Machiavellianism (psychology)

Last updated
Machiavellianism
The Dark Triad.png
Machiavellianism is one of the traits in the dark triad model, along with psychopathy and narcissism.
Specialty Personality psychology
CausesGenetics and upbringing [1] [2]

In the field of personality psychology, Machiavellianism (sometimes abbreviated as MACH) is the name of a personality trait construct characterized by interpersonal manipulation, indifference to morality, lack of empathy, and a calculated focus on self-interest. [3] [4] [5] [6] Psychologists Richard Christie and Florence L. Geis created the construct and named it after Niccolò Machiavelli, as they used truncated and edited statements similar to his writing style to study variations in human behaviors. [7] [8] [9] The construct's relation to the thinker himself is exclusively nominal. [10] Their Mach IV test, a 20-question, Likert-scale personality survey, became the standard self-assessment tool and scale of the Machiavellianism construct. Those who score high on the scale (High Machs) are more likely to have a high level of manipulativeness, deceitfulness and a cynical, unemotional temperament. [11] [12]

Contents

It is one of the dark triad traits, along with the subclinical versions of narcissism and psychopathy. [13] [14] [15]

Origin of the construct

Richard Christie was influenced by the works of Niccolo Machiavelli as an undergraduate. Portrait of Niccolo Machiavelli by Santi di Tito.jpg
Richard Christie was influenced by the works of Niccolò Machiavelli as an undergraduate.

Adapting Machiavelli for psychometric use

In 1955, psychologist Richard Christie set out to study the thought processes and actions of those who manipulated others, such as political ideologues and religious extremists. [16] He found that there was much literature on those who followed organizations and movements, but very little on those who led them. He began by conceptualizing what qualities a potential manipulator would have, such as a lack of empathy and affect, and being unconcerned with moral standards. Then as Christie was developing a psychometric for interpersonal manipulation, he was reminded of his encounter with Machiavelli's writings as an undergraduate, and wanted to adapt them to suit his research. [17] In the 1960s, Christie and his colleagues would then develop a test using a selection of statements, including truncated and edited sentences that they viewed were similar to certain phrases found in Machiavelli's works such as The Prince and The Discourses on Livy as test items, naming the construct "Machiavellianism" after him. [18] [19] [4] They wanted to assess whether or not those who were in agreement with the statements would behave differently than others who disagreed, specifically in regards to manipulative actions. Christie and his research partner Florence L. Geis published their results in their book, titled " Studies in Machiavellianism ", in 1970. [20]

Christie made light of the difficulty in adapting Machiavelli's writings into a non-political test, joking that his advice was "better suited for Renaissance princes" than for college undergraduates. [21] Christie used more colloquial, everyday wording when creating the scale items. [22] Thus the test items used were "theoretically congruent with" or loosely based on Machiavelli's tone, instead of literal, direct statements from his works. [23]

On the name of the scale

While the construct is named "Machiavellianism", it does not refer to the political theory espoused in Machiavelli's books (also called Machiavellianism). [24] [25] [26] [27] [28] Because the two concepts share the same name, they can be confused for and conflated with one another, even though his political ideas are not relevant to psychology. [28] [29] [27] [30] Scholars have asserted that the variable has no relation to Machiavelli outside of bearing his name, and that it has nothing to do with Machiavelli's politics. [29] Christie himself makes clear that he used sentences inspired by Machiavelli's works only as a sort of litmus test to study deceptive and manipulative behavior, and that his concern was not with Machiavelli's historical or political influence, stating specifically that: [31]

Historians disagree as to whether Machiavelli was a cynic who wrote political satire, a patriot, or the first modern political scientist. The present concern is not with Machiavelli as an historic figure, but as the source of ideas about those who manipulate others.

Christie stated that he chose the name "Machiavellianism" out of convenience seeing as other names such as "M scale" (M for manipulation) and "Ma scale" (which was used by the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory for hypomania) were taken. Names also considered were the "manipulator" or "operator" scale, but these terms had issues regarding behavioral validity. He also admits that by using the name "Machiavellianism", the scale would "create problems of public misunderstanding" due to the pre-existing political meaning of the term. [32] In a later essay, Christie even states that some have viewed the name problematic and that "the use of the word Machiavellianism to describe the content of the scales has been questioned". [33]

MACH-IV

Christie and Geis's MACH IV test, a 20-question, Likert-scale personality survey, became the standard self-report tool to measure one's level of Machiavellianism. [8] Those who score highly on the scale are classified as high Machs, while those who score low are classified as low Machs. [4] "Machiavellian" is also used but has a far more technical use in this context, primarily in reference to this scale and those who score high on it. [34] Using their scale, Christie and Geis conducted multiple experimental tests that showed that the interpersonal strategies and behavior of "high Machs" and "low Machs" differ. [35] People scoring high on the scale tend to endorse manipulative statements, and behave accordingly, contrary to those who score lowly. [36] People scoring high on the scale tend to endorse statements such as, "Never tell anyone the real reason you did something unless it is useful to do so," (No. 1) but not ones like, "Most people are basically good and kind" (No. 4), "There is no excuse for lying to someone else" (No. 7) or "Most people who get ahead in the world lead clean, moral lives" (No. 11) [37] Their basic results have been widely replicated. [38] Measured on the Mach IV scale, males score, on average, slightly higher on Machiavellianism than females. [35] [39]

The Mach IV test influenced the creation of an assessment called the Dirty Dozen, which contains 12 items, and the Short Dark Triad, composed of 27 items. [40] The MACH-IV (and other Machiavellianism tests) also feature questions loosely inspired by other thinkers such as Chinese political authors, not just Machiavelli. [41] [42] [43]

High vs Low Machiavellianism

Like all personality traits, Machiavellianism is a characteristic that exists on a spectrum or continuum. [44] [45] Christie and others noted that High Machs were more likely to cheat in experimental games, manipulate others around them, and were generally detached in their interactions. This was opposed to the behavior of Low Machs, who found such behaviors immoral, and refrained from them. [46]

The higher a person is on the Machiavellianism scale, the more likely they are to deceive and exploit at the expense of someone else, engage in unprincipled behavior, and have a lack of empathetic feelings. [4]

Core features

In developing the construct, Christie theorized that a "manipulator" or "operator" would possess the following characteristics: [47]

1. A relative lack of affect in interpersonal relationships: Manipulators do not empathize with their victims. The more empathy one has, Christie says, the less likely one will manipulate a person to do their bidding.

2. A lack of concern for conventional morality: Christie asserts that the manipulator is not concerned with the morality of behaviors such as lying and cheating.

3. A lack of gross psychopathology : Christie states that manipulators usually have an instrumentalist view of the world, which shows a lack of psychosis or other mental impairments.

4. Low ideological commitment: Manipulators prefer to focus on getting things done pragmatically rather than focus on ideological allegiances. Christie states that while manipulators are to be found in organizations of differing ideals, they are more likely to be interested in tactics that achieve individual ends than inflexible idealistic ones. [47]

Five-Factor Model

Under the recently devised Five-Factor Model of Machiavellianism, three characteristics underlie the construct: [48]

Causes

Genetic and environmental

Several behavioral genetics studies have shown that Machiavellianism has both significantly genetic and environmental influences. [49] [50] [51] [52] Researchers have noted that while Machiavellianism is heritable to a substantial degree, it can also be influenced by the shared-environment (i.e. sibling groups) slightly more than narcissism and psychopathy. [53] Other traits associated with Machiavellianism are influenced by genetics as well, as one study notes that "The co-occurrence of alexithymia and Machiavellianism was most heavily influenced by genetic factors, and to a lesser but significant extent by non-shared environmental factors." [54] Machiavellianism is also heavily correlated with primary psychopathy which is itself strongly heritable. [55] A study on the "core" of dark triad traits also emphasized that the residual traits of Machiavellianism had "significant genetic components". [2] The relationship between Machiavellianism and mental toughness is also moderated by environmental factors and a genetic basis. [56]

One particular study found a gene responsible for dopamine reception was positively associated with individuals who scored high on the MACH IV, but it is unclear what specific mechanisms cause this effect. [57] [58] A study noted that the emotionality of Machiavellianism may also be genetically determined, with the authors stating that "it can be expected that in the case of Machiavellianism, the genetic influence may manifest itself by affecting the emotional sphere. That affective insensitivity – as McIlwain (2003) designates it--makes Machiavellians similar to psychopaths. In the study by Vernon et al (2008), genetic factors for Machiavellianism and psychopathy were correlated, which suggests that the variability of both qualities is greatly affected by the same genes. The genetically determined “cool syndrome” makes it easier for the child to use effective manipulation, leading to the development of a permanent behaviour strategy, at the same time protecting the child from internal punishments such as shame or guilt." [59]

Studies have found a link between Machiavellianism and a blunted cortisol awaking response (CAR). [60] A person's cortisol awaking response has been associated with a lack of affective empathy, and is highly heritable. [61] [62]

Environmental causes

The environmental causes (such as shared, and non-shared environment) that contribute to the development of machiavellianism were childhood maltreatment and neglect, social reinforcement of manipulative behaviors from an early age, and poor family functioning. [63] [64] One study even stated that "the etiology of Machiavellianism, similarly to the development of a dismissing-avoidant pattern, partly originates from childhood experiences obtained in relationships with unexpressive, less understanding, highly punitive or restrictive caregivers". [65] In many studies, Machiavellianism has been heavily correlated with negative home atmospheres, loneliness, and adverse parental experiences. One study noted that punishment specifically led to the "emergence of deceitful and exploitative interpersonal tactics." The authors of the study concluded that these results "might give further support for the idea that Machiavellian personality traits are possible strategic responses to childhood adversities". [64] Another study found the traits of Machiavellianism to be a response to early maladaptive schemas (EMS), essentially coping mechanisms for emotional deprivation, mistrust, abuse, and caregiver rejection. [66] Irregular parent care, such as a mother being neglectful and a father being overprotective, has also been correlated with Machiavellianism. [67] The hereditary influence on Machiavellianism may make it challenging to disentangle the genetic underpinnings from the effects of parental upbringing and environmental factors. [68] Another study also echoed similarly that it is difficult to separate genetic contributions from parenting environments, and that "negative fathers might be high on Machiavellian traits and therefore, the transmission might be genetic rather than experiential". [68]

In general, Jones (2020) asserts that "there may be a myriad of reasons, including some environmental reasons, as to why someone would become cynical, habitually ignore morality, or use manipulative persuasion. There may be others who are born cynical, manipulative, and amoral. Thus, there may be differences in etiology that lead to differences in how fixed the trait is, and how it is expressed". [69]

Machiavellianism in children

Ever since the creation of the construct in the 1960s, there has been extensive research on Machiavellianism in young children and adolescents, via a measure dubbed the "Kiddie Mach" test. [70] [71] The first study was done in 1966 as a part of Dorothea Braginsky's doctoral dissertation, with the subjects being as young as 10 years old. [70] Studies have shown that traits of Machiavellianism and other dark triad traits were already present in preschool children, [72] and more pronounced in adolescents aged 11–17. [73] There have been studies to measure Machiavellianism in 6 year olds using adult informants to analyze the child's behavior. [74] [75] Deceptive behaviors by children even as young as 3 were also investigated heavily. [76] Peer reports suggest that children higher in Machiavellianism exhibit behaviors such as using both prosocial and coercive strategies based on how much is to be gained in a situation, and they tend to manipulate indirectly. [77] Children who score highly on the Machiavellianism scale tend to be more successful in manipulation, do it more frequently, and are judged as better at manipulation than those who score lower. [78] Parental levels of Machiavellianism seem to have a slight effect on the child's own level. Machiavellianism levels in fathers was positively correlated with the Machiavellianism levels of their children, but the mother's level had no significant effect. One study concluded that "parental Machiavellianism is a predictor and perhaps a cause of children's Machiavellian beliefs and their manipulative success". [79] Machiavellianism is also correlated with childhood aggression, especially concerning the control of social hierarchies. [80] One study found a trend upwards with respect to Machiavellianism from late childhood to adolescence, when levels of Machiavellianism are thought to peak. From adolescence throughout adulthood there is a significant and steady downward trend with regard to levels of Machiavellianism, until the age of 65 where an overall lifetime minimum is reached. [81]

Peer ratings of children high on Machiavellianism are inconsistent, with some researchers reporting that juvenile High Machs are rated as popular, [82] and some reporting that they are less well liked by peers. [83] [84] A study involving Greek children ages 8–12 noted that the children highest in Machiavellianism showed aggressive tendencies and were more likely to bully others, though the tactics varied by gender. [85] [86]

Dark triad

In 1998, John McHoskey, William Worzel, and Christopher Szyarto proposed that narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy are more or less interchangeable in normal samples. [87] [88] Delroy L. Paulhus and McHoskey debated these perspectives at an American Psychological Association conference, inspiring a body of research that continues to grow in the published literature. Delroy Paulhus and Kevin Williams found enough differences between the traits to suggest that they were distinct despite their similarities, thus the concept of a "triad" of offensive personality traits was conceptualized. [89] There has been research on Machiavellianism using various dark triad measures, including the Short Dark Triad (SD3), and the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen test. [90] [91]

Miller, Lynam, and Sharpe (2022) state that the reason why the multidimensional nature of Machiavellianism has "received less attention" is because there is "little research on Machiavellianism outside the context of the D3". [92]

Psychopathy

Many psychologists consider Machiavellianism to be essentially indistinguishable from psychopathy, as they both share manipulative tendencies, disregard for morality, and cold callousness as their primary attributes. [93] [94] There is an immense and ongoing debate amongst researchers as to whether or not Machiavellianism and psychopathy should be treated as the same construct, or at least treat Machiavellianism as a subset of psychopathy. [95] When tested, High Machs scored consistently high on measures of psychopathy, more than Low Machs. Primary psychopaths also scored higher on the Machiavellianism scale than secondary psychopaths. [96] According to John McHoskey, the MACH-IV test is merely "a global measure of psychopathy in noninstitutionalized populations", and that this is a result of the disconnect between clinical and personality psychology. [88] Many have stated that the Machiavellianism scale measures nothing more than "successful" psychopathy, or psychopathy without the extreme clinical characteristics. [97] Even compared to other "dark" traits, research has shown that Psychopathy correlates with Machiavellianism far more than it correlates with narcissism. [94] [98] Some authors have stated that Machiavellianism and psychopathy represent the issue of a jangle fallacy, as both constructs are named differently yet describe the same concept. [99] A recent paper published in 2022 stated that Machiavellianism "is theoretically distinct from psychopathy, but empirically they are nearly indistinguishable". [100] Beverly Fehr even suggested that psychopaths may be simply "High Machs who have had run up against the law". [101] Robert Hare, author of the Psychopathy Checklist, stated that Machiavellianism is more closely related to PCL Factor 1, affective detachment, than to Factor 2, an antisocial lifestyle. [102] According to Christopher Patrick, high Psychopathy Checklist scores correlated with much higher Machiavellianism scores, as well as higher scores on boldness and aggressiveness. [103]

Researchers note that Machiavellianism is well represented in every measure of psychopathy, such as the Factor 1 characteristics on the Psychopathy Checklist, more specifically traits such as "conning/manipulative", "pathological lying,” "callous/lack of empathy", and “glibness/superficial charm”. [104] [105] [106] [101] Donald Lynam and others noted that Machiavellianism is also represented in the "Interpersonal Manipulation" factor in Hare's Self-Report Psychopathy Scale-III and in the "Manipulation" scale in the Elemental Psychopathy Assessment. The researchers state that "ultimately, measures of psychopathy and MACH appear to be measuring the same construct, and MACH assessments fail to capture the construct as articulated in theoretical descriptions". [104] Michael Levenson, author of the Levenson Self-Report Psychopathy Scale, disagreed with the notion of distinguishing machiavellianism from psychopathy, stating that it "does not accord" with empirical research which shows Machiavellianism being heavily related to primary psychopathy. [107] Machiavellianism has a strong relationship with both primary and secondary psychopathy on Levenson's self report test. [108] In experiments High Machs also scored highly on the Psychopathic Deviate subscale on the MMPI. [75] [109] [110] On the Psychopathic Personality Inventory, created by Scott Lilienfeld and Brian Andrews, there is even a subscale similarly named "Machiavellian Egocentricity". [111] [112] The subscale is said to assess a "ruthless and self centered willingness to exploit others". [112] [113] Machiavellianism and psychopathy have both been negatively correlated with the recognition of facial emotions, with one study stating that "if the ability to recognise facial expressions is exclusively an affective empathy task, it indeed accounts for the facial recognition deficits found in primary psychopathy and Machiavellianism." [114] High scores on the Triarchic Psychopathy Measure questionnaire strongly correlated with Machiavellianism. [115] One group of researchers noted that by definition, "the interpersonal features of psychopathy involve a calculating, cunning, and manipulative style" thus a highly psychopathic person is ipso facto also a person high on Machiavellianism. [116]

Machiavellianism and psychopathy overlap so heavily that researchers have proposed merging the two traits into each other, preferably incorporating Machiavellianism into psychopathy. [105] There have been attempts to combine Machiavellianism and psychopathy into one construct, such as the creation of a "Dark Dyad" as opposed to a "Dark Triad", with the exclusion of narcissism. [117] [118]

A recent study done on criminal psychopaths noted that Machiavellianism showed the strongest association with the affective aspect of psychopathy. [119]

Difference between constructs:Impulse control

Many other psychologists state that while Machiavellianism and psychopathy overlap heavily, there is much evidence to suggest that they are distinct personality constructs. [42] [43] [120] Psychologists who stress the differences between Machiavellianism and psychopathy state that, in total contrast to high Machs, psychopaths are impulsive, tend to be reckless, and lack long term planning skills. [48] [121] [122] Delroy Paulhus and others have stated that this difference between the two traits is often underappreciated. [91] Scholars also note that those high on Machiavellianism can delay gratification, and have more sensitivity to punishment and awareness of consequences than psychopaths. [120] Though both traits have a heritable basis, Machiavellianism is more influenced by the environment than psychopathy. [59] High Machs have been described as "master manipulators" and far better at manipulation than psychopaths and narcissists. [123]

Daniel Jones notes that even though both psychopaths and High Machs share a manipulative and callous nature, the difference between Machiavellianism and psychopathy lies in the type of manipulation employed by the two constructs. Machiavellianism is marked by calculated planning, and manipulating only when something is to be gained, while psychopathy is marked by rashness, and manipulating regardless of situation. [69] O'Boyle and others found however that the notion that Machiavellianism is marked by cautiousness does not match empirical research which shows that it can be correlated with reckless behavior in certain situations. [124]

In response to this interpretation, Miller et al (2017) responded by saying that empirical research casts "substantial doubt" on the notion of Machiavellianism being distinguishable from psychopathy, and that ultimately literature on Machiavellianism should be framed as an alternative literature on psychopathy, as they view that Machiavellianism literature is far more helpful in understanding psychopathy. [104]

Narcissism

Individuals high in Machiavellianism and narcissism both manipulate to improve their reputations, and how they appear to others. [125] [126] Individuals high in the two traits do this as a form of self aggrandizement to help their chances of success in a given situation. [127] Machiavellianism scores were positively associated with aspects of narcissism such as entitlement and exploitativeness, and inversely associated with adaptive narcissistic tendencies, like self-sufficiency. [87] Studies have also shown that those higher on Machiavellianism are more realistic about their character, while narcissists are less realistic about theirs. [128] Compared to High Machs, narcissists are less malevolent and show a more socially positive personality. They also have higher levels of self-rated happiness. [129]

Machiavellianism and narcissism both share a lack of empathy and a focus on self-interest, though the two traits differ in how they manifest in people, and what their motivations are. High Machs have tendencies to be driven by personal gain, whereas narcissists are driven by a need for validation and admiration. [130] Opposed to those high on narcissism, High Machs tend to be more strategic and calculating in their interpersonal interactions, while narcissists may be more impulsive and attention-seeking. [131]

In terms of social relationships, High Machs are more likely to be more exploitative towards others, while narcissists may be more likely to seek out relationships that serve to bolster their self-esteem and provide them with the admiration they desire. [132]

Compared to subclinical narcissists, High Machs exhibit a greater lack of empathy, because of the emotional deficiencies inherent within Machiavellianism. [133]

White collar crime

Research has shown that individuals high in Machiavellianism may be more willing to engage in white collar crimes. [134] Psychologist Daniel Jones has stated that "individuals higher on Machiavellianism are well suited for crimes in the financial world, especially crimes that skirt the legal system". [135] Delroy Paulhus has stated that Machiavellianism is the main trait for white collar criminals and con artists, and not psychopathy, stating that:

Although direct research on this topic is difficult, it seems clear that malevolent stockbrokers such as Bernie Madoff do not qualify as psychopaths: They are corporate Machiavellians who use deliberate, strategic procedures for exploiting others. A genuine psychopath, even at the subclinical level, lacks the self-control to orchestrate the schemes of a shrewd stockbroker. [136]

In a research paper, Daniel Jones and others stated that a person high on Machiavellianism would also be possibly drawn to cybercrime, noting that "although we did not directly assess crimes, we did find patterns of system infiltration that were different among the three traits, with Machiavellianism being associated with the stealthiest approach among the three traits." They also stated that Machiavellianism would only be associated with crime if the "benefits outweighed the risks". They went on to clarify that this is opposed to individuals high in psychopathy, whom are prone to crime regardless of the situation. [137] [138]

Other "dark" groupings

Machiavellianism has been featured in many other groupings of dark traits, such as the dark tetrad, which adds sadism to the dark triad traits. [139] The characteristics of Machiavellianism have also been viewed as potentially correlated with sadism. [140]

The paper titled "The Dark Core of Personality" introduced a theoretical framework to understand various "dark traits" in personality as manifestations of a single underlying factor called the Dark Factor of Personality. This factor represents a general dispositional tendency where individuals prioritize their own utility (self-interest) at the expense of others, often justifying their actions through certain beliefs. The concept of D encapsulates all of the main "dark traits", with the addition of Spitefulness, Egoism, Moral Disengagement, Entitlement, and Self-interest. [141] The authors of the study argued that while Machiavellianism, along with other dark traits like Narcissism and Psychopathy, has its unique features, it also shares a common core with these traits, which is encapsulated in the D factor. This means that people who score high in Machiavellianism tend to exhibit behaviors that prioritize their own benefit over others' which correlate with characteristis that align with the D factor. [142] It is said that manipulativeness and callousness is responsible for the covariances among the dark personalities in general. [143]

The light triad is a personality model that contrasts the dark triad, which comprises Kantianism, faith in humanity, and humanism. Kantianism was compared to Machiavellianism as its opposite, as it is characterized by a non-exploitative orientation. Both Kantianism and Machiavellianism are traits which are tongue-in-cheek references to philosophers. The "Light Triad" and its characteristics were most strongly negatively correlated with Machiavellianism. [144]

DSM: Trait, not disorder

Machiavellianism has never been considered a disorder, nor has it been referenced in any version of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders or International Classification of Diseases. [145] [146] It has been treated as strictly a personality construct. It is primarily studied by personality psychologists, as it is a non-clinical personality style. [147] [148]

Relations with other personality traits

There is immense literature concerning the relationships between Machiavellianism and other personality dimensions, such as the traits in the Big Five trait model. [94] Machiavellianism has also been related to interpersonal aggression and hostile behavior. [149]

Big Five

Mach-IV scores are negatively correlated with agreeableness (r = −0.47) and conscientiousness (r = −0.34), two dimensions of the Big Five personality model (NEO-PI-R). [121] The FFMI corrects for this by including aspects of high conscientiousness in the scale (e.g. order, deliberation). [48] Additionally, Machiavellianism correlates more highly with the honesty-humility dimension of the six-factor HEXACO model than with any of the big five dimensions. [42] Machiavellianism has also been located within the interpersonal circumplex, which consists of the two independent dimensions of agency and communion. [150] [87] [151] Agency refers to the motivation to succeed and to individuate the self, whereas communion refers to the motivation to merge with others and to support group interests. [152] Machiavellianism lies in the quadrant of the circumplex defined by high agency and low communion. [42] Machiavellianism has been found to lie diagonally opposite from a circumplex construct called self-construal, a tendency to prefer communion over agency. This suggests that people high in Machiavellianism do not simply wish to achieve, they wish to do so at the expense of (or at least without regard to) others. [42] [151]

Hot and cold empathy

There are two distinct types of empathy which people use to relate to each other which are referred to as hot and cold empathy. [153] [154] Cold empathy (or cognitive empathy) refers to the understanding of how others might react to one's actions or a certain event. [155] Hot empathy (or emotional/affective empathy) refers to the emotional reaction others might have to the emotions of another person. [156] [157] Machiavellianism was consistently negatively correlated with affective empathy in practically every study. [158] [68] [159] [160] Machiavellianism was also negatively correlated with affective resonance (feeling good when others feel good) and positively associated with affective dissonance (e.g. feeling happy when others are sad). [160] People high in Machiavellianism tend to have a better understanding of cold empathy and do not feel hot empathy which explains why they seem cold and uncaring. [161] [162] Research results have also suggested that High Machs are deficient only at the level of affective empathy (sharing of emotions), whereas their cognitive empathy is intact, even high. [163] [164] Another study suggested that high Machs are deficient at both kinds of empathy. [165] Studies also assert that high Machs do not feel guilt or remorse over the consequences of their manipulations. [166] [167] High Machs are less likely to be altruistic, and they are less likely to be concerned with the problems of others. [168] High Machs are also less emotionally expressive and have a hard time recognizing and understanding the emotional states of others. [88] One study proposed that High Machs have more automatic (that is, unconscious) recognition of other's negative emotions more than low Machs, and though they don't internalize these feelings, the understanding of those emotions may in fact aid in the manipulation of others. [169] [170] Children who scored higher on Machiavellianism showed a lack of empathy and more delinquent behaviors compared to those who scored lower, such as a lack of guilt, lying, cheating, and truancy. [102]

Some authors have stated that since Machiavellianism is so diametrically opposed to empathy, it should be included in empathy scales as the polar opposite. Total scores on Machiavellianism and empathy were "significantly negatively correlated". [171]

Unemotionality

One of the primary traits of machiavellianism is a detached, unemotional attitude and lack of affect in regards to others. [172] [173] Christie and Geis noted that the primary difference between high machs and low machs was the degree of emotion invested in interpersonal relations, with those scoring high having the lowest. [174] [46] Research has been done on the extent of the low emotionality of those who score high on the Machiavellianism scale. [133] Doris Mcllwain noted that "Machiavellians do not inhabit the realm of emotion in the same way as others, yet they use it to manipulate others. They do not experience feelings, empathy, or morality in normative ways. yet they are consummate manipulators and deceivers precisely by playing upon these sentiments and convictions in others. Thus they induce in others the guilt they hardly feel themselves." [175] A study done by Farah Ali and others noted that Machiavellianism seems to have emotional reactions to stimuli similar to primary psychopathy, differing only in higher levels of anxiety, which those who score highly on machiavellianism may be prone to. [176] [88] Machiavellianism has an unclear relation with anxiety levels, some researchers have found positive correlations, while some have found no relation at all. [177] High Machs demonstrate reduced skills in expressing their emotions. [178] [179]

A recent analysis discovered that, in addition to acting for mostly self interest and profit, High Machs used significantly less words when referring to emotional involvement. They concluded that "this study confirmed previous findings that High Machs have a cool and rational character and a pro-self orientation and showed that their lack of group orientation may account for their low cooperation in social dilemmas." [180]

Alexithymia is also considered a key trait that has correlations with Machiavellianism. [181] It is the lack of awareness of one's own emotions as well as the emotions of others. [182] When tested, healthy alexithymic individuals have been found to obtain high Machiavellianism scores. [183] [163] This was not surprising to researchers, seeing as the unemotionality of Machiavellianism shows similarities to what alexithymics experience. [12] One study examined the relationship between alexithymia and endorsement of beliefs associated with Machiavellianism amongst university students. Results showed a positive correlation between alexithymia and exploitative beliefs, suggesting those with higher levels of alexithymia were more likely to endorse the view that manipulating others is an effective strategy. [184]

Neuroticism and depression

Researchers have often debated the potential links between Machiavellianism and a neurotic, anxious disposition. [177] In a study done by psychologists Hans Eysenck, his wife Sybil, and John Allsopp, they note that they found "virtually no relationship" between Machiavellianism and neuroticism, unlike the relationships they found with Machiavellianism and Extraversion-Psychoticism respectively. [185] However John McHoskey found links between Machiavellianism and neuroticism, along with the other personality traits popularized by Eysenck. [185] Studies using measures of the Big Five personality traits have variously found positive or no correlation between Machiavellianism and Neuroticism. [177]

Machiavellianism has very scant correlations with depression, and the higher one's Machiavellianism score was, the lower their depression level was. [186] One study even noted that depressed males "were significantly less Machiavellian than were nondepressed males". They then estimate that "depression in males, then, may be of a more self-aggressive, self-destructive nature". [187] High Machs who have higher levels of emotional intelligence scores show less depressive symptoms. [188]

Motivation

A 1992 review described the motivation of those high on the Machiavellianism scale as related to cold selfishness and pure instrumentality, and those high on the trait were assumed to pursue their motives (e.g. sex, achievement, sociality) in duplicitous ways. More recent research on the motivations of high Machs compared to low Machs found that they gave high priority to money, success, and competition and relatively low priority to community building, self-love, and family commitment. High Machs admitted to focusing on unmitigated achievement and winning at any cost. [189] The research on behaviors which high Machs engage in suggest that they are willing to achieve their goals by bending and breaking rules, cheating, and stealing. [190] People high in Machiavellianism are able to easily switch between working with others to taking advantage of others to achieve their goals, and they are more willing to do things others see as terrible or immoral. [161] [191] [192]

Intelligence and other cognitive skills

Due to their skill at interpersonal manipulation, there has often been an assumption that high Machs possess superior intelligence, or ability to understand other people in social situations. Recent research provides some support for this assumption. [193] However, other research has established that Machiavellianism is unrelated to IQ. [191] Paulhus and Williams found "significant associations of psychopathy and Machiavellianism with a relatively higher nonverbal to verbal IQ score". [121]

Studies on emotional intelligence have usually found that high Machiavellianism is associated with low emotional intelligence as assessed by both performance and questionnaire measures. [194]

Research has examined the relationship between trait emotional intelligence (EI), Machiavellianism, and the personality trait of agreeableness. The findings of one paper revealed that while trait EI and the ability to manage others' emotions were negatively correlated with Machiavellianism, this relationship was mediated by agreeableness. Specifically, those high in trait EI tended to be low in Machiavellianism primarily because they were high in agreeableness, reflecting a cooperative and prosocial nature. [195] One study found that Machiavellianism was negatively associated with most facets of socio-emotional intelligence (SEI), including social expressivity, social sensitivity, emotional expressivity, and emotional sensitivity. However, Machiavellianism showed no significant relationship with emotional control, which involves regulating one's own emotional displays. Machiavellianism was positively associated with emotional manipulation, the tendency to strategically influence others' emotions. Despite this association, Machiavellianism did not moderate the relationship between any facet of SEI and emotional manipulation. This suggests that while High Machs have a propensity for emotionally manipulative behavior, they may not be utilizing socio-emotional skills towards that end. The authors propose this could be due to the High Mach's externally-oriented and unemotional perspective. [196]

Both emotional empathy and emotion recognition have been shown to have negative correlations with Machiavellianism. [47] [9] Additionally, research has shown that Machiavellianism is unrelated to a more advanced theory of mind, that is, the ability to anticipate what others are thinking in social situations. [165] However, research results have suggested the contrary viewpoint that high Machiavellianism is associated with excellent theory of mind skills. [197] [163]

When it comes to manipulation, individuals high in Machiavellianism may, according to Bereczkei, "have certain cognitive and social skills that enable them to properly adapt to the challenges of environmental circumstances". [198] They also are incredibly perceptive to the presence of others, and are able to feign altruism to enhance their reputation. [199] A recent study investigated whether Machiavellianism is associated with the production of "bullshit", which is defined as inaccurate or meaningless information intended to impress, persuade or mislead. The researchers found that the manipulative aspect of Machiavellianism (Machiavellian approach) was linked to "persuasive bullshitting", aimed at gaining desired resources. The distrustful aspect (Machiavellian avoidance) was associated with "evasive bullshitting", spreading vague information to prevent disadvantages. Those high in Machiavellian avoidance were even better at distinguishing misinformation from valuable information. [200]

Neurological studies

There have been few studies on the neural correlates of Machiavellianism. Research has shown that Machiavellianism has been correlated with changes in gray matter in the areas of the basal ganglia, left prefrontal cortex, bilaterally in the insula, and in the right hippocampus and the left parahippocampal gyrus. [201] Researcher Tamas Bereczkei stated that the manipulation skill in High Machs is associated with neural correlates that are responsible for decision making. [55] [202] He also noted that behaviors associated with Machiavellianism need to "recruit more neural resources than a honest behavior, especially when manipulators face a cooperative partner as a potential victim. Machiavellians have to inhibit the norm of reciprocity and, additionally, generate an opposite response." [203] Machiavellianism has also been linked with lesions in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. [204] Frontal dysfunction was also linked with Machiavellianism. [205] Daniel Jones also concluded that those who score high on Machiavellianism have the "neurological structure of a strategic manipulator". [206] The activation of the task-positive network (TPN) and the default mode network (DMN) have also been associated with a lack of genuine empathy, and also have been observed in Machiavellianism. [207] A recent study linked regional gray matter volume in the left superior frontal gyrus to both Machiavellianism and social aggression, which is "intentional antisocial behavior directed to damage others’ social reputations or interpersonal relationships through socially manipulative tactics". [208]

Social relationships

The effects that one's level of Machiavellianism has on a person's socialization and interpersonal relationships, such as friendships and romantic relationships, has been studied extensively. High Machs are highly likely to ingratiate themselves within social groups via compliments and conforming their opinions to those around them. [209] High Machs are more than likely to choose better quality friends, as they have a better time at guessing who is a good person and thus more pliable for manipulation. [210] Machiavellianism was also correlated with withdrawal and avoidance in romantic relationships. [211] Individuals high in all dark triad traits find it easy to end relationships, and tend to prefer short-term relationships over long term ones. [212] Studies done on courtship showed that women higher on machiavellianism tend to go on dates not for sexual reasons but for free food, a phenomenon known as a "foodie call". [213] Because a lack of empathy and affect with regards to others is one of the main features of Machiavellianism, individuals high on the trait tend to act in a utilitarian, self interested manner, prefer emotionally detached relationships, and are not concerned with the other person's needs. [214] [215] [216] High machs report lower relationship satisfaction than those lower on the scale. [217] Though there has been research on the potential "attractiveness" of the dark triad traits, [218] out of all of the traits in the dark triad, Machiavellianism was the least attractive to the opposite sex. [219] [220] [221] One of the studies concluded that "The third DT trait, Machiavellianism, was significantly negatively associated with being chosen and mate appeal for STR (short term relationships) in women." [220] Another study claimed that this was because high Machs tend to be way less extroverted than narcissists and psychopaths, and that "it is possible that individuals do not like cynical, manipulative, aggressive, remorseless, and duplicitous people such as Machiavellians and psychopaths". [221]

Like the other dark triad traits, those high on Machiavellianism have been reportedly more willing to troll or flame others on the internet, however a 2021 study found no particularly strong connection between trolling and dark personality traits. [222] [223] Machiavellianism has also been correlated with a higher than average belief in conspiracy theories. [224] Because of their emphasis on hierarchy, high scorers in Machiavellianism were also found with higher levels of prejudice, and higher social dominance orientation scores. [225]

Gender differences

Research has consistently shown that men score higher than women in Machiavellianism. [4] There is evidence to suggest that Machiavellianism is represented differently in both sexes, with men being more opportunistic, self confident, and willing to take risks, while women higher in Machiavellianism are more likely to be avoidant and have anxious features. [177] While only psychopathy was an indicator of future infidelity amongst men, both psychopathy and Machiavellianism were predictors amongst women. [226] The findings of one research paper showed that men, but not women, high in Machiavellianism were non-impulsive and high in planning, which suggests that the apparent impulsivity of Machiavellianism may be a question of gender. [227] Peter Jonason suggested that the reason why men score much higher than women in dark triad traits is because men require less emotional connection in order to get ahead in life. [133]

Cross-cultural studies

There have been many studies on how Machiavellianism is presented in people from different countries and with different cultures. Multiple studies found that in nearly all countries, men scored higher than women in Machiavellianism, and that the gender differences were notable. [228] The populations of many other countries varied from their western counterparts in their levels of dark triad traits overall, which the authors of one study attributed to sociopolitical factors and levels of economic engagement. [229] In another cross cultural study, Machiavellianism also showed associations with limited interactive or normative values. [230] A study investigated the relationship between emotion recognition and dark personality traits (including Machiavellianism) across cultures. The effects were gender and culture-dependent. Among both German males and females, Machiavellianism showed strong positive associations with emotionally manipulative tactics. [231] Some scholars noted an issue with many cross cultural studies on machiavellianism, primarily that "researchers have used measures of Machiavellianism (Mach IV) which were derived from Western concepts and which may not have similar meanings when applied to non-Western groups." [232]

Aggression and antisocial behavior

Machiavellianism has little association with the outright display of aggression. [233] Those high in Machiavellianism tend to be more aggressive to short term as opposed to long-term partners. [120] While Machiavellianism is associated with hostility, those high on the trait may mask it depending on the manipulation tactic used. [234] Machiavellianism also is associated with the tuning of aggression to the benefit long-term objectives, only engaging in antisocial behavior when the stakes are low and it proffers benefits, unlike psychopathy and narcissism. [120] [233] In a study by Delroy Paulhus and Daniel Jones, High Machs were found to refrain from cheating under risky situations, preferring to sustain their reputation for the long term than to engage in short-term financial gain. The authors then state that High Machs may cheat under high risk scenarios, but only when "ego-depleted", which then makes their behavior appear similar to those of psychopaths. [235] McHoskey found that MACH is associated with "cheating, divulging intimate sexual secrets to third parties, and both feigning love and inducing intoxication to secure sex". [236] He also suggested that Machiavellianism is correlated with an extensive focus on financial gain, and is also correlated with antisocial behaviors such as stealing, vandalism, and cheating as opposed to prosocial actions like helping others. [237]

In the workplace

Machiavellianism is also studied by organizational psychologists, especially those who study manipulative behaviors in workplace settings. [238] Workplace behaviors associated with this concept include flattery, deceit, coercion, and the abuse of others through one's position of leadership. [239] These behaviors in the workplace are ultimately done to advance personal interests. [240] [126] Research has shown that one's level of Machiavellianism can be a major factor in situations where workplace manipulation is involved because this trait can have an effect on the ability for an individual to "fit" into a highly political work environment. [241] Research has found individuals with Dark Triad traits are drawn to entrepreneurship. Certain qualities found in the Dark Triad are similar to traits needed for effective entrepreneurship, such as confidence, charisma and risk taking. [242] [243] Individuals high dark traits in general show mixed relations with workplace success, with some being successful, and some falling behind. [244]

Individuals high in machiavellianism tend to gravitate towards particular careers, especially those that require a high degree of competitiveness needed to succeed. [245] High Machs are ambitious enough to cut corners and use aggressive means if it is necessary to get ahead in their careers. [245] It was shown that those high on Machiavellianism are more drawn to academic majors like economics, law, and politics, as opposed to the "person-oriented" majors like education, nursing, and social work that were associated with lower Machiavellianism scores. [246]

High levels of Machiavellianism negatively affect subordinates' career success and well-being. [247] Individuals high in MACH found it easier to obtain leadership positions, and a better salary. [248] Machiavellianism was identified as a significant moderator in the relationship between perceptions of both adhocracy and hierarchy cultures and bullying victimization. [249]

Scale evaluation

Dimensions of the MACH scale

Although there have been a multitude of proposed factor structures, two dimensions emerge most consistently within factor-analytic research which separates the views from the behaviors in Machiavellianism. [101] Although many posit that the Mach IV scale is unable to reliably capture the two dimensions, a 10-item subset of the scale known as the "two-dimensional Mach IV" (TDM-V), reproduces the views and tactics dimensions across countries, genders, sample types, and scale category length. [45] [63] The "Views" dimension appears to capture the neurotic, narcissistic, pessimistic, and distrustful aspects of Machiavellianism, while the "Tactics" component captures the more unconscientious, self-serving, and deceitful behavioral aspects. More recently, in response to criticisms of the Mach-IV, researchers developed the Five-Factor Machiavellianism Inventory (FFMI), which attempts to include concepts (like being calculated and planful) that are not adequately captured by the Mach-IV. [48]

Construct validity and criticism

There has been debate on how valid Machiavellianism scales are in tapping the construct. [192] It is often stated by those critical of Machiavellianism scales that it does not actually measure the theoretical trait, but something identical to psychopathy and narcissism. For example, it is often stated that Machiavellianism is marked by less impulsivity and better long term thinking as opposed to psychopathy, but some empirical research shows that even High Machs can act impulsive in certain scenarios. [250] Lynam and others stated "we suggest that existing measures of Machiavellianism are functioning as proxy measures of psychopathy." [104] Psychologist John Rauthmann viewed the MACH-IV as more of a measure of cynicism, and that it doesn't really capture other qualities of Machiavellianism. [251]

Most of the research done on Machiavellianism has been done with either the Mach IV or Mach V, though the Mach V is no longer in use due to psychometric issues. [252] Many have expressed concerns with the reliability of the Mach IV scale to capture all of the features of Machiavellianism, thus many proposals have been made in favor of other Machiavellianism scales. [251] [9]

John Rauthmann and others have stated that, while the MACH-IV is "a generally reliable and valid scale", it has its shortcomings. These include the response styles of the test takers, the varying factor structures, and "insufficient content and construct validity". The researchers developed their own scale instead to study Machiavellianism multidimensionally instead of unidimensionally to prevent the construct from becoming hard to study effectively. [9] Psychologist Jason Dahling and others have created another measure of Machiavellianism, dubbed the Machiavellian Personality Scale (or MPS for short). [253]

Daniel Jones has stated that the MACH IV, despite all of its merits, is an "outdated instrument" and "is not helping the field with precision". He ultimately states that it "should be replaced by assessments that are more precise reflections of the construct." [254] Scholars note that there is a tendency to conflate the scale with Machiavelli's ideas, even though it has zero relation to the historical figure or his political works. [29] Regarding the MACH-IV, recent scholars have stated that almost all of items on the scale attributed to Machiavelli do not come from him, with only one item (item 20) corresponding somewhat to his actual writings. [29] Also, two items on the MACH-IV reference P.T. Barnum (born 1810) and voluntary euthanasia, both of which are unrelated to Machiavelli's ideas. [255]

Application of the scale in other research

In 2002, the Machiavellianism scale of Christie and Geis was applied by behavioral game theorists Anna Gunnthorsdottir, Kevin McCabe and Vernon L. Smith in their search for explanations for specific behavior in experimental games, especially individual choices which do not correspond to assumptions of material self-interest captured by the standard Nash equilibrium prediction. [39] It was found that in a trust game, those with high Mach-IV scores tended to follow Homo economicus ' equilibrium strategies while those with low Mach-IV scores tended to deviate from the equilibrium, and instead made choices that reflected widely accepted moral standards and social preferences. [256] In one study, Machiavellianism was positively associated with making economically opportunistic decisions in order to maximize overall profit. Levels of the trait were also an influence on the level of trust an individual had in regards to another person's motives. The authors go on to explain that the use of "cooperative or defecting strategies is subject to important individual differences". [257]

A study done by David Wilson and other researchers noted that while High Machs tend to defect from their groups, they are also unlikely to succeed in the long term simply by manipulating others, and that some cooperation is necessary for further success and to avoid a situation in which they are retaliated against. [258] Wilson also noted that since those who are high in Machiavellianism often come across as charming and attractive in brief social encounters, it is unclear whether they are being deceitful or just very skilled socially. The researchers had people who scored high and low on Machiavellianism write first-person stories. Other participants then evaluated these stories. The results showed that people with low Machiavellianism tended to be more cooperative, while those with high Machiavellianism were more exploitative. Based on these stories, high Machs were generally rejected as social partners, except when their manipulative skills could be used against members of other groups. [259]

See also

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Seduction</span> Enticing a person to sexual behaviour

In sexuality, seduction means enticing someone else into sexual intercourse or other sexual activity. Strategies of seduction include conversation and sexual scripts, paralingual features, non-verbal communication, and short-term behavioural strategies.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Sadistic personality disorder</span> Former personality disorder involving sadism

Sadistic personality disorder is an obsolete term for a proposed personality disorder defined by a pervasive pattern of sadistic and cruel behavior. People who fitted this diagnosis were thought to have a desire to control others and to have accomplished this through use of physical or emotional violence. The diagnosis proposal appeared in the appendix of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-III-R), however it was never put to use in clinical settings and later versions of the DSM had it removed. Among other reasons, psychiatrists believed it would be used to legally excuse sadistic behavior.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Narcissism</span> Excessive preoccupation with oneself

Narcissism is a self-centered personality style characterized as having an excessive preoccupation with oneself and one's own needs, often at the expense of others. Narcissism, rooted in Greek mythology, has evolved into a psychological concept studied extensively since the early 20th century, highlighting its relevance across various societal domains.

Psychopathy, or psychopathic personality, is a personality construct characterized by impaired empathy and remorse, in combination with traits of boldness, disinhibition, and egocentrism. These traits are often masked by superficial charm and immunity to stress, which create an outward appearance of feigned normalcy.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Psychopathy Checklist</span> Psychopathy scale

The Psychopathy Checklist or Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised, now the Psychopathy Checklist—revised (PCL-R), is a psychological assessment tool that is commonly used to assess the presence and extent of psychopathy in individuals—most often those institutionalized in the criminal justice system—and to differentiate those high in this trait from those with antisocial personality disorder, a related diagnosable disorder. It is a 20-item inventory of perceived personality traits and recorded behaviors, intended to be completed on the basis of a semi-structured interview along with a review of "collateral information" such as official records. The psychopath tends to display a constellation or combination of high narcissistic, borderline, and antisocial personality disorder traits, which includes superficial charm, charisma/attractiveness, sexual seductiveness and promiscuity, affective instability, suicidality, lack of empathy, feelings of emptiness, self-harm, and splitting. In addition, sadistic and paranoid traits are usually also present.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Dark triad</span> Offensive personality types

The dark triad is a psychological theory of personality, first published by Delroy L. Paulhus and Kevin M. Williams in 2002, that describes three notably offensive, but non-pathological personality types: Machiavellianism, sub-clinical narcissism, and sub-clinical psychopathy. Each of these personality types is called dark because each is considered to contain malevolent qualities.

Social adroitness is a personality trait measured in the Jackson Personality Inventory and the Jackson Personality Inventory-Revised. Adroitness is not explicitly measured by these tests, but rather the characteristics are measured through different scales.

In psychology, grandiosity is a sense of superiority, uniqueness, or invulnerability that is unrealistic and not based on personal capability. It may be expressed by exaggerated beliefs regarding one's abilities, the belief that few other people have anything in common with oneself, and that one can only be understood by a few, very special people. The personality trait of grandiosity is principally associated with narcissistic personality disorder (NPD), but also is a feature in the occurrence and expression of antisocial personality disorder, and the manic and hypomanic episodes of bipolar disorder.

The Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI) was developed in 1979 by Raskin and Hall, and since then, has become one of the most widely utilized personality measures for non-clinical levels of the trait narcissism. Since its initial development, the NPI has evolved from 220 items to the more commonly employed NPI-40 (1984) and NPI-16 (2006), as well as the novel NPI-1 inventory (2014). Derived from the DSM-III criteria for Narcissistic personality disorder (NPD), the NPI has been employed heavily by personality and social psychology researchers.

In psychology, manipulation is defined as an action designed to influence or control another person, usually in an underhanded or unfair manner which facilitates one's personal aims. Methods someone may use to manipulate another person may include seduction, suggestion, coercion, and blackmail to induce submission. Manipulation is generally considered a dishonest form of social influence as it is used at the expense of others. Barring mental disabilities, humans are inherently capable of manipulative and deceptive behavior, with the main differences being of specific personality characteristics or disorders.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">HEXACO model of personality structure</span> Six-dimensional model of human personality

The HEXACO model of personality structure is a six-dimensional model of human personality that was created by Ashton and Lee and explained in their book, The H Factor of Personality, based on findings from a series of lexical studies involving several European and Asian languages. The six factors, or dimensions, include honesty-humility (H), emotionality (E), extraversion (X), agreeableness (A), conscientiousness (C), and openness to experience (O). Each factor is composed of traits with characteristics indicating high and low levels of the factor. The HEXACO model was developed through similar methods as other trait taxonomies and builds on the work of Costa and McCrae and Goldberg. The model, therefore, shares several common elements with other trait models. However, the HEXACO model is unique mainly due to the addition of the honesty-humility dimension.

The Psychopathic Personality Inventory (PPI-Revised) is a personality test for traits associated with psychopathy in adults. The PPI was developed by Scott Lilienfeld and Brian Andrews to assess these traits in non-criminal populations, though it is still used in clinical populations as well. In contrast to other psychopathy measures, such as the Hare Psychopathy Checklist (PCL), the PPI is a self-report scale, rather than an interview-based assessment. It is intended to comprehensively index psychopathic personality traits without assuming particular links to anti-social or criminal behaviors. It also includes measures to detect impression management or careless responding.

The Levenson Self-Report Psychopathy scale (LSRP) is a 26-item, 4-point Likert scale, self-report inventory to measure primary and secondary psychopathy in non-institutionalized populations. It was developed in 1995 by Michael R. Levenson, Kent A. Kiehl and Cory M. Fitzpatrick. The scale was created for the purpose of conducting a psychological study examining antisocial disposition among a sample of 487 undergraduate students attending psychology classes at the University of California, Davis.

Delroy L. Paulhus is a personality psychology researcher and professor. He received his doctorate from Columbia University and has worked at the University of California, Berkeley and the University of California, Davis. Currently, Paulhus is a professor of psychology at the University of British Columbia in Vancouver, Canada where he teaches undergraduate and graduate courses. He is best known for being the co creator of the dark triad, along with fellow researcher Kevin Williams.

Machiavellianism in the workplace is a concept studied by many organizational psychologists. Conceptualized originally by Richard Christie and Florence Geis, Machiavellianism in psychology refers to a personality trait construct based on a cold, callous and exploitative orientation. It has been adapted and applied to the context of the workplace and organizations by psychology academics. Oliver James wrote on the effects of Machiavellianism and other dark triad personality traits in the workplace, the others being narcissism and psychopathy.

Everyday sadism is a personality trait characterized by the enjoyment of inflicting or witnessing pain upon other people or animals. Those who fit the characteristic are referred to as "sadists" or as "sadistic". Sadism has been associated with a lack of empathy, and with psychopathic traits.

The Dark Triad Dirty Dozen (DTDD) is a brief 12-question personality inventory test to assess the possible presence of the three subclinical dark triad traits: Machiavellianism, narcissism, and psychopathy. The DTDD was developed to identify the dark triad traits among subclinical adult populations. It is a screening test.

Richard Christie (1918–1992) was an American social and personality psychologist. He was influential in many studies on human psychology, most notably in the creation of the personality trait known as Machiavellianism.

The MACH-IV is a 20 question Likert scale psychometric designed to test levels of Machiavellianism in individuals. In personality psychology, Machiavellianism refers to a personality construct which comprises manipulativeness, deceitfulness, and a callous, calculating orientation. It is the most widely used Machiavellianism test by researchers.

Studies in Machiavellianism is a book published in 1970 by psychologists Richard Christie and Florence L. Geis. It is a collection of 38 studies into the interpersonal personality variable that they dubbed Machiavellianism. It is the first book on the subject, the first use of the word "Machiavellianism" as the name of a personality variable, and would launch an entire field of study.

References

  1. Vernon, Philip A.; Villani, Vanessa C.; Vickers, Leanne C.; Harris, Julie Aitken (January 2008). "A behavioral genetic investigation of the Dark Triad and the Big 5". Personality and Individual Differences. 44 (2): 445–452. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007.09.007.
  2. 1 2 Schermer, Julie Aitken; Jones, Daniel N. (February 2020). "The behavioral genetics of the dark triad core versus unique trait components: A pilot study". Personality and Individual Differences. 154: 109701. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2019.109701.
  3. Geis, Florence; Christie, Richard; Nelson, Carnot (1970). "In Search of the Machiavel". Studies in Machiavellianism. p. 76. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-174450-2.50010-5. ISBN   978-0-12-174450-2.
  4. 1 2 3 4 5 Jones, Daniel N.; Paulhus, Delroy L. (2009). "Machiavellianism". In Leary, Mark R.; Hoyle, Rick H. (eds.). Handbook of Individual Differences in Social Behavior. Guilford Press. pp. 93–108. ISBN   978-1-59385-647-2.
  5. In sum, the key elements of Machiavellianism appear to be (a) manipulativeness, (b) callous affect, and (c) a strategic-calculating orientation.
    • Jones, D. N., & Paulhus, D. L. (2014). Introducing the short dark triad (SD3) a brief measure of dark personality traits. Assessment, 21(1), 28-41.
  6. Machiavellianism is a trait defined by manipulative, callous and strategic dispositions.
    • Jones, D. N., & Figueredo, A. J. (2013). The core of darkness: Uncovering the heart of the Dark Triad. European Journal of Personality, 27(6), 521-531.
    "Machiavellianism is manifest by manipulative exploitation of others, a cynical disregard for laws and morals and a focus on self-interest and deception."
    • Furnham, A., & Treglown, L. (2021). The dark side of high-fliers: the dark triad, high-flier traits, engagement, and subjective success. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 647676.
  7. Christie & Geis 1970, p. 10, 339.
  8. 1 2 Colman, Andrew M. (2015). "Machiavellianism". A Dictionary of Psychology. Oxford University Press. p. 434. ISBN   978-0-19-965768-1.
  9. 1 2 3 4 Rauthmann, John F.; Will, Theresa (30 April 2011). "Proposing a Multidimensional Machiavellianism Conceptualization". Social Behavior and Personality. 39 (3): 391–403. doi:10.2224/sbp.2011.39.3.391.
  10. Cohen, A. Counterproductive Work Behaviors pg. 121
  11. Spielberger & Butcher 2013.
  12. 1 2 Geis, F. L. (1978). "Machiavellianism". In London, Harvey; Exner, John E. (eds.). Dimensions of Personality. Wiley. pp. 305–363. ISBN   978-0-471-54392-3.
  13. Paulhus, Delroy L; Williams, Kevin M (December 2002). "The Dark Triad of personality: Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy". Journal of Research in Personality. 36 (6): 556–563. doi:10.1016/S0092-6566(02)00505-6. S2CID   6535576.
  14. Lyons 2019, p. 2.
  15. Furnham, Adrian; Richards, Steven C.; Paulhus, Delroy L. (March 2013). "The Dark Triad of Personality: A 10 Year Review". Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 7 (3): 199–216. doi:10.1111/spc3.12018.
  16. Christie & Geis 1970, pp. 2–6.
  17. See the chapter "Why Machiavelli" in Studies in Machiavellianism, 1970. pages 1-9
  18. Christie, Richard (1970). "Scale Construction". Studies in Machiavellianism. Elsevier. pp. 10–34. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-174450-2.50007-5. ISBN   978-0-12-174450-2.
  19. Christie, Richard; Geis, Florence (1970b). "Implications and Speculations". Studies in Machiavellianism. pp. 339–358. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-174450-2.50022-1. ISBN   978-0-12-174450-2.
  20. "Despite such popular uses of concepts from Machiavelli, “Machiavellianism”, as a concept in psychology, was not formulated until 1970."
    • Monaghan, C. (2019). Two-Dimensional Machiavellianism: Conceptualisation, Measurement, and Well-Being.
  21. Christie & Geis 1970, p. 14.
  22. Christie & Geis 1970, p. 14, 340.
  23. Christie & Geis 1970, p. 10.
  24. "Note that the definition of Machiavellianism in psychology is simply a collection of specific traits (a person being cold, calculating, etc.). This is different from how the term has been used in, e.g., political theory."
    • Laakasuo, M., Sundvall, J. R., Berg, A., Drosinou, M., Herzon, V., Kunnari, A., ... & Palomäki, J. (2021). Moral psychology and artificial agents (Part two): The transhuman connection. In Machine law, ethics, and morality in the age of artificial intelligence (pp. 189-204). IGI Global.
  25. "...The Machiavellian personality in Mach-IV has little, if anything, to do with Machiavelli, either the man or his work."
    • Cohen, A. (2018). Counterproductive work behaviors: Understanding the dark side of personalities in organizational Life. Routledge. pg 121
  26. Quote: "It is important to note the vast conceptual divergence between “Machiavellianism” within psychology and “Machiavellianism” within political theory or philosophy"
    -Matthews, M. J., Kelemen, T. K., Matthews, S. H., & Matthews, J. M. (2022). The machiavellian organization: A multilevel model to understand decision making in organizations. Group & Organization Management, 47(2), 413-439.
  27. 1 2 Hartley, Dale (2023). Machiavellians: Gulling the Rubes. Draft2digital. p. 2. ISBN   979-8-223-34675-3. When psychologists refer to the terms, Machiavellian or Machiavellianism, they do not mean Machiavelli's political advice. They refer instead to the underlying personality traits of cynicism, duplicity and scheming by which master manipulators conceive and execute their agendas.
  28. 1 2 "Because of the reactivity association with its nomenclature, the personality orientation, Machiavellianism, has frequently been confused with a political philosophy which is in conflict with democratic principles."
  29. 1 2 3 4 Cohen, A. (2018). Counterproductive work behaviors: Understanding the dark side of personalities in organizational Life. Routledge. pg 121
  30. Pandey, J. (2022). Manipulative Social Behavior and Personality Disposition. In: Nature and Dynamics of Social Influence. Springer.
  31. Christie & Geis 1970, p. 1.
  32. Quote: "Abbreviations are not readily found; psychological jargon has already stamped a simple M as the term for movement response on the Rorschach, Ma has been preempted for the hypomania scale on the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory..."
    • Christie, R., & Merton, R. K. (1958). Procedures for the sociological study of the values climate of medical schools. Academic medicine, 33(10), 125-153.
  33. Christie, R. Geis, F. "Some Consequences with Taking Machiavelli Seriously" in Edgar F. Borgatta and William W. Lambert (eds.). Handbook of Personality Theory and Research
    • "The terms ‘‘dark personalities,’’ ‘‘narcissists,’’ ‘‘Machiavellians,’’ and ‘‘psychopaths’’ are not used as diagnostic labels, but as abbreviations only for people scoring (highly) on respective personality dimensions. Further, no pathology is implied; the terms are solely used for subclinical forms of the respective personality dimensions"----Rauthmann, J. F. (2012). The Dark Triad and interpersonal perception: Similarities and differences in the social consequences of narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3(4), 487-496.
    • Furtner, M. R., Maran, T., & Rauthmann, J. F. (2017). Dark leadership: The role of leaders’ dark triad personality traits. Leader development deconstructed, 75-99.
    • "Machiavellians—individuals who have high scores on Mach scales—display a specific emotion regulation style."----Deak, A., Bodrogi, B., Biro, B., Perlaki, G., Orsi, G., & Bereczkei, T. (2017). Machiavellian emotion regulation in a cognitive reappraisal task: An fMRI study. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 17, 528-541.
  34. 1 2 Christie & Geis 1970, p. 260.
  35. Hartley, D. "Meet the Machiavellians". Psychology Today. Archived from the original on 2017-10-19. Retrieved 2022-09-14.
  36. Christie & Geis 1970.
  37. McIlwain, Doris (2004). "Bypassing Empathy: A Machiavellian Theory of Mind and Sneaky Power". In Repacholi, Betty; Slaughter, Virginia (eds.). Individual Differences in Theory of Mind. Psychology Press. pp. 50–77. doi:10.4324/9780203488508-7. ISBN   978-0-203-48850-8.
  38. 1 2 Gunnthorsdottir, Anna; McCabe, Kevin; Smith, Vernon (2002). "Using the Machiavellianism instrument to predict trustworthiness in a bargaining game". Journal of Economic Psychology. 23 (1): 49–66. doi:10.1016/S0167-4870(01)00067-8.
  39. Muris, Peter; Merckelbach, Harald; Otgaar, Henry; Meijer, Ewout (March 2017). "The Malevolent Side of Human Nature: A Meta-Analysis and Critical Review of the Literature on the Dark Triad (Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and Psychopathy)". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 12 (2): 183–204. doi:10.1177/1745691616666070. PMID   28346115. S2CID   24809359.
  40. Christie & Geis 1970, pp. 6.
  41. 1 2 3 4 5 Jones, Daniel N.; Paulhus, Delroy L. (2009). "Chapter 7. Machiavellianism". In Leary, Mark R.; Hoyle, Rick H (eds.). Handbook of Individual Differences in Social Behavior . New York/London: The Guilford Press. pp.  257–273. ISBN   978-1-59385-647-2.
  42. 1 2 Paulhus, Delroy L.; Jones, Daniel N. (2015). "Measures of Dark Personalities". Measures of Personality and Social Psychological Constructs. pp. 562–594. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-386915-9.00020-6. ISBN   978-0-12-386915-9.
    • "Machiavellianism is defined as a quantitative personality trait, implying that individuals might score higher or lower on the continuum of Machiavellianism." ---Belschak, F. D., Jacobs, G., Giessner, S. R., Horton, K. E., & Bayerl, P. S. (2020). When the going gets tough: Employee reactions to large‐scale organizational change and the role of employee Machiavellianism. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 41(9), 830-850.
    • Hutter, Katja; Füller, Johann; Hautz, Julia; Bilgram, Volker; Matzler, Kurt (3 July 2015). "Machiavellianism or Morality: Which Behavior Pays Off In Online Innovation Contests?". Journal of Management Information Systems. 32 (3): 197–228. doi:10.1080/07421222.2015.1099181.
  43. 1 2 Monaghan, Conal; Bizumic, Boris; Sellbom, Martin (May 2016). "The role of Machiavellian views and tactics in psychopathology". Personality and Individual Differences. 94: 72–81. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2016.01.002.
  44. 1 2 Christie & Geis 1970, p. 312.
  45. 1 2 3 Christie & Geis 1970, p. 3.
  46. 1 2 3 4 Collison, Katherine L.; Vize, Colin E.; Miller, Joshua D.; Lynam, Donald R. (October 2018). "Development and preliminary validation of a five factor model measure of Machiavellianism". Psychological Assessment. 30 (10): 1401–1407. doi:10.1037/pas0000637. PMID   30047746.
  47. Petrides, K. V.; Vernon, Philip A.; Schermer, Julie Aitken; Veselka, Livia (February 2011). "Trait Emotional Intelligence and the Dark Triad Traits of Personality". Twin Research and Human Genetics. 14 (1): 35–41. doi:10.1375/twin.14.1.35. PMID   21314254.
  48. Vernon, Philip A.; Villani, Vanessa C.; Vickers, Leanne C.; Harris, Julie Aitken (2008). "A behavioral genetic investigation of the Dark Triad and the Big 5". Personality and Individual Differences. 44 (2): 445–452. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007.09.007.
  49. Furnham, Adrian; Richards, Steven C.; Paulhus, Delroy L. (March 2013). "The Dark Triad of Personality: A 10 Year Review". Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 7 (3): 199–216. doi:10.1111/spc3.12018. S2CID   32281294.
  50. Veselka, Livia; Schermer, Julie Aitken; Vernon, Philip A. (April 2011). "Beyond the Big Five: The Dark Triad and the Supernumerary Personality Inventory". Twin Research and Human Genetics. 14 (2): 158–168. doi:10.1375/twin.14.2.158. PMID   21425898.
    • Monaghan, Conal; Bizumic, Boris; Sellbom, Martin (August 2018). "Nomological network of two-dimensional Machiavellianism". Personality and Individual Differences. 130: 161–173. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2018.03.047. S2CID 150165701
    • Petrides, K. V.; Vernon, Philip A.; Schermer, Julie Aitken; Veselka, Livia (February 2011). "Trait Emotional Intelligence and the Dark Triad Traits of Personality". Twin Research and Human Genetics. 14 (1): 35–41. doi:10.1375/twin.14.1.35. PMID 21314254.
    • "What Makes Someone a Master Manipulator? | Psychology Today". www.psychologytoday.com. Retrieved 2023-06-24.
  51. Cairncross, Molly; Veselka, Livia; Schermer, Julie Aitken; Vernon, Philip A. (June 2013). "A Behavioral Genetic Analysis of Alexithymia and the Dark Triad Traits of Personality". Twin Research and Human Genetics. 16 (3): 690–697. doi:10.1017/thg.2013.19. PMID   23561050. S2CID   40361747.
  52. 1 2 Bereczkei, Tamas (October 2015). "The manipulative skill: Cognitive devices and their neural correlates underlying Machiavellian's decision making". Brain and Cognition. 99: 24–31. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2015.06.007. PMID   26189112.
  53. Onley, M., Veselka, L., Schermer, J. A., & Vernon, P. A. (2013). Survival of the scheming: a genetically informed link between the dark triad and mental toughness. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 16(6), 1087-1095.
  54. Montag, Christian; Hall, Jeremy; Plieger, Thomas; Felten, Andrea; Markett, Sebastian; Melchers, Martin; Reuter, Martin (March 2015). "The DRD3 Ser9Gly polymorphism, Machiavellianism, and its link to schizotypal personality". Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics. 8 (1): 48–57. doi:10.1037/npe0000034.
  55. Bereczkei, Tamás (2017). Machiavellianism: The Psychology of Manipulation. Routledge. p. 92. ISBN   978-1-351-60558-8.
  56. 1 2 Siwy-Hudowska, Anna; Pilch, Irena (2014). "Machiavellianism in families: analysis of relationships between the machaiavellanism level of grown-up children and their parents". Studia Psychologiczne. 52 (3): 16–23.
  57. Johnson, Megan M.; Caron, Kelly M.; Mikolajewski, Amy J.; Shirtcliff, Elizabeth A.; Eckel, Lisa A.; Taylor, Jeanette (September 2014). "Psychopathic Traits, Empathy, and Aggression are Differentially Related to Cortisol Awakening Response". Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 36 (3): 380–388. doi:10.1007/s10862-014-9412-7.
  58. Bartels, Meike; De Geus, Eco J. C.; Kirschbaum, Clemens; Sluyter, Frans; Boomsma, Dorret I. (2003). "Heritability of Daytime Cortisol Levels in Children". Behavior Genetics. 33 (4): 421–433. doi:10.1023/A:1025321609994. PMID   14574141.
  59. Wüst, Stefan; Federenko, Ilona; Hellhammer, Dirk H; Kirschbaum, C (October 2000). "Genetic factors, perceived chronic stress, and the free cortisol response to awakening". Psychoneuroendocrinology. 25 (7): 707–720. doi:10.1016/S0306-4530(00)00021-4. PMID   10938450.
  60. 1 2 Monaghan, Conal; Bizumic, Boris; Sellbom, Martin (August 2018). "Nomological network of two-dimensional Machiavellianism". Personality and Individual Differences. 130: 161–173. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2018.03.047. S2CID   150165701.
  61. 1 2 Láng, András; Lénárd, Kata (April 2015). "The relation between memories of childhood psychological maltreatment and Machiavellianism". Personality and Individual Differences. 77: 81–85. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2014.12.054.
  62. This claim was made by
  63. Láng, András (December 2015). "Machiavellianism and early maladaptive schemas in adolescents". Personality and Individual Differences. 87: 162–165. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2015.07.039.
  64. Abell, Loren; Lyons, Minna; Brewer, Gayle (2014). "The relationship between parental bonding, Machiavellianism and adult friendship quality". Individual Differences Research. 12 (4–B): 191–197.
  65. 1 2 3 Massey-Abernathy, Amber; Byrd-Craven, Jennifer (September 2016). "Seeing but Not Feeling: Machiavellian Traits in Relation to Physiological Empathetic Responding and Life Experiences". Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology. 2 (3): 252–266. doi:10.1007/s40750-016-0041-0.
  66. 1 2 Jones, D. (2020). Machiavellianism. In: Zeigler-Hill, V., Shackelford, T.K. (eds) Encyclopedia of Personality and Individual Differences. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-24612-3_1245
  67. 1 2 Christie & Geis 1970, p. 331.
  68. Chabrol, Henri; Van Leeuwen, Nikki; Rodgers, Rachel; Séjourné, Natalène (November 2009). "Contributions of psychopathic, narcissistic, Machiavellian, and sadistic personality traits to juvenile delinquency". Personality and Individual Differences. 47 (7): 734–739. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.06.020.
  69. Slaughter, V. (2011). Early adoption of Machiavellian attitudes: Implications for children’s interpersonal relationships. In C. T. Barry, P. K. Kerig, K. K. Stellwagen, & T. D. Barry (Eds.), Narcissism and Machiavellianism in youth: Implications for the development of adaptive and maladaptive behavior (pp. 177–192). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/12352-010
  70. Lau, Katherine S. L.; Marsee, Monica A. (April 2013). "Exploring Narcissism, Psychopathy, and Machiavellianism in Youth: Examination of Associations with Antisocial Behavior and Aggression". Journal of Child and Family Studies. 22 (3): 355–367. doi:10.1007/s10826-012-9586-0.
  71. Repacholi, B.; Slaughter, V.; Pritchard, M.; Gibbs, V. (2004). "Theory of mind, Machiavellianism, and social functioning in childhood". Individual differences in theory of mind. Psychology Press. pp. 68–98. ISBN   9780203488508.
  72. 1 2 Mealey, Linda; Kinner, Stuart (2003-03-14). "Psychopathy, Machiavellianism and Theory of Mind". The Social Brain. Wiley. pp. 355–372. doi:10.1002/0470867221.ch16. ISBN   978-0-470-84960-6.
  73. Abell, Loren; Qualter, Pamela; Brewer, Gayle; Barlow, Alexandra; Stylianou, Maria; Henzi, Peter; Barrett, Louise (20 August 2015). "Why Machiavellianism Matters in Childhood: The Relationship Between Children's Machiavellian Traits and Their Peer Interactions in a Natural Setting". Europe's Journal of Psychology. 11 (3): 484–493. doi:10.5964/ejop.v11i3.957. PMC   4873058 . PMID   27247672.
  74. Braginsky, Dorothea D. (January 1970). "Machiavellianism and manipulative interpersonal behavior in children". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 6 (1): 77–99. doi:10.1016/0022-1031(70)90077-6.
  75. Kraut, Robert E.; Price, J. Douglas (1976). "Machiavellianism in parents and their children". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 33 (6): 782–786. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.33.6.782. PMID   1271237.
  76. Kerig, Patricia K.; Stellwagen, Kurt K. (September 2010). "Roles of Callous-Unemotional Traits, Narcissism, and Machiavellianism in Childhood Aggression". Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 32 (3): 343–352. doi:10.1007/s10862-009-9168-7.
  77. Götz, Friedrich M.; Bleidorn, Wiebke; Rentfrow, Peter J. (October 2020). "Age differences in Machiavellianism across the life span: Evidence from a large-scale cross-sectional study". Journal of Personality. 88 (5): 978–992. doi:10.1111/jopy.12545. PMID   32145085.
  78. Hawley, Patricia H. (2003). "Prosocial and Coercive Configurations of Resource Control in Early Adolescence: A Case for the Well-Adapted Machiavellian". Merrill-Palmer Quarterly. 49 (3): 279–309. doi:10.1353/mpq.2003.0013. hdl: 1808/15796 .
  79. Palmen, J. M. H. (15 June 2009). Friendship and Aggression in Elementary School. The friendships of aggressive children and the effects of having aggressive friends (Thesis). hdl:1874/34096.
  80. Abell, Loren; Qualter, Pamela; Brewer, Gayle; Barlow, Alexandra; Stylianou, Maria; Henzi, Peter; Barrett, Louise (20 August 2015). "Why Machiavellianism Matters in Childhood: The Relationship Between Children's Machiavellian Traits and Their Peer Interactions in a Natural Setting". Europe's Journal of Psychology. 11 (3): 484–493. doi:10.5964/ejop.v11i3.957. PMC   4873058 . PMID   27247672.
  81. Andreou, Eleni (2000). "Bully/victim problems and their association with psychological constructs in 8- to 12-year-old Greek schoolchildren". Aggressive Behavior. 26 (1): 49–56. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-2337(2000)26:1<49::AID-AB4>3.0.CO;2-M. ISSN   0096-140X.
  82. Andreou, Eleni (2004). "Bully/victim problems and their association with Machiavellianism and self-efficacy in Greek primary school children". British Journal of Educational Psychology. 74 (2): 297–309. doi:10.1348/000709904773839897. ISSN   0007-0998. PMID   15130193.
  83. 1 2 3 McHoskey, John (December 1995). "Narcissism and Machiavellianism". Psychological Reports. 77 (3): 755–759. doi:10.2466/pr0.1995.77.3.755. PMID   8559913.
  84. 1 2 3 4 McHoskey, John W.; Worzel, William; Szyarto, Christopher (1998). "Machiavellianism and psychopathy". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 74 (1): 192–210. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.192. PMID   9457782.
  85. "The Light Triad vs. Dark Triad of Personality".
  86. Jonason, Peter K.; Webster, Gregory D. (June 2010). "The dirty dozen: A concise measure of the dark triad". Psychological Assessment. 22 (2): 420–432. doi:10.1037/a0019265. PMID   20528068.
  87. 1 2 Jones, Daniel N.; Paulhus, Delroy L. (February 2014). "Introducing the Short Dark Triad (SD3): A Brief Measure of Dark Personality Traits". Assessment. 21 (1): 28–41. doi:10.1177/1073191113514105. PMID   24322012.
  88. Miller, J. D., Sharpe, B. M., & Lynam, D. R. (2022). Controversies Surrounding the Dark Triad. Shining light on the dark side of personality: Measurement properties and theoretical advances, 284.
  89. As this is a recurring theme in the Machiavellianism literature, this list is by no means exhaustive:
    • McHoskey, Worzel & Szyarto 1998
    • Miller, Joshua D.; Hyatt, Courtland S.; Maples-Keller, Jessica L.; Carter, Nathan T.; Lynam, Donald R. (2017). "Psychopathy and Machiavellianism: A Distinction Without a Difference?". Journal of Personality. 85 (4): 439–453. doi:10.1111/jopy.12251. PMID   26971566.
    • Lyons, Minna (2019b). The Dark Triad of Personality: Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and Psychopathy in Everyday Life. Academic Press. p. 44. ISBN   978-0-12-814292-9.
    • Kowalski, C. M., Rogoza, R., Saklofske, D. H., & Schermer, J. A. (2021). Dark triads, tetrads, tents, and cores: Why navigate (research) the jungle of dark personality models without a compass (criterion)?. Acta Psychologica, 221, 103455.
    • Miller, Joshua D.; Vize, Colin; Crowe, Michael L.; Lynam, Donald R. (August 2019). "A Critical Appraisal of the Dark-Triad Literature and Suggestions for Moving Forward". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 28 (4): 353–360. doi:10.1177/0963721419838233. S2CID   155545945.
    • Smith, Robert J. (August 1999). "Psychopathic behavior and issues of treatment". New Ideas in Psychology. 17 (2): 165–176. doi:10.1016/S0732-118X(99)00018-5.
    • Persson, Björn N. (May 2019). "Searching for Machiavelli but finding psychopathy and narcissism". Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment. 10 (3): 235–245. doi:10.1037/per0000323. PMID   30628801.
    • Sharpe et al. 2021
    • Kavish, Nicholas; Jones, Michelle A.; Rock, Rachel C.; Johnson, Alexandria K.; Anderson, Jaime L. (June 2019). "On the Overlap between Psychopathic Traits and Machiavellianism in a Forensic Population". Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 41 (2): 198–207. doi:10.1007/s10862-018-9708-0.
    • Glenn & Sellbom 2015
    • DeLisi, M., Pechorro, P., Maroco, J., & Simões, M. (2021). Overlapping measures or constructs? An empirical study of the overlap between self-control, psychopathy, Machiavellianism and narcissism. Forensic science international: synergy, 3, 100141.
    • DeShong, Hilary L., Ashley C. Helle, Gregory J. Lengel, Neil Meyer, and Stephanie N. Mullins-Sweatt. "Facets of the dark triad: Utilizing the five-factor model to describe Machiavellianism." Personality and Individual Differences 105 (2017): 218-223.
  90. 1 2 3 Lee, Kibeom; Ashton, Michael C. (May 2005). "Psychopathy, Machiavellianism, and Narcissism in the Five-Factor Model and the HEXACO model of personality structure". Personality and Individual Differences. 38 (7): 1571–1582. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2004.09.016.
    • Glenn, Andrea L.; Sellbom, Martin (June 2015). "Theoretical and Empirical Concerns Regarding the Dark Triad as a Construct". Journal of Personality Disorders. 29 (3): 360–377. doi:10.1521/pedi_2014_28_162. PMID   25248015.
    • Rauthmann, John F.; Kolar, Gerald P. (November 2012). "How 'dark' are the Dark Triad traits? Examining the perceived darkness of narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy". Personality and Individual Differences. 53 (7): 884–889. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2012.06.020.
    • Miller, Joshua D.; Lynam, Donald R. (December 2015). "Psychopathy and Personality: Advances and Debates". Journal of Personality. 83 (6): 585–592. doi:10.1111/jopy.12145. PMID   25329442.
    • Kowalski, C. M., Rogoza, R., Saklofske, D. H., & Schermer, J. A. (2021). Dark triads, tetrads, tents, and cores: Why navigate (research) the jungle of dark personality models without a compass (criterion)?. Acta Psychologica, 221, 103455.
    • Muris et al. 2017 , p. 188
  91. Skinner, Nicholas F. (1988). "Personality correlates of Machiavellianism: VI. Machiavellianism and the psychopath". Social Behavior and Personality. 16 (1): 33–37. doi:10.2224/sbp.1988.16.1.33.
  92. Stellwagen, Kurt K. (2011). "Psychopathy, narcissism, and Machiavellianism: Distinct yet intertwining personality constructs". In Stellwagen, Kurt K. (ed.). Narcissism and Machiavellianism in youth: Implications for the development of adaptive and maladaptive behavior. Washington: American Psychological Association. pp. 25–45. doi:10.1037/12352-002. ISBN   978-1-4338-0845-6.
  93. Lyons 2019, p. 4.
  94. Kowalski, Christopher Marcin; Vernon, Philip A.; Schermer, Julie Aitken (2021). "The Dark Triad and facets of personality". Current Psychology. 40 (11): 5547–5558. doi:10.1007/s12144-019-00518-0. ISSN   1046-1310.
  95. Kay, Cameron S.; Arrow, Holly (2022). "Taking an elemental approach to the conceptualization and measurement of Machiavellianism, narcissism, and psychopathy". Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 16 (4). doi:10.1111/spc3.12662. ISSN   1751-9004.
  96. 1 2 3 Fehr, Beverley; Samsom, Deborah; Paulhus, Delroy L. (2013). "The Construct of Machiavellianism: Twenty Years Later". In Spielberger, Charles D.; Butcher, James N. (eds.). Advances in Personality Assessment. pp. 77–116. doi:10.4324/9781315827483. ISBN   978-1-315-82748-3.
  97. 1 2 Loftus, S. T., & Glenwick, D. S. (2001). Machiavellianism and empathy in an adolescent residential psychiatric population. Residential Treatment for Children & Youth, 19(2), 39-57.
  98. Patrick, C. J., Fowles, D. C., & Krueger, R. F. (2009). Triarchic conceptualization of psychopathy: Developmental origins of disinhibition, boldness, and meanness. Development and psychopathology, 21(3), 913-938.
  99. 1 2 3 4 Miller, Joshua D.; Hyatt, Courtland S.; Maples-Keller, Jessica L.; Carter, Nathan T.; Lynam, Donald R. (August 2017). "Psychopathy and Machiavellianism: A Distinction Without a Difference?". Journal of Personality. 85 (4): 439–453. doi:10.1111/jopy.12251. PMID   26971566.
  100. 1 2 Glenn, A. L., & Sellbom, M. (2015). Theoretical and empirical concerns regarding the dark triad as a construct. Journal of personality disorders, 29(3), 360-377.
    • McIlwain, Doris; Evans, Jess; Caldis, Eleni; Cicchini, Fred; Aronstan, Avi; Wright, Adam; Taylor, Alan (2012). "Strange Moralities: Vicarious Emotion and Moral Emotions in Machiavellian and Psychopathic Personality Styles". In Langdon, Robyn; MacKenzie, Catriona (eds.). Emotions, Imagination, and Moral Reasoning. pp. 119–148. doi:10.4324/9780203803134. ISBN   978-1-136-63165-8.
    • Skeem, Jennifer L.; Polaschek, Devon L. L.; Patrick, Christopher J.; Lilienfeld, Scott O. (December 2011). "Psychopathic Personality: Bridging the Gap Between Scientific Evidence and Public Policy". Psychological Science in the Public Interest. 12 (3): 95–162. doi:10.1177/1529100611426706. PMID   26167886.
  101. Levenson, Michael R. (May 1993). "Psychopaths are Not Necessarily Impulsive, etc.: A Reply to Feelgood and Rantzen". Theory & Psychology. 3 (2): 229–234. doi:10.1177/0959354393032007.
  102. Lilienfeld, S. O. (1998). Methodological advances and developments in the assessment of psychopathy. Behaviour research and therapy, 36(1), 99-125.
  103. Ray, J. J.; Ray, J. A. B. (1982). "Some Apparent Advantages of Subclinical Psychopathy". The Journal of Social Psychology. 117 (1): 135–142. doi:10.1080/00224545.1982.9713415. ISSN   0022-4545. PMID   7109561.
  104. Skinner, Nicholas F. (1982-01-01). "Personality Correlates of Machiavellianism: Iv. Machiavellianism and Psychopathology". Social Behavior and Personality. 10 (2): 201–203. doi:10.2224/sbp.1982.10.2.201. ISSN   0301-2212.
    • Lilienfeld, Scott O.; Andrews, Brian P. (1996). "Development and Preliminary Validation of a Self-Report Measure of Psychopathic Personality Traits in Noncriminal Population". Journal of Personality Assessment. 66 (3): 488–524. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa6603_3. ISSN   0022-3891. PMID   8667144. Factor 1, Machiavellian Egocentricity, contains elements of Christie and Geis' (1969) Machiavellianism construct and appears similar to the construct of "Ruthless Practicality" identified by Hundleby and Ross (1977) in their factor analysis of psychopathy-related measures.
    • Kastner, Rebecca M.; Sellbom, Martin; Lilienfeld, Scott O. (March 2012). "A comparison of the psychometric properties of the psychopathic personality inventory full-length and short-form versions". Psychological Assessment. 24 (1): 261–267. doi:10.1037/a0025832. PMID   22004537.
    • Benning, Stephen D.; Patrick, Christopher J.; Hicks, Brian M.; Blonigen, Daniel M.; Krueger, Robert F. (2003). "Factor Structure of the Psychopathic Personality Inventory: Validity and Implications for Clinical Assessment". Psychological Assessment. 15 (3): 340–350. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.15.3.340. PMID   14593834.
  105. 1 2 Poythress, Norman G.; Edens, John F.; Lilienfeld, Scott O. (December 1998). "Criterion-related validity of the Psychopathic Personality Inventory in a prison sample". Psychological Assessment. 10 (4): 426–430. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.10.4.426.
  106. Patrick, Christopher J. (2019-06-14). Handbook of Psychopathy. Guilford Publications. ISBN   978-1-4625-4123-2.
  107. Wai, Michael; Tiliopoulos, Niko (May 2012). "The affective and cognitive empathic nature of the dark triad of personality". Personality and Individual Differences. 52 (7): 794–799. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2012.01.008.
  108. Sica, C., Drislane, L., Caudek, C., Angrilli, A., Bottesi, G., Cerea, S., & Ghisi, M. (2015). A test of the construct validity of the Triarchic Psychopathy Measure in an Italian community sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 82, 163-168.
  109. DeLisi, M., Pechorro, P., Maroco, J., & Simões, M. (2021). Overlapping measures or constructs? An empirical study of the overlap between self-control, psychopathy, Machiavellianism and narcissism. Forensic science international: synergy, 3, 100141.
  110. Rogoza, Radosław; Cieciuch, Jan (2020). "Dark Triad traits and their structure: An empirical approach". Current Psychology. 39 (4): 1287–1302. doi: 10.1007/s12144-018-9834-6 . ISSN   1046-1310.
  111. Egan, Vincent; Chan, Stephanie; Shorter, Gillian W. (2014). "The Dark Triad, happiness and subjective well-being". Personality and Individual Differences. 67. Elsevier BV: 17–22. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2014.01.004. ISSN   0191-8869.
  112. Kavish, Nicholas; Jones, Michelle A.; Rock, Rachel C.; Johnson, Alexandria K.; Anderson, Jaime L. (June 2019). "On the Overlap between Psychopathic Traits and Machiavellianism in a Forensic Population". Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 41 (2): 198–207. doi:10.1007/s10862-018-9708-0.
  113. 1 2 3 4 Jones, Daniel N.; Mueller, Steven M. (March 2022). "Is Machiavellianism Dead or Dormant? The Perils of Researching a Secretive Construct". Journal of Business Ethics. 176 (3): 535–549. doi:10.1007/s10551-020-04708-w.
  114. 1 2 3 Paulhus, Delroy L.; Williams, Kevin M. (2002). "The Dark Triad of personality: Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy". Journal of Research in Personality. 36 (6): 556–563. doi:10.1016/S0092-6566(02)00505-6. S2CID   6535576.
  115. Bereczkei, Tamás (2017). "Dark Triad". Machiavellianism. Routledge. pp. 44–52. doi:10.4324/9781315106922-4. ISBN   978-1-315-10692-2.
  116. O’Boyle, E. H., Forsyth, D. R., Banks, G. C., Story, P. A., & White, C. D. (2015). A meta-analytic test of redundancy and relative importance of the Dark Triad and five-factor model of personality. Journal of Personality, 83, 644–664.
  117. Rauthmann, John F. (September 2011). "Acquisitive or protective self-presentation of dark personalities? Associations among the Dark Triad and self-monitoring". Personality and Individual Differences. 51 (4): 502–508. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.05.008.
  118. 1 2 Lyons, Minna (2019b). "The Dark Triad in the Workplace". The Dark Triad of Personality. Elsevier. pp. 137–160. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-814291-2.00006-1. ISBN   978-0-12-814291-2.
  119. Tracy, Jessica L.; Cheng, Joey T.; Robins, Richard W.; Trzesniewski, Kali H. (April 2009). "Authentic and Hubristic Pride: The Affective Core of Self-esteem and Narcissism". Self and Identity. 8 (2–3): 196–213. doi:10.1080/15298860802505053. S2CID   18447793.
  120. Paulhus, Delroy L.; Williams, Kevin; Harms, Peter (2001). Shedding light on the dark triad of personality: Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy (PDF). Annual meeting of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology. San Antonio, TX.
  121. Egan, Vincent; Chan, Stephanie; Shorter, Gillian W. (September 2014). "The Dark Triad, happiness and subjective well-being". Personality and Individual Differences. 67: 17–22. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2014.01.004.
  122. Zitek, Emily M.; Jordan, Alexander H. (March 2019). "Psychological Entitlement Predicts Failure to Follow Instructions". Social Psychological and Personality Science. 10 (2): 172–180. doi:10.1177/1948550617729885.
  123. Jones, Daniel N.; Paulhus, Delroy L. (October 2011). "The role of impulsivity in the Dark Triad of personality". Personality and Individual Differences. 51 (5): 679–682. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.04.011.
  124. Czarna, Anna Z.; Jonason, Peter K.; Dufner, Michael; Kossowska, Małgorzata (30 March 2016). "The Dirty Dozen Scale: Validation of a Polish Version and Extension of the Nomological Net". Frontiers in Psychology. 7: 445. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00445 . PMC   4811972 . PMID   27065915.
  125. 1 2 3 Jonason, Peter K.; Krause, Laura (September 2013). "The emotional deficits associated with the Dark Triad traits: Cognitive empathy, affective empathy, and alexithymia". Personality and Individual Differences. 55 (5): 532–537. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2013.04.027.
  126. Jones, Daniel N. (2016). "The nature of Machiavellianism: Distinct patterns of misbehavior". The dark side of personality: Science and practice in social, personality, and clinical psychology. Washington: American Psychological Association. pp. 87–107. doi:10.1037/14854-005. ISBN   978-1-4338-2187-5.
  127. Paulhus, Delroy L. (December 2014). "Toward a Taxonomy of Dark Personalities". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 23 (6): 421–426. doi:10.1177/0963721414547737. S2CID   44987860.
  128. Jones, Daniel N.; Padilla, Edgar; Curtis, Shelby R.; Kiekintveld, Christopher (September 2021). "Network discovery and scanning strategies and the Dark Triad". Computers in Human Behavior. 122: 106799. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2021.106799 .
  129. Hare, Robert D. (March 1996). "Psychopathy: A Clinical Construct Whose Time Has Come". Criminal Justice and Behavior. 23 (1): 25–54. doi:10.1177/0093854896023001004.
  130. Međedović, Janko, and Boban Petrović. "The dark tetrad." Journal of Individual Differences (2015).
  131. "Finally, sadism is potentially more similar to psychopathy and Machiavellianism, than narcissism."
    • Bonfá-Araujo, B., Lima-Costa, A. R., Hauck-Filho, N., & Jonason, P. K. (2022). Considering sadism in the shadow of the Dark Triad traits: A meta-analytic review of the Dark Tetrad. Personality and Individual Differences, 197, 111767.
  132. Moshagen, M., Zettler, I., & Hilbig, B. E. (2020). Measuring the dark core of personality. Psychological Assessment, 32(2), 182.
  133. Moshagen, M., Hilbig, B. E., & Zettler, I. (2018). The dark core of personality. Psychological review, 125(5), 656.
  134. Jones, D. N., & Figueredo, A. J. (2013). The core of darkness: Uncovering the heart of the Dark Triad. European Journal of Personality, 27(6), 521-531.
  135. Kaufman, Scott Barry; Yaden, David Bryce; Hyde, Elizabeth; Tsukayama, Eli (2019-03-12). "The Light vs. Dark Triad of Personality: Contrasting Two Very Different Profiles of Human Nature". Frontiers in Psychology. 10: 467. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00467 . ISSN   1664-1078. PMC   6423069 . PMID   30914993.
  136. "Psychiatry.org - DSM History".
  137. "Machiavellianism | Definition, Politics, Psychology, & Facts | Britannica".
  138. "MACH has historically been the province of personality and social psychology"
    – McHoskey, J. W., Worzel, W., & Szyarto, C. (1998). Machiavellianism and psychopathy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(1), 192–210.
  139. "Machiavellianism is the only one of the D3 that does not have its origin in clinical psychology."----- Harms, P. D., Landay, K., & Fezzey, T. (2024). Explorations of the Shadow Realm. The Oxford Handbook of Individual Differences in Organizational Contexts, 122.
  140. Jones, Daniel N.; Neria, Adon L. (November 2015). "The Dark Triad and dispositional aggression". Personality and Individual Differences. 86: 360–364. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2015.06.021.
  141. Rauthmann, John F.; Kolar, Gerald P. (April 2013). "Positioning the Dark Triad in the interpersonal circumplex: The friendly-dominant narcissist, hostile-submissive Machiavellian, and hostile-dominant psychopath?". Personality and Individual Differences. 54 (5): 622–627. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2012.11.021.
  142. 1 2 Jones, Daniel N.; Paulhus, Delroy L. (2010). "Differentiating the Dark Triad within the Interpersonal Circumplex". Handbook of Interpersonal Psychology. pp. 249–267. doi:10.1002/9781118001868.ch15. ISBN   978-0-470-47160-9.
  143. Gurtman, Michael B. (July 2009). "Exploring Personality with the Interpersonal Circumplex". Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 3 (4): 601–619. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2009.00172.x.
  144. Lyons 2019, p. 16.
  145. Arefi, Mozhgan (2010). "Present of a causal model for social function based on theory of mind with mediating of Machiavellian beliefs and hot empathy". Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences. 5: 694–697. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2010.07.167 .
  146. Ang, Rebecca P.; Goh, Dion H. (August 2010). "Cyberbullying Among Adolescents: The Role of Affective and Cognitive Empathy, and Gender". Child Psychiatry & Human Development. 41 (4): 387–397. doi:10.1007/s10578-010-0176-3. PMID   20238160.
  147. Cox, Christine L.; Uddin, Lucina Q.; Di Martino, Adriana; Castellanos, F. Xavier; Milham, Michael P.; Kelly, Clare (August 2012). "The balance between feeling and knowing: affective and cognitive empathy are reflected in the brain's intrinsic functional dynamics". Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 7 (6): 727–737. doi:10.1093/scan/nsr051. PMC   3427869 . PMID   21896497.
  148. Thompson, N. M.; Van Reekum, C. M.; Chakrabarti, B. (2021). "Cognitive and Affective Empathy Relate Differentially to Emotion Regulation". Affective Science. 3 (1): 118–134. doi:10.1007/s42761-021-00062-w. PMC   8989800 . PMID   35465047.
  149. Pajevic, M., Vukosavljevic-Gvozden, T., Stevanovic, N., & Neumann, C. S. (2018). The relationship between the Dark Tetrad and a two-dimensional view of empathy. Personality and Individual Differences, 123, 125-130.
  150. Lyons 2019, pp. 17–18.
  151. 1 2 Blötner, Christian; Steinmayr, Ricarda; Bergold, Sebastian (October 2021). "Malicious mind readers? A meta-analysis on Machiavellianism and cognitive and affective empathy". Personality and Individual Differences. 181: 111023. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2021.111023.
  152. 1 2 Taylor, Ben. "Machiavellianism, Cognition, and Emotion: Understanding how the Machiavellian Thinks, Feels, and Thrives". Psych Central. Archived from the original on 2019-03-30.
  153. Paal, Tunde; Bereczkei, Tamas (August 2007). "Adult theory of mind, cooperation, Machiavellianism: The effect of mindreading on social relations". Personality and Individual Differences. 43 (3): 541–551. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2006.12.021.
  154. 1 2 3 Al Aïn, Syrina; Carré, Arnaud; Fantini-Hauwel, Carole; Baudouin, Jean-Yves; Besche-Richard, Chrystel (2013). "What is the emotional core of the multidimensional Machiavellian personality trait?". Frontiers in Psychology. 4: 454. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00454 . PMC   3717508 . PMID   23885245.
    • Barnett, Mark A.; Thompson, Shannon (September 1985). "The Role of Perspective Taking and Empathy in Children's Machiavellianism, Prosocial Behavior, and Motive for Helping". The Journal of Genetic Psychology. 146 (3): 295–305. doi:10.1080/00221325.1985.9914459.
    • Repacholi, Betty; Slaughter, Virginia; Pritchard, Michelle; Gibbs, Vicki (2004). "Theory of mind, Machiavellism, and social functioning in childhood". In Repacholi, Betty; Slaughter, Virginia (eds.). Individual Differences in Theory of Mind. pp. 99–120. doi:10.4324/9780203488508. ISBN   978-1-135-43234-8.
    • Richell, R.A; Mitchell, D.G.V; Newman, C; Leonard, A; Baron-Cohen, S; Blair, R.J.R (2003). "Theory of mind and psychopathy: can psychopathic individuals read the 'language of the eyes'?". Neuropsychologia. 41 (5): 523–526. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00175-6. PMID   12559146.
  155. 1 2 Lyons, M.; Caldwell, T.; Shultz, S. (September 2010). "Mind-reading and manipulation — Is Machiavellianism related to theory of mind?". Journal of Evolutionary Psychology. 8 (3): 261–274. doi:10.1556/JEP.8.2010.3.7.
  156. See the following:
    • Jones, D. N. (2016). The nature of Machiavellianism: Distinct patterns of misbehavior. In V. Zeigler-Hill & D. K. Marcus (Eds.), The dark side of personality: Science and practice in social, personality, and clinical psychology (pp. 87–107). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/14854-005
    • Jonason, P. K., Slomski, S., & Partyka, J. (2012). The Dark Triad at work: How toxic employees get their way. Personality and individual differences, 52(3), 449-453.
    • Lee, K., & Ashton, M. C. (2005). Psychopathy, Machiavellianism, and narcissism in the Five-Factor Model and the HEXACO model of personality structure. Personality and Individual differences, 38(7), 1571-1582.
    • Murphy, P. R. (2012). Attitude, Machiavellianism and the rationalization of misreporting. Accounting, Organizations and Society, 37(4), 242-259.
    • McHoskey, John W.; Worzel, William; Szyarto, Christopher (1998). "Machiavellianism and psychopathy". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 74 (1): 192–210. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.192. PMID 9457782.
  157. Leary & Hoyle 2013, p. 93.
  158. Kaufman, Scott Barry. "The Light Triad vs. Dark Triad of Personality". Scientific American Blog Network.
  159. Bagozzi, Richard P.; Verbeke, Willem J. M. I.; Dietvorst, Roeland C.; Belschak, Frank D.; van den Berg, Wouter E.; Rietdijk, Wim J. R. (November 2013). "Theory of Mind and Empathic Explanations of Machiavellianism: A Neuroscience Perspective". Journal of Management. 39 (7): 1760–1798. doi:10.1177/0149206312471393.
    • "However, some individuals high on Machiavellian traits have been shown to be skilled at affective-perspective taking and thereby may appear to exhibit an empathic response" ----Massey-Abernathy, A., & Byrd-Craven, J. (2016). Seeing but not feeling: Machiavellian traits in relation to physiological empathetic responding and life experiences. Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology, 2, 252-266.
    • "Following these conflicting findings, neurological research by Bagozzi and colleagues (2013) discovered that Machiavellian individuals are more attuned to others with respect to the detection of negative emotions but less likely to take another’s perspective. This negative association suggests an ability trade-off in Machiavellianism."---- Jones, D. N. (2016). The nature of Machiavellianism: Distinct patterns of misbehavior.
    • Waldman, David A.; Ward, M.K.; Becker, William J. (2017-03-21). "Neuroscience in Organizational Behavior". Annual Review of Organizational Psychology and Organizational Behavior. 4 (1): 425–444. doi:10.1146/annurev-orgpsych-032516-113316. ISSN   2327-0608.
  160. Andrew, J., Cooke, M., & Muncer, S. J. (2008). The relationship between empathy and Machiavellianism: An alternative to empathizing–systemizing theory. Personality and Individual Differences, 44(5), 1203-1211.
  161. Heym, Nadja; Firth, Jennifer; Kibowski, Fraenze; Sumich, Alexander; Egan, Vincent; Bloxsom, Claire A. J. (12 March 2019). "Empathy at the Heart of Darkness: Empathy Deficits That Bind the Dark Triad and Those That Mediate Indirect Relational Aggression". Frontiers in Psychiatry. 10: 95. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00095 . PMC   6423894 . PMID   30930800.
  162. "Machiavellianism | Definition, Politics, Psychology, & Facts | Britannica". www.britannica.com. Retrieved 2024-09-19.
  163. Wastell, Colin; Booth, Alexandra (December 2003). "Machiavellianism: An Alexithymic Perspective". Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 22 (6): 730–744. doi:10.1521/jscp.22.6.730.22931.
  164. McIlwain, Doris (2011). "Young Machiavellians and the traces of shame: Coping with vulnerability to a toxic affect". Narcissism and Machiavellianism in youth: Implications for the development of adaptive and maladaptive behavior. Washington: American Psychological Association. pp. 213–231. doi:10.1037/12352-012. ISBN   978-1-4338-0845-6.
  165. Ali, Farah; Amorim, Ines Sousa; Chamorro-Premuzic, Tomas (November 2009). "Empathy deficits and trait emotional intelligence in psychopathy and Machiavellianism". Personality and Individual Differences. 47 (7): 758–762. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.06.016.
  166. 1 2 3 4 Czibor, Andrea; Szabo, Zsolt Peter; Jones, Daniel N.; Zsido, Andras Norbert; Paal, Tunde; Szijjarto, Linda; Carre, Jessica R.; Bereczkei, Tamas (October 2017). "Male and female face of Machiavellianism: Opportunism or anxiety?". Personality and Individual Differences. 117: 221–229. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2017.06.002.
  167. Pilch, I. (2008). Machiavellianism, emotional intelligence, and social competence: Are Machiavellians interpersonally skilled? Polish Psychological Bulletin, 39, 158–164.
  168. Szijjarto, Linda; Bereczkei, Tamas (June 2015). "The Machiavellians' 'Cool Syndrome': They Experience Intensive Feelings but Have Difficulties in Expressing Their Emotions". Current Psychology. 34 (2): 363–375. doi:10.1007/s12144-014-9262-1.
  169. Czibor, Andrea; Vincze, Orsolya; Bereczkei, Tamas (December 2014). "Feelings and motives underlying Machiavellian behavioural strategies; narrative reports in a social dilemma situation". International Journal of Psychology. 49 (6): 519–524. doi:10.1002/ijop.12077. PMID   24842298.
    • Wastell, Colin; Booth, Alexandra (December 2003). "Machiavellianism: An Alexithymic Perspective". Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 22 (6): 730–744. doi:10.1521/jscp.22.6.730.22931.
    • Al Aïn, Syrina; Carré, Arnaud; Fantini-Hauwel, Carole; Baudouin, Jean-Yves; Besche-Richard, Chrystel (2013). "What is the emotional core of the multidimensional Machiavellian personality trait?". Frontiers in Psychology. 4: 454.
    • "What Drives Master Manipulators | Psychology Today". www.psychologytoday.com. Retrieved 2022-09-15.
  170. Leonard, Jayne (29 March 2023). "Alexithymia: Symptoms, diagnosis, and links with mental health". Medical News Today.
  171. Simon, Lorna J.; Francis, Patricia L.; Lombardo, John P. (August 1990). "Sex, Sex-Role, and Machiavellianism as Correlates of Decoding Ability". Perceptual and Motor Skills. 71 (1): 243–247. doi:10.2466/pms.1990.71.1.243.
  172. Sheikhi, Siamak; Issazadegan, Ali; Norozy, Merseda; Saboory, Ehsan (27 May 2017). "Relationships between alexithymia and Machiavellian personality beliefs among university students". British Journal of Guidance & Counselling. 45 (3): 297–304. doi:10.1080/03069885.2015.1072128.
  173. 1 2 Allsopp, J.; Eysenck, H.J.; Eysenck, S.B.G. (1991). "Machiavellianism as a component in psychoticism and extraversion". Personality and Individual Differences. 12 (1). Elsevier BV: 29–41. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(91)90129-y. ISSN   0191-8869.
  174. Bianchi, Renzo; Mirkovic, Danijela (2020). "Is Machiavellianism associated with depression? A cluster-analytic study". Personality and Individual Differences. 152. Elsevier BV: 109594. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2019.109594. ISSN   0191-8869.
  175. Latorre, Ronald A.; McLeoad, Emily (1978). "Machiavellianism and clinical depression in A geriatric sample". Journal of Clinical Psychology. 34 (3): 659–660. doi:10.1002/1097-4679(197807)34:3<659::AID-JCLP2270340315>3.0.CO;2-B. PMID   690204.
  176. Bianchi, Renzo; Patthey, Nathan; Mirkovic, Danijela; Lemaitre, Bruno; Schlegel, Katja (2020). "Machiavellian males with high emotional intelligence exhibit fewer depressive symptoms". Personality and Individual Differences. 158. Elsevier BV: 109867. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2020.109867. ISSN   0191-8869.
    • Jones, Daniel N.; Paulhus, Delroy L. (2009). "Chapter 7. Machiavellianism". In Leary, Mark R.; Hoyle, Rick H (eds.). Handbook of Individual Differences in Social Behavior. New York/London: The Guilford Press. pp. 257–273. ISBN 978-1-59385-647-2.
    • Spielberger & Butcher 2013
    • Christie & Geis 1970 , p. 93
  177. Leary & Hoyle 2013, p. 99.
  178. 1 2 Wilson, David Sloan; Near, David; Miller, Ralph R. (March 1996). "Machiavellianism: A synthesis of the evolutionary and psychological literatures". Psychological Bulletin. 119 (2): 285–299. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.119.2.285. PMID   8851275. ProQuest   614326001.
  179. 1 2 Rauthmann, John F. (February 2012). "Towards multifaceted Machiavellianism: Content, factorial, and construct validity of a German Machiavellianism Scale". Personality and Individual Differences. 52 (3): 345–351. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.10.038.
  180. Kowalski, Christopher Marcin; Kwiatkowska, Katarzyna; Kwiatkowska, Maria Magdalena; Ponikiewska, Klaudia; Rogoza, Radosław; Schermer, Julie Aitken (2018). "The Dark Triad traits and intelligence: Machiavellians are bright, and narcissists and psychopaths are ordinary". Personality and Individual Differences. 135: 1–6. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2018.06.049. S2CID   149807368.
  181. Austin, Elizabeth J.; Farrelly, Daniel; Black, Carolyn; Moore, Helen (2007). "Emotional intelligence, Machiavellianism and emotional manipulation: Does EI have a dark side?". Personality and Individual Differences. 43 (1): 179–189. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2006.11.019.
  182. O'Connor, P. J., & Athota, V. S. (2013). The intervening role of agreeableness in the relationship between trait emotional intelligence and Machiavellianism: Reassessing the potential dark side of EI. Personality and Individual Differences, 55(7), 750-754. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.06.006
  183. Nagler, U. K. J., Reiter, K. J., Furtner, M. R., & Rauthmann, J. F. (2014). Is there a "dark intelligence"? Emotional intelligence is used by dark personalities to emotionally manipulate others. Personality and Individual Differences, 65, 47-52. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.01.025
    • Sutton, Jon (October 2001). "Bullies: Thugs or thinkers?". The Psychologist. 14 (10): 530–534.
    • Davies, Martin; Stone, Tony (2004). "Exploring the Relationship Between Theory of Mind and Social- Communicative Functioning in Children with Autism". Individual Differences in Theory of Mind. pp. 207–222. doi:10.4324/9780203488508-13. ISBN   978-0-203-48850-8.
    • Esperger, Zsofia; Bereczkei, Tamas (November 2012). "Machiavellianism and Spontaneous Mentalization: One Step Ahead of Others". European Journal of Personality. 26 (6): 580–587. doi:10.1002/per.859.
  184. Czibor, Andrea; Bereczkei, Tamas (August 2012). "Machiavellian people's success results from monitoring their partners". Personality and Individual Differences. 53 (3): 202–206. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2012.03.005.
  185. Kerekes, Zsuzsanna (January 2010). "The Presence of Others, Prosocial Traits, Machiavellianism: A Personality × Situation Approach". Social Psychology. 41 (4): 238–245. doi:10.1027/1864-9335/a000032.
  186. Blötner, Christian; Bergold, Sebastian (January 2023). "It is double pleasure to deceive the deceiver: Machiavellianism is associated with producing but not necessarily with falling for bullshit". British Journal of Social Psychology. 62 (1): 467–485. doi: 10.1111/bjso.12559 . PMID   35804484.
  187. Verbeke, Willem J. M. I.; Rietdijk, Wim J. R.; van den Berg, Wouter E.; Dietvorst, Roeland C.; Worm, Loek; Bagozzi, Richard P. (November 2011). "The making of the Machiavellian brain: A structural MRI analysis". Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics. 4 (4): 205–216. doi:10.1037/a0025802.
  188. Bereczkei, Tamas; Deak, Anita; Papp, Peter; Perlaki, Gabor; Orsi, Gergely (June 2013). "Neural correlates of Machiavellian strategies in a social dilemma task". Brain and Cognition. 82 (1): 108–116. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2013.02.012. PMID   23548839.
  189. Bereczkei, Tamas; Papp, Peter; Kincses, Peter; Bodrogi, Barbara; Perlaki, Gabor; Orsi, Gergely; Deak, Anita (August 2015). "The neural basis of the Machiavellians' decision making in fair and unfair situations". Brain and Cognition. 98: 53–64. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2015.05.006. PMID   26093237.
  190. Cohen-Zimerman, Shira; Chau, Aileen; Krueger, Frank; Gordon, Barry; Grafman, Jordan (December 2017). "Machiavellian tendencies increase following damage to the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex". Neuropsychologia. 107: 68–75. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.11.007 . PMID   29126929.
  191. McNamara, Patrick; Durso, Raymon; Harris, Erica (July 2007). "'Machiavellianism' and frontal dysfunction: Evidence from Parkinson's disease". Cognitive Neuropsychiatry. 12 (4): 285–300. doi:10.1080/13546800701202316. PMID   17558639.
  192. Jones, Daniel N.; De Roos, Melissa S. (June 2017). "Machiavellian flexibility in negative mate retention". Personal Relationships. 24 (2): 265–279. doi:10.1111/pere.12181.
  193. Boyatzis, Richard E.; Rochford, Kylie; Jack, Anthony I. (4 March 2014). "Antagonistic neural networks underlying differentiated leadership roles". Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 8: 114. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00114 . PMC   3941086 . PMID   24624074.
  194. Gong, Xinyu; Quan, Fangying; Wang, Liang; Zhu, Wenfeng; Lin, Danhua; Xia, Ling-Xiang (October 2023). "The relationship among regional gray matter volume in the brain, Machiavellianism and social aggression in emerging adulthood: A voxel-based morphometric study". Current Psychology. 42 (29): 25160–25170. doi:10.1007/s12144-022-03574-1.
  195. Pandey, Janak; Rastogi, Renu (August 1979). "Machiavellianism and Ingratiation". The Journal of Social Psychology. 108 (2): 221–225. doi:10.1080/00224545.1979.9711635.
  196. Jonason, Peter K.; Schmitt, David P. (July 2012). "What Have You Done for Me Lately? Friendship-Selection in the Shadow of the Dark Triad Traits". Evolutionary Psychology. 10 (3): 400–421. doi:10.1177/147470491201000303. PMC   10480828 . PMID   22947669.
  197. Brewer, Gayle; Parkinson, Madison; Pickles, Alice; Anson, Joshua; Mulinder, Georgia (April 2023). "Dark Triad traits and relationship dissolution". Personality and Individual Differences. 204: 112045. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2022.112045 . S2CID   254775278.
  198. Ali, Farah; Chamorro-Premuzic, Tomas (January 2010). "The dark side of love and life satisfaction: Associations with intimate relationships, psychopathy and Machiavellianism". Personality and Individual Differences. 48 (2): 228–233. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.10.016.
  199. "APA Dictionary of Psychology". dictionary.apa.org. Retrieved 2024-09-19.
  200. Christie & Geis 1970, pp. 3, 93, 210.
  201. Brewer, G.; Abell, L. (2017). "Machiavellianism, Relationship Satisfaction, and Romantic Relationship Quality". Europe's Journal of Psychology. 13 (3): 491–502. doi:10.5964/ejop.v13i3.1217. PMC   5590532 . PMID   28904597.
  202. Carter, Gregory Louis; Campbell, Anne C.; Muncer, Steven (January 2014). "The Dark Triad personality: Attractiveness to women". Personality and Individual Differences. 56: 57–61. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2013.08.021.
  203. Rauthmann, John F.; Kolar, Gerald P. (April 2013). "The perceived attractiveness and traits of the Dark Triad: Narcissists are perceived as hot, Machiavellians and psychopaths not". Personality and Individual Differences. 54 (5): 582–586. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2012.11.005.
  204. 1 2 Jauk, Emanuel; Neubauer, Aljoscha C.; Mairunteregger, Thomas; Pemp, Stephanie; Sieber, Katharina P.; Rauthmann, John F. (March 2016). "How Alluring Are Dark Personalities? the Dark Triad and Attractiveness in Speed Dating". European Journal of Personality. 30 (2): 125–138. doi:10.1002/per.2040.
  205. 1 2 Borráz-León, Javier I.; Rantala, Markus J. (January 2021). "Does the Dark Triad predict self-perceived attractiveness, mate value, and number of sexual partners both in men and women?". Personality and Individual Differences. 168: 110341. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2020.110341.
  206. Buckels, Erin E.; Trapnell, Paul D.; Paulhus, Delroy L. (September 2014). "Trolls just want to have fun". Personality and Individual Differences. 67: 97–102. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2014.01.016.
  207. Brubaker, Pamela Jo; Montez, Daniel; Church, Scott Haden (April 2021). "The Power of Schadenfreude: Predicting Behaviors and Perceptions of Trolling Among Reddit Users". Social Media + Society. 7 (2): 205630512110213. doi: 10.1177/20563051211021382 . S2CID   235724896.
  208. Hodson, Gordon, Sarah M. Hogg, and Cara C. MacInnis. "The role of “dark personalities”(narcissism, Machiavellianism, psychopathy), Big Five personality factors, and ideology in explaining prejudice." Journal of research in personality 43.4 (2009): 686-690.
  209. Jones, Daniel N.; Weiser, Dana A. (2014). "Differential infidelity patterns among the Dark Triad". Personality and Individual Differences. 57. Elsevier BV: 20–24. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2013.09.007. ISSN   0191-8869.
  210. Szabó, Edit; Jones, Daniel N. (2019). "Gender differences moderate Machiavellianism and impulsivity: Implications for Dark Triad research". Personality and Individual Differences. 141. Elsevier BV: 160–165. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2019.01.008. ISSN   0191-8869.
  211. Jonason, Peter K.; Foster, Joshua; Oshio, Atsushi; Sitnikova, Maria; Birkas, Bela; Gouveia, Valdiney (July 2017). "Self-construals and the Dark Triad traits in six countries". Personality and Individual Differences. 113: 120–124. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2017.02.053.
  212. Jonason, Peter K.; Żemojtel-Piotrowska, Magdalena; Piotrowski, Jarosław; Sedikides, Constantine; Campbell, W. Keith; Gebauer, Jochen E.; Maltby, John; Adamovic, Mladen; Adams, Byron G.; Kadiyono, Anissa Lestari; Atitsogbe, Kokou A.; Bundhoo, Harshalini Y.; Bălțătescu, Sergiu; Bilić, Snežana; Brulin, Joel Gruneau; Chobthamkit, Phatthanakit; Del Carmen Dominguez, Alejandra; Dragova-Koleva, Sonya; El-Astal, Sofián; Esteves, Carla Sofia; Labib M. Eldesoki, Walaa; Gouveia, Valdiney V.; Gundolf, Katherine; Ilisko, Dzintra; Jauk, Emanuel; Kamble, Shanmukh V.; Khachatryan, Narine; Klicperova-Baker, Martina; Knezovic, Emil; Kovacs, Monika; Lei, Xuejun; Liik, Kadi; Mamuti, Agim; Moreta-Herrera, Carlos Rodrigo; Milfont, Taciano L.; Wei Ong, Chin; Osin, Evgeny; Park, Joonha; Petrovic, Boban; Ramos-Diaz, Jano; Ridic, Goran; Qadir, Abdul; Samekin, Adil; Sawicki, Artur; Tiliouine, Habib; Tomsik, Robert; Umeh, Charles S.; van den Bos, Kees; Van Hiel, Alain; Uslu, Osman; Wlodarczyk, Anna; Yahiiaev, Illia (December 2020). "Country-level correlates of the Dark Triad traits in 49 countries". Journal of Personality. 88 (6): 1252–1267. doi:10.1111/jopy.12569. hdl: 11343/276888 . PMID   32557617.
  213. Jonason, Peter K.; Foster, Joshua D.; Kavanagh, Phillip S.; Gouveia, Valdiney V.; Birkás, Béla (October 2018). "Basic Values and the Dark Triad Traits". Journal of Individual Differences. 39 (4): 220–228. doi:10.1027/1614-0001/a000267. S2CID   149501810.
  214. Schmitt, Helena S.; Sindermann, Cornelia; Li, Mei; Ma, Yina; Kendrick, Keith M.; Becker, Benjamin; Montag, Christian (2020). "The Dark Side of Emotion Recognition – Evidence From Cross-Cultural Research in Germany and China". Frontiers in Psychology. 11: 1132. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01132 . PMC   7363803 . PMID   32733302.
  215. Kuo, Hwang Kwang; Marsella, Anthony J. (April 1977). "The Meaning and Measurement of Machiavellianism in Chinese and American College Students". The Journal of Social Psychology. 101 (2): 165–173. doi:10.1080/00224545.1977.9924005. PMID   865093.
  216. 1 2 Jones, Daniel N.; Paulhus, Delroy L. (2010). "Different Provocations Trigger Aggression in Narcissists and Psychopaths". Social Psychological and Personality Science. 1 (1): 12–18. doi:10.1177/1948550609347591. ISSN   1948-5506.
  217. Rauthmann, John F.; Kolar, Gerald P. (2013). "Positioning the Dark Triad in the interpersonal circumplex: The friendly-dominant narcissist, hostile-submissive Machiavellian, and hostile-dominant psychopath?". Personality and Individual Differences. 54 (5). Elsevier BV: 622–627. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2012.11.021. ISSN   0191-8869.
  218. Jones, Daniel N.; Paulhus, Delroy L. (2017). "Duplicity among the dark triad: Three faces of deceit". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 113 (2): 329–342. doi:10.1037/pspp0000139. ISSN   1939-1315. PMID   28253006.
  219. McHoskey, John W (2001). "Machiavellianism and sexuality: on the moderating role of biological sex". Personality and Individual Differences. 31 (5). Elsevier BV: 779–789. doi:10.1016/s0191-8869(00)00180-x. ISSN   0191-8869.
  220. McHoskey, John W. (1999). "Machiavellianism, Intrinsic Versus Extrinsic Goals, and Social Interest: A Self-Determination Theory Analysis". Motivation and Emotion. 23 (4): 267–283. doi:10.1023/A:1021338809469.
    • Drory, Amos; Gluskinos, Uri M. (February 1980). "Machiavellianism and leadership". Journal of Applied Psychology. 65 (1): 81–86. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.65.1.81.
    • James, Oliver (2013). Office Politics: How to Thrive in a World of Lying, Backstabbing and Dirty Tricks. Random House. p. 36. ISBN   978-1-4090-0557-5.
    • Dugan, Riley; Rouziou, Maria; Hochstein, Bryan (December 2019). "It is better to be loved than feared: Machiavellianism and the dark side of internal networking". Marketing Letters. 30 (3–4): 261–274. doi:10.1007/s11002-019-09503-w. S2CID   210553791.
    • Kiazad, Kohyar; Restubog, Simon Lloyd D.; Zagenczyk, Thomas J.; Kiewitz, Christian; Tang, Robert L. (August 2010). "In pursuit of power: The role of authoritarian leadership in the relationship between supervisors' Machiavellianism and subordinates' perceptions of abusive supervisory behavior". Journal of Research in Personality. 44 (4): 512–519. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2010.06.004.
    • Ellen, B. Parker; Alexander, Katherine C.; Mackey, Jeremy D.; McAllister, Charn P.; Carson, Jack E. (December 2021). "Portrait of a workplace deviant: A clearer picture of the Big Five and Dark Triad as predictors of workplace deviance". Journal of Applied Psychology. 106 (12): 1950–1961. doi:10.1037/apl0000880. PMID   33600196.
    • "'Cheating under pressure: A self-protection model of workplace cheating behavior': Correction to Mitchel et al. (2017)". Journal of Applied Psychology. 103 (1): 36. January 2018. doi:10.1037/apl0000275. PMID   28967764.
    • Shu, Lisa L.; Gino, Francesca; Bazerman, Max H. (March 2011). "Dishonest Deed, Clear Conscience: When Cheating Leads to Moral Disengagement and Motivated Forgetting". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 37 (3): 330–349. doi:10.1177/0146167211398138. PMID   21307176.
  221. Djurdjevic, Emilija; Rosen, Christopher C.; Conroy, Samantha A.; Rawski, Shannon L.; Sosna, Katherine U (June 2019). "The influence of political climate on job pursuit intentions and the moderating effect of Machiavellianism". International Journal of Selection and Assessment. 27 (2): 180–192. doi:10.1111/ijsa.12242. S2CID   151216697.
  222. Brownell, Katrina M.; McMullen, Jeffery S.; O'Boyle, Ernest H. (May 2021). "Fatal attraction: A systematic review and research agenda of the dark triad in entrepreneurship". Journal of Business Venturing. 36 (3): 106106. doi:10.1016/j.jbusvent.2021.106106. S2CID   233542634.
  223. Hmieleski, Keith M.; Lerner, Daniel A. (October 2016). "The Dark Triad and Nascent Entrepreneurship: An Examination of Unproductive versus Productive Entrepreneurial Motives". Journal of Small Business Management. 54: 7–32. doi:10.1111/jsbm.12296. S2CID   152230559.
  224. Volmer, J., Koch, I. K., & Göritz, A. S. (2016). The bright and dark sides of leaders' dark triad traits: Effects on subordinates' career success and well-being. Personality and Individual Differences, 101, 413-418.
  225. 1 2 Gürlek, Mert (2 January 2021). "Shedding light on the relationships between Machiavellianism, career ambition, and unethical behavior intention". Ethics & Behavior. 31 (1): 38–59. doi:10.1080/10508422.2020.1764846. S2CID   225719181.
  226. Gruda, Dritjon; McCleskey, Jim; Khoury, Issa (April 2023). "Cause we are living in a Machiavellian world, and I am a Machiavellian major: Machiavellianism and academic major choice". Personality and Individual Differences. 205: 112096. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2023.112096. hdl: 10400.14/40205 .
  227. Volmer, J., Koch, I. K., & Göritz, A. S. (2016). The bright and dark sides of leaders' dark triad traits: Effects on subordinates' career success and well-being. Personality and Individual Differences, 101, 413-418.
  228. Spurk, D., Keller, A. C., & Hirschi, A. (2016). Do bad guys get ahead or fall behind? Relationships of the dark triad of personality with objective and subjective career success. Social psychological and personality science, 7(2), 113-121.
  229. Pilch, I., Turska, E. Relationships Between Machiavellianism, Organizational Culture, and Workplace Bullying: Emotional Abuse from the Target’s and the Perpetrator’s Perspective. J Bus Ethics 128, 83–93 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-014-2081-3
  230. O'Boyle, Ernest H.; Forsyth, Donelson R.; Banks, George C.; Story, Paul A.; White, Charles D. (2015). "A Meta-Analytic Test of Redundancy and Relative Importance of the Dark Triad and Five-Factor Model of Personality". Journal of Personality. 83 (6): 644–664. doi:10.1111/jopy.12126. ISSN   0022-3506. PMID   25168647.
  231. 1 2 Rauthmann, John F. (July 2013). "Investigating the MACH–IV With Item Response Theory and Proposing the Trimmed MACH*". Journal of Personality Assessment. 95 (4): 388–397. doi:10.1080/00223891.2012.742905. PMID   23186231.
  232. Fehr, Beverley, Deborah Samsom, and Delroy L. Paulhus. "The construct of Machiavellianism: Twenty years later." Advances in personality assessment pg.101-108.
  233. Dahling, Jason J.; Whitaker, Brian G.; Levy, Paul E. (March 2009). "The Development and Validation of a New Machiavellianism Scale". Journal of Management. 35 (2): 219–257. doi:10.1177/0149206308318618. S2CID   54937924.
  234. Shining Light on the Dark Side of Personality: Measurement Properties and Theoretical Advances, p. 80
  235. "Machiavellianism Inventory-IV" Studies In Machiavellianism (1970), pg. 8, 10, and 378.
  236. Wörsdörfer, Manuel (2014). "Inside the 'Homo Oeconomicus Brain': Towards a Reform of the Economics Curriculum?". Journal of Business Ethics Education. 11: 5–40. doi:10.5840/jbee2014112. SSRN   2484618.
  237. Sakalaki, Maria; Richardson, Clive; Thépaut, Yves (2007). "Machiavellianism and Economic Opportunism". Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 37 (6): 1181–1190. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2007.00208.x.
    • Wilson, David Sloan; Near, David; Miller, Ralph R. (March 1996). "Machiavellianism: A synthesis of the evolutionary and psychological literatures". Psychological Bulletin. 119 (2): 285–299. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.119.2.285. PMID   8851275. ProQuest   614326001.
    • Dahling, Jason J.; Whitaker, Brian G.; Levy, Paul E. (March 2009). "The Development and Validation of a New Machiavellianism Scale". Journal of Management. 35 (2): 219–257. doi:10.1177/0149206308318618.
  238. Wilson, D. S., Near, D. C., & Miller, R. R. (1998). Individual differences in Machiavellianism as a mix of cooperative and exploitative strategies. Evolution and Human Behavior, 19(3), 203-212.

Sources