 13–22
  6. 1 2 3 4 O’Connell-Rodwell, C.E., Arnason, B. and Hart, L.A., (2000). Seismic properties of elephant vocalizations and locomotion. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 108: 3066–3072
  7. 1 2 3 "Web of Life:Vibrational communication in animals" . Retrieved 8 December 2012.
  8. Mcnett, G.D. and Cocroft, R.B. (2008). Host shifts favor vibrational signal divergence in Enchenopa binotata treehoppers. Behavioral Ecology. 19.3: 650–656.
  9. Wood, T.K., 1980. Intraspecific divergence in Enchenopa binotata Say (Homoptera: Membracidae) effected by host plant adaptation. Evolution, 34: 147–160
  10. Wood, T.K. & Guttman, S.I., 1982. Ecological and behavioural basis for reproductive isolation in the sympatric Enchenopa binotata complex (Homoptera: Membracidae). Evolution, 36: 233–242.
  11. Meyhöfer, R., Casas, J. and Dorn, S., (1994). Host location by a parasitoid using leafminer vibrations: characterizing the vibrational signals produced by the leafmining host. Physiological Entomology, 19: 349–359
  12. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Hill, P.S.M., (2009). How do animals use substrate-borne vibrations as an information source? Naturwissenschaften, 96: 1355–1371. doi:10.1007/s00114-009-0588-8
  13. Castellanos, Ignacio; Barbosa, Pedro; Zuria, Iriana; Caldas, Astrid (2010). "Does the Mimetic Posture ofMacaria aemulataria(Walker) (Geometridae) Larvae Enhance Survival Against Bird Predation?". Journal of the Lepidopterists' Society. 64 (2). Lepidopterists' Society: 113–115. doi:10.18473/lepi.v64i2.a9. ISSN   0024-0966. S2CID   88233211.
  14. Kaufmann, J.H. (1986) Stomping for earthworms by wood turtles, Clemmys insculpta: a newly discovered foraging technique. Copeia, 1986: 1001–1004
  15. Tinbergen, N., (1960). The Herring Gull's World. New York: Basic Books, Inc.
  16. 1 2 Catania, K.C., (2008). Worm grunting, fiddling, and charming — humans unknowingly mimic a predator to harvest bait. Public Library of Science ONE, 3(10): e3472. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0003472
  17. 1 2 Wignall, A.E. and Taylor, P.W., (2010). Assassin bug uses aggressive mimicry to lure spider prey. Proceedings of the Royal Society B. doi:10.1098/rspb.2010.2060
  18. Jackson, R.R. and Wilcox, R.S., (1990). Aggressive mimicry, prey-specific predatory behaviour and predator-recognition in the predator–prey interactions of Portia fimbriata and Euryattus sp., jumping spiders from Queensland. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 26: 111–119. doi:10.1007/BF00171580
  19. Barth, F.G., Bleckmann, H., Bohnenberger, J. and Seyfarth, E-A., (1988). Spiders of the genus Cupiennius Simon 1891 (Araneae, Ctenidae): II. On the vibratory environment of a wandering spider. Oecologia, 77:194–201
  20. 1 2 O'Connell-Rodwell, C.E., Hart,L.A. and Arnason, B.T., (2001). Exploring the potential use of seismic waves as a communication channel by elephants and other large mammals. American Zoologist, 41(5): 1157–1170. doi:
  21. Markl, H., (1983). Vibrational communication. In: Neuroethology and Behavioral Physiology, edited by Huber, F. and Markl, H. Berlin: Springer-Verlag, pp. 332–353
  22. 1 2 Virant-Doberlet, Meta; Čokl, Andrej (2004). "Vibrational communication in insects". Neotropical Entomology. 33 (2): 121–134. doi: 10.1590/S1519-566X2004000200001 .
  23. Sarmiento-Ponce, Edith Julieta (2014). "Acoustic communication in insects" (PDF). Quehacer Científico en Chiapas. 9: 50. Retrieved 4 November 2018.
  24. 1 2 3 O’Connell-Rodwell, E.O., (2007). Keeping an “ear” to the ground: seismic communication in elephants. Physiology, 22: 287–294. doi:10.1152/physiol.00008.200
  25. Hill, P.S.M. & J.R. Shadley. (2001). Talking back: Sending soil vibration signals to lekking prairie mole cricket males. American Zoologist, 41: 1200–1214
  26. Kalmring, K., Jatho, M., Rossler, W. and Sickmann, T. (1997). Acousto-vibratory communication in bushcrickets (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae). Entomology Genetics, 21: 265–291
  27. Stölting, H., Moore T.E. and Lakes-Harlan. R., (2002). Substrate vibrations during acoustic signalling in the cicada Okanagana rimosa. Journal of Insect Science, 2: 2: 1–7
  28. Gregory, J.E., McIntyre, A.K. and Proske, U., (1986). Vibration-evoked responses from lamellated corpuscles in the legs of kangaroos. Experimental Brain Research, 62: 648–653
  29. Catania, K.C., (1999). A nose that looks like a hand and acts like an eye: the unusual mechanosensory system of the star-nosed mole. Journal of Comparative Physiology [A], 185: 367–372
  30. "Web of Life:Vibrational communication in insects and spiders" . Retrieved 8 December 2012.
  31. Arnason, B.T., Hart, L.A.and O’Connell-Rodwell, C.E., (2002). The properties of geophysical fields and their effects on elephants and other animals. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 116: 123–132
  32. "Web of Life:Vibrational communication in insects and spiders" . Retrieved 8 December 2012.
  33. 1 2 Gemeno, César; Baldo, Giordana; Nieri, Rachele; Valls, Joan; Alomar, Oscar; Mazzoni, Valerio (2015-07-01). "Substrate-Borne Vibrational Signals in Mating Communication of Macrolophus Bugs". Journal of Insect Behavior. 28 (4): 482–498. Bibcode:2015JIBeh..28..482G. doi:10.1007/s10905-015-9518-0. hdl: 10459.1/72205 . ISSN   1572-8889. S2CID   86053.
  34. Fattoruso, Valeria; Anfora, Gianfranco; Mazzoni, Valerio (2021-03-22). "Vibrational communication and mating behavior of the greenhouse whitefly Trialeurodes vaporariorum (Westwood) (Hemiptera: Aleyrodidae)". Scientific Reports. 11 (1): 6543. Bibcode:2021NatSR..11.6543F. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-85904-0 . ISSN   2045-2322. PMC   7985380 . PMID   33753797.
  35. 1 2 3 Cocroft, Reginald B (2005-05-22). "Vibrational communication facilitates cooperative foraging in a phloem-feeding insect". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 272 (1567): 1023–1029. doi:10.1098/rspb.2004.3041. ISSN   0962-8452. PMC   1599876 . PMID   16024360.
  36. 1 2 Cocroft, Reginald B.; Hamel, Jennifer A. "4. Vibrational communication in the "other insect societies": A diversity of ecology, signals and signal functions" (PDF). Transworld Research Network.
  37. Yadav, C.; Guedes, R. N. C.; Matheson, S. M.; Timbers, T. A.; Yack, J. E. (2017-02-21). "Invitation by vibration: recruitment to feeding shelters in social caterpillars". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 71 (3): 51. doi:10.1007/s00265-017-2280-x. ISSN   1432-0762. S2CID   25446485.
  38. Gemeno, César; Baldo, Giordana; Nieri, Rachele; Valls, Joan; Alomar, Oscar; Mazzoni, Valerio (July 2015). "Substrate-Borne Vibrational Signals in Mating Communication of Macrolophus Bugs". Journal of Insect Behavior. 28 (4): 482–498. Bibcode:2015JIBeh..28..482G. doi:10.1007/s10905-015-9518-0. hdl: 10459.1/72205 . ISSN   0892-7553. S2CID   86053.
  39. Stewart, Kenneth W. (1997-04-01). "Insect Life: Vibrational Communication in Insects". American Entomologist. 43 (2): 81–91. doi: 10.1093/ae/43.2.81 . ISSN   1046-2821.
  40. Male alligator "sprinkling" while bellowing in near-infrasound during courtship. YouTube.com (April 28, 2010). Retrieved on 2016-09-07.
  41. Garrick, L. D.; Lang, J. W. (1977). "Social Displays of the American Alligator". American Zoologist. 17: 225–239. doi: 10.1093/icb/17.1.225 .
  42. Lewis, E.R.and Narins, P.M., (1985). Do frogs communicate with seismic signals? Science, 227: 187–189
  43. Narins, P.M., Lewis, E.R., Jarvis, J.J.U.M. and O’Riain, J., (1997). The use of seismic signals by fossorial southern African mammals: a neuroethological gold mine. Brain Research Bulletin, 44: 641–646
  44. Mason, M.J., (2003). Bone conduction and seismic sensitivity in golden moles (Chrysochloridae). Journal of Zoology, 260: 405–413
  45. Mason, M.J. and Narins, P.M., (2002). Seismic sensitivity in the desert golden mole (Eremitalpa granti): a review. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 116: 158–163
  46. 1 2 "Scientists unravel the secret world of elephant communication" . Retrieved 2010-08-25.
  47. "Elephant Listening Project" . Retrieved 2007-06-16.
  48. 1 2 Larom, D., Garstang, M., Payne, K., Raspet, R. and Lindeque, M., (1999). The influence of surface atmospheric conditions on the range and area reached by animal vocalizations. Journal of Experimental Biology, 200: 421–431 [url=http://jeb.biologists.org/cgi/reprint/200/3/421.pdf]
  49. BARCUS, CHRISTY ULLRICH (2014-05-02). "What Elephant Calls Mean: A User's Guide". National Geographic. Archived from the original on April 25, 2017. Retrieved 4 November 2018.
  50. Granli, Petter. "Seismic communication" . Retrieved 2010-08-25.
  51. Payne, K.B.; Langbauer, W.R.; Thomas, E.M. (1986). "Infrasonic calls of the Asian elephant (Elephas maximus)". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 18 (4): 297–301. doi:10.1007/BF00300007. S2CID   1480496.
  52. Günther, R.H., O'Connell-Rodwell, C.E. and Klemperer, S.L., (2004). Seismic waves from elephant vocalizations: A possible communication mode? Geophysical Research Letters, 31: L11602. doi:10.1029/2004GL019671
  53. O’Connell-Rodwell, C.E., Wood, J.D., Rodwell, T.C. Puria, S., Partan, S.R., Keefe, R., Shriver, D., Arnason, B.T. and Hart, L.A., (2006). Wild elephant (Loxodonta africana) breeding herds respond to artificially transmitted seismic stimuli. Behavioral and Ecological Sociobiology, 59: 842–850. doi:10.1007/s00265-005-0136-2 "Archived copy" (PDF). Archived from the original (PDF) on 2013-12-03. Retrieved 2012-11-28.{{cite web}}: CS1 maint: archived copy as title (link)
  54. Cividini, Sofia; Montesanto, Giuseppe (2020). "Biotremology in arthropods". Learning & Behavior. 48 (3): 281–300. doi:10.3758/s13420-020-00428-3. ISSN   1543-4494. PMC   7473968 . PMID   32632754.
  55. Schneider, S.S.; Painter-Kurt, S.; Degrandi-Hoffman, G. (2001-06-01). "The role of the vibration signal during queen competition in colonies of the honeybee, Apis mellifera". Animal Behaviour. 61 (6): 1173–1180. doi:10.1006/anbe.2000.1689. ISSN   0003-3472. S2CID   26650968.
  56. Hayashi, Yaoko; Yoshimura, Jin; Roff, Derek A.; Kumita, Tetsuro; Shimizu, Akira (2018-10-10). "Four types of vibration behaviors in a mole cricket". PLOS ONE. 13 (10): e0204628. Bibcode:2018PLoSO..1304628H. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0204628 . ISSN   1932-6203. PMC   6179226 . PMID   30304041.
  57. Hill, P. S. M; Shadley, J. R. (1997-10-30). "Substrate Vibration as a Component of a Calling Song". Naturwissenschaften. 84 (10): 460–463. Bibcode:1997NW.....84..460H. doi:10.1007/s001140050429. ISSN   0028-1042. S2CID   6917151.
  58. Lighton, John R. B. (1987). "Cost of tokking: the energetics of substrate communication in the tok-tok beetle,Psammodes striatus". Journal of Comparative Physiology B. 157 (1): 11–20. doi:10.1007/BF00702723. ISSN   0174-1578. S2CID   20119092.
  59. Lighton, John R.B. (2019-01-02). "Knock-knock, who's there: Sex-coding, competition and the energetics of tapping communication in the tok-tok beetle, Psammodes striatus (Coleoptera: Tenebrionidae)": 509257. doi:10.1101/509257. S2CID   92277559.{{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help)