Halo effect

Last updated

The halo effect (sometimes called the halo error) is the proclivity for positive impressions of a person, company, country, brand, or product in one area to positively influence one's opinion or feelings. [1] [2] The halo effect is "the name given to the phenomenon whereby evaluators tend to be influenced by their previous judgments of performance or personality." [3] The halo effect is a cognitive bias which can prevent someone from forming an image of a person, a product or a brand based on the sum of all objective circumstances at hand.

Contents

The term was coined by Edward Thorndike. A simplified example of the halo effect is when a person, after noticing that an individual in a photograph is attractive, well groomed, and properly attired, then assumes, using a mental heuristic, that the person in the photograph is a good person based upon the rules of their own social concept. [4] [5] [6] This constant error in judgment is reflective of the individual's preferences, prejudices, ideology, aspirations, and social perception. [7] [6] [8] [9] [10]

Context and applications

Psychology

The halo effect is a perception distortion (or cognitive bias) that affects the way people interpret the information about someone with whom they have formed a positive gestalt. [11] An example of the halo effect is when a person finds out someone they have formed a positive gestalt with has cheated on their taxes. Because of the positive gestalt, the person may dismiss the significance of this behavior. They may even think that the person simply made a mistake. The person would justify the behavior and connect it with the other person's positive gestalt. The halo effect refers to the tendency of evaluating an individual positively on many traits because of a shared belief. [12]

It is a type of immediate judgment discrepancy, or cognitive bias, in which a person making an initial assessment of another person, place, or thing will assume ambiguous information based upon concrete information. [13] [14] :p. xi [7] The halo effect is an evaluation by an individual and can affect the perception of a decision, action, idea, business, person, group, entity, or other whenever concrete data is generalized or influences ambiguous information. [13] [14] :p. xi [9] [15]

The halo effect can also be explained as the behavior (usually unconscious) of using evaluations based on unrelated criteria, to make judgments about something or someone. The halo effect is sometimes used to refer specifically to when this behavior has a positive correlation, such as viewing someone who is attractive as likely to be successful and popular. When this judgment has a negative connotation, such as when someone unattractive is more readily blamed for a crime than someone attractive, it is sometimes referred to as the horn effect. [16]

Marketing

The term halo effect is used in marketing to explain consumer bias toward certain products because of favorable experience with other products made by the same company. [17] It is used in the part of brand marketing called "line extensions". One common halo effect is when the perceived positive features of a particular item extend to a broader brand. A notable example is the manner in which the popularity of Apple's iPod generated enthusiasm for the corporation's other products. [18] [19] Advertising often makes use of television shows, movies and those who star in them, to promote products via the halo effect. [20] [21]

In the automotive industry, exotic, limited production luxury models or low-volume sports cars made by a manufacturer's racing, motorsports, or in-house modification teams, are sometimes referred to as "halo cars" for the effect they are intended to produce on selling other vehicles within the make. [22] To contrast this with the automotive terminology "flagship model", see here.

Advertising in one channel has been shown to have a halo effect on advertising in another channel. [23] [24]

A halo effect with regard to health, dubbed a "health halo", is used in food marketing to increase sales of a product; it can result in increased consumption of the product in the halo which may be unhealthy. [25] [26]

The term "halo effect" has also been applied to human rights organizations that have used their status to move away from their stated goals. Political scientist Gerald Steinberg has claimed that non-governmental organizations (NGOs) take advantage of the halo effect and are "given the status of impartial moral watchdogs" by governments and the news media. [27] [28]

The Ronald McDonald House, a widely known NGO, openly celebrates the positive outcomes it receives from the halo effect. The web page for the Ronald McDonald House in Durham, North Carolina, states that 95% of survey participants were aware of Ronald McDonald House Charities. This awareness is attributed to the halo effect, as employees, customers, and stakeholders are more likely to be involved in a charity that they recognize and trust, with a name and logo that are familiar. [29]

A brand's halo effect can protect its reputation in the event of a crisis. An event that is detrimental to a brand that is viewed favorably would not be as threatening or damaging to a brand that consumers view unfavorably. [30] [31]

Other uses

Non-psychology/business use of the term "halo effect" describes the monetary value of the spillover effect [lower-alpha 1] when an organization's marketing budget is subsequently reduced. [lower-alpha 2] This was first demonstrated to students via the 1966 version of a textbook and a software package named "The Marketing Game." [lower-alpha 3]

The halo effect can also be used in the case of institutions as one's favorable perceptions regarding an aspect of an organization could determine a positive view of its entire operations. [32] For example, if a hospital is known for its excellent open heart and cardiac program, then the community would expect it to excel in other areas as well. This can also be demonstrated in the positive perceptions of financial institutions that gained favorable coverage in the media due to meteoric growth but eventually failed afterward. [33]

The term "halo effect" is also used in metal detecting [34] to denote the enhanced detectability of a metal item or coin when it has been left undisturbed for some period of time in wet soil. The object can leach some metallic properties into the soil, making it more detectable. The area surrounding the object is called its "halo."

History

The halo effect was originally identified in 1907 by the American psychologist Frederick L. Wells (1884–1964). [35] However, it was only officially recognized in 1920 with empirical evidence provided by the psychologist Edward Thorndike (1874–1949). [35] Edward Thorndike was the first to say the halo effect is a specific cognitive bias in which one aspect of the person, brand, product, or institution affects one's thoughts or judgment of the entity's other aspects or dimensions. [36] Thorndike, an early behaviorist, was an important contributor to the study of the psychology of learning. He gave the phenomenon its name in his 1920 article "A Constant Error in Psychological Ratings". [4] In "Constant Error", Thorndike set out to replicate the study in hopes of pinning down the bias that he thought was present in these ratings. Subsequent researchers have studied it in relation to attractiveness and its bearing on the judicial and educational systems. [16] Thorndike originally coined the term referring only to people; however, its use has been greatly expanded, especially in the area of brand marketing. [4]

Supporting evidence

In Thorndike's words, "Ratings were apparently affected by a marked tendency to think of the person in general as rather good or rather inferior and to color the judgments of the qualities by this general feeling." [37] In "A Constant Error in Psychological Ratings", Thorndike asked two commanding officers to evaluate their soldiers in terms of physical qualities (neatness, voice, physique, bearing, and energy), intellect, leadership skills, and personal qualities (including dependability, loyalty, responsibility, selflessness, and cooperation). [4] In Thorndike's study, attractiveness plays an important role in how people tend to consider a person, such as whether a person is friendly or not based on his or her physical appearance. His goal was to see how the ratings of one characteristic affected other characteristics.

Thorndike's study showed how there was too great a correlation in the commanding officers' responses. In his review, he stated, "The correlations are too high and too even. For example, for the three raters next studied[,] the average correlation for physique with intelligence is .31; for physique with leadership, .39; and for physique with character, .28". [38] The ratings of one of the special qualities of an officer often started a trend in the rating results. The halo effect is not an indication of the existence of a correlation, but instead indicates that the correlation is too high. Thorndike used the halo effect to describe both a positive and negative halo.

Cognitive bias

Cognitive bias is a pattern in perception, interpretation, or judgment that consistently leads to an individual misunderstanding something about themselves or their social environment, leading to poor decision-making or irrational behavior. [39] The halo effect is classified as a cognitive bias because the halo effect is a perception error that distorts the way a person sees someone, and cognitive bias is a perception error that distorts the way that people see themselves. [12]

The term "halo" is used in analogy with the religious concept: a glowing circle crowning the heads of saints in countless medieval and Renaissance paintings, bathing the saint's face in heavenly light. The observer may be subject to overestimating the worth of the observed by the presence of a quality that adds light on the whole like a halo. In other words, observers tend to bend their judgement according to one patent characteristic of the person (the "halo") or a few of his traits, [40] generalizing toward a judgement of that person's character (e.g., in the literal hagiologic case, "entirely good and worthy").

The effect works in both positive and negative directions (and is hence sometimes called the horns and halo effect). If the observer likes one aspect of something, they will have a positive predisposition toward everything about it. If the observer dislikes one aspect of something, they will have a negative predisposition toward everything about it. [41]

Role of attractiveness

A person's attractiveness has also been found to produce a halo effect. Attractiveness provides a valuable aspect of the halo effect to consider because it can be influenced by several specific traits. [42] These perceptions of attractiveness may affect judgments tied to personality traits. Physical attributes contribute to perceptions of attractiveness (e.g., physique, hair, eye color). For example, someone who is perceived as attractive, due in part to physical traits, may be more likely to be perceived as kind or intelligent. The role of attractiveness in producing the halo effect has been illustrated through a number of studies. Recent research, for example, has revealed that attractiveness may affect perceptions tied to life success and personality. [43] In this study, attractiveness was correlated with weight, indicating that attractiveness itself may be influenced by various specific traits. Included in the personality variables were trustworthiness and friendliness. [42] People perceived as being more attractive were more likely to be perceived as trustworthy and friendly. What this suggests is that perceptions of attractiveness may influence a variety of other traits, which supports the concept of the halo effect.

On personality

People's first impressions of others influence their later decision to either approach or avoid those individuals. [44] When people first encounter someone, the information present about that individual is limited; therefore, people will use the information available to assume other characteristics about that person; for instance, observable behaviors such as eye contact, leaning forward, smiling and positive hand gestures (ex. steepling hands) are linked to positive emotions, while avoiding eye contact, leaning back, avoiding touch, and defensive hand gestures (ex. hands in pockets) or no gestures at all are linked to feelings of detachment. [44] Besides that, another popular example used when referring to the halo effect is the phenomenon called the attractiveness stereotype [6] or when encountering individuals who are similar to others in some aspects, like personality or life history like the school they attended. [45] People tend to assume that physically attractive individuals are more likely to be more healthy, successful, courteous, containing higher moral standards, and greater social competence than other people; on the other hand, the attractiveness stereotype can also carry a negative connotation as some people may think of attractive people as less honest and more conceited than others. [6]

Dion, Berscheid & Walster (1972) conducted a study on the relationship between attractiveness and the halo effect. Sixty students, thirty males and thirty females from the University of Minnesota took part in the experiment. Each subject was given three different photos to examine: one of an attractive individual, one of an individual of average attractiveness, and one of an unattractive individual. [46] The participants judged the photos' subjects along 27 different personality traits (including altruism, conventionality, self-assertiveness, stability, emotionality, trustworthiness, extraversion, kindness, and sexual promiscuity). Participants were then asked to predict the overall happiness the photos' subjects would feel for the rest of their lives, including marital happiness (least likely to get divorced), parental happiness (most likely to be a good parent), social and professional happiness (most likely to experience life fulfillment), and overall happiness. Finally, participants were asked if the subjects would hold a job of high status, medium status, or low status. [46] Results showed that most of the participants overwhelmingly believed more attractive subjects have more socially desirable personality traits than either averagely attractive or unattractive subjects, would lead happier lives in general, have happier marriages, and have more career success, including holding more secure, prestigious jobs. Participants, however, believed that attractive individuals would be worse parents than both averagely-attractive and unattractive individuals.

Academics and intelligence

A study by Landy & Sigall (1974) demonstrated the Halo Effect, looking at male judgments of female intelligence and competence on academic tasks. Sixty male undergraduate students rated the quality of essays which included both well- and poorly-written samples. One third were presented with a photo of an attractive female as author, another third with that of an unattractive female as author, and the last third were shown neither. On average, most of the participants gave significantly better writing evaluations for the more attractive author. On a scale of 1 to 9, the well-written essay by the attractive author received an average of 6.7 while the unattractive author received a 5.9 (with a 6.6 as a control). The gap was larger on the poor essay: the attractive author received an average of 5.2, the control a 4.7, and the unattractive author a 2.7, suggesting male readers are generally more willing to give physically attractive females the benefit of the doubt when performance is below standard than those not considered attractive.

Research conducted by Moore, Filippou & Perrett (2011) sought residual cues to intelligence in female and male faces while attempting to control for the attractiveness halo effect. Over 300 photographs of Caucasian British college students were rated for perceived intelligence. The photographs that were scored lowest in perceived intelligence were used to create a low-intelligence composite face and those photographs that were scored highest in perceived intelligence were used to create a high-intelligence composite face. Both female and male faces of high- and low-perceived intelligence were created, resulting in four groups of composite faces. Participants for the study were recruited online; 164 female and 92 male heterosexual residents of the UK rated each of the composite faces for intelligence and attractiveness. Of the female composites, attractiveness seemed to be controlled as both the high- and low-perceived intelligence groups were rated as equally attractive. However, of the male face composites, the high-perceived intelligence group was rated as significantly more attractive than the low-perceived intelligence group, suggesting that either the authors could not adequately control for the attractiveness halo effect for the male composite photographs or that intelligence is an integral factor of attractiveness in high-intelligence male faces. The second part of the study found that the composites in the high-perceived intelligence group were rated highest in the factors of friendly and funny as markers of intelligence in both the female and male groups. While intelligence does not seem to be a factor that contributes to attractiveness in women, with regards to men, attractive faces are perceived to be more intelligent, friendly, and funny by women and men.

Political effects

Officeholders who create what The New York Times called "a living legacy" benefit from a halo effect when their overall accomplishments are subsequently evaluated. [47] [lower-alpha 4]

Researchers have shown that perceived physical and vocal attractiveness (or their opposite) lead to bias in judgment. [48] [49] A 2010 study [50] found that attractiveness and familiarity are strong predictors of decisions regarding who is put in a position of leadership. Judgments made following one-second exposures to side-by-side photos of two US congressional candidates were reasonably predictive of election outcomes. Similar studies ( Palmer & Peterson 2012 ) found that even when taking factual knowledge into account, candidates who were rated as more attractive were still perceived as more knowledgeable. Thus, beauty evaluations also emerge as major predictors of electoral success. [51] [52]

The judicial context

Study results showing the influence of the halo effect in the judicial context exist:

Gender differences

Kaplan (1978) found that some women were influenced by the halo effect on attractiveness only when presented with members of the opposite sex. Dermer & Thiel (1975) continued this line of research, going on to demonstrate that jealousy of an attractive individual has a slight effect in evaluation of that person. These works showed these halo effect more prevalent among females than males. Later research by Moore, Filippou & Perrett (2011) was able to control for attractiveness in composite photographs of females who were perceived to be of high or low intelligence, while showing that the attractiveness halo effect was seen in high intelligent male composite faces by heterosexual residents of the UK. Either the halo effect is negated by feelings of jealousy in women [53] or the halo effect is lessened when women are looking at same sex individuals [54] or the attractiveness halo effect can be controlled for in women. [55]

Possible causes

Rating error effect, mistakes made by raters when they use a rating scale, reflect the task competence of the rater, as well as the rater's sex, social position, race, religion, and age. Researchers showed that halo effect is one component of this error. [37] Fisicaro and Lance introduced three explanatory models. [56] The first model named the general impression model states that global evaluation affects the rating of individual characteristics. [57] The salient dimension model states that how people perceive an individual characteristic affects their evaluation of other characteristics. [56] The inadequate discrimination model refers to the rater's failure to identify different behaviors of the person being evaluated. [57]

The reverse halo effect

The reverse halo effect occurs when positive evaluations of an individual cause negative consequences. Rater errors pose special problems for the issues of "reliability and validity". [37] Furthermore, ratings that differ in time may accurately reflect a change in behavior even though this difference would demonstrate an artificial lack of reliability. A follow-up study with both men and women participants supported this, as well as showing that attractive women were expected to be conceited and have a higher socioeconomic status. Eagly et al. (1991) also commented on this phenomenon, showing that more attractive individuals of both sexes were expected to be higher in vanity and possibly egotistic. [58] Applied instances of the reverse halo effect include negative evaluations of criminals who use their attractiveness to their advantage [16] and rating a philosophical essay lower when written by a young female than an old male. [16] [59]

The horn effect

A negative form of the halo effect, called the horn effect, the devil effect, or the reverse halo effect, allows one a disliked trait or aspect of a person or product to negatively influence globally. [36] Psychologists call it a "bias blind spot:" [60] "Individuals believe (that negative) traits are inter-connected." [61] due to a negative first impression. [62] [63] The Guardian wrote of the devil effect in relation to Hugo Chavez: "Some leaders can become so demonized that it's impossible to assess their achievements and failures in a balanced way." [64] For those seen in a negative light, anything they do that is negative is exemplified, while the positive things they do are not seen, or are doubted. [16]

Education

Abikoff et al. (1993) found the halo effect is also present in the classroom. In this study, both regular and special education elementary school teachers watched videotapes of what they believed to be children in regular 4th-grade classrooms. In reality, the children were actors, depicting behaviors present in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), oppositional defiant disorder (ODD), or standard behavior. The teachers were asked to rate the frequency of hyperactive behaviors observed in the children. Teachers rated hyperactive behaviors accurately for children with ADHD; however, the ratings of hyperactivity were much higher for the children with ODD-like behaviors, showing a horn effect for children who appeared to have ODD.

Foster & Ysseldyke (1976) also found the halo effect present in teachers' evaluations of children. Regular and special education elementary school teachers watched videos of a normal child whom they were told was either "emotionally disturbed", possessing a learning disorder, "mentally retarded", or "normal". The teachers then completed referral forms based on the child's behavior. The results showed that teachers held negative expectancies toward emotionally disturbed children, maintaining these expectancies even when presented with normal behavior. In addition, the "mentally retarded" label showed a greater degree of negative bias than the "emotionally disturbed" or "learning disabled" label.

Observations

"In the classroom, teachers are subject to the halo effect rating error when evaluating their students. For example, a teacher who sees a well-behaved student might tend to assume this student is also bright, diligent, and engaged before that teacher has objectively evaluated the student's capacity in these areas. When these types of halo effects occur, they can affect students' approval ratings in certain areas of functioning and can even affect students' grades." (Rasmussen, Encyclopedia of Educational Psychology, Volume 1, 2008)

"In the work setting, the halo effect is most likely to show up in a supervisor's appraisal of a subordinate's job performance. In fact, the halo effect is probably the most common bias in performance appraisal. Think about what happens when a supervisor evaluates the performance of a subordinate. The supervisor may give prominence to a single characteristic of the employee, such as enthusiasm, and allow the entire evaluation to be colored by how he or she judges the employee on that one characteristic. Even though the employee may lack the requisite knowledge or ability to perform the job successfully, if the employee's work shows enthusiasm, the supervisor may very well give him or her a higher performance rating than is justified by knowledge or ability." (Schneider, F.W., Gruman, J. A., & Coutts, L. M., Applied Social Psychology, 2012)

Further research findings

Murphy, Jako & Anhalt (1993) argue: "Since 1980, there have been a large number of studies dealing directly or indirectly with halo error in rating. Taken together, these studies suggest that all seven of the characteristics that have defined halo error for much of its history are problematic and that the assumptions that underlie some of them are demonstrably wrong." Their work claims that the assumption that the halo effect is always detrimental is incorrect, with some halo effects resulting in an increase in the accuracy of the rating, in their opinion. Additionally, they discuss the idea of "true halo"—the actual correlation between, for example, attractiveness and performance as an instructor—and "illusory halo" that refers to cognitive distortions, errors in observation and judgement, and the rating tendencies of the individual rater. They claim that any true differentiation between true and illusory halos is impossible in a real-world setting, because the different ratings are strongly influenced by the specific behaviors of the person observed by the raters.

A study by Forgas (2011) states that one's mood can affect the degree of the halo effect's influence. When someone is in a favorable mood, the halo effect is more likely to be influential—this was demonstrated by study participants choosing between pictures of an elderly man with a beard and a young woman, and deciding which subject possessed more philosophical attributes. Additionally, when asked to list the happy, neutral, or negative times in their life, the halo effect was more evident in the perceptions of the participants who chose to write about happy prior experiences. Forgas's study suggests that when one is gauging the extent of the halo effect in a situation, one must consider the emotional state of the person making the judgment.

A 2013 report on "the link between disease and leader preferences" claimed that "congressional districts with a higher incidence of disease" were more likely to show a halo effect "on electoral outcomes." [65]

See also

Notes

  1. related to Net Present Value
  2. The loss of recency is compensated from the effective frequency of advertising expenditures of prior periods.
  3. The textbook has been revised more than once, and the mainframe program from 1966 is now a PC program.
  4. The Times was referring to F. D. Roosevelt and Ronald Reagan

Related Research Articles

Bias is a disproportionate weight in favor of or against an idea or thing, usually in a way that is inaccurate, closed-minded, prejudicial, or unfair. Biases can be innate or learned. People may develop biases for or against an individual, a group, or a belief. In science and engineering, a bias is a systematic error. Statistical bias results from an unfair sampling of a population, or from an estimation process that does not give accurate results on average.

Actor–observer asymmetry is a bias one makes when forming attributions about the behavior of others or themselves. When people judge their own behavior, they are more likely to attribute their actions to the particular situation than to their personality. However, when an observer is explaining the behavior of another person, they are more likely to attribute this behavior to the actors' personality rather than to situational factors.

In psychology, an attribution bias or attributional errors is a cognitive bias that refers to the systematic errors made when people evaluate or try to find reasons for their own and others' behaviors. It refers to the systematic patterns of deviation from norm or rationality in judgment, often leading to perceptual distortions, inaccurate assessments, or illogical interpretations of events and behaviors.

The physical attractiveness stereotype, commonly known as the "beautiful-is-good" stereotype, is the tendency to assume that physically attractive individuals, coinciding with social beauty standards, also possess other desirable personality traits, such as intelligence, social competence, and morality. The target benefits from what has been coined as “pretty privilege”, namely social, economic, and political advantages or benefits. Physical attractiveness can have a significant effect on how people are judged in terms of employment or social opportunities, friendship, sexual behavior, and marriage.

Depressive realism is the hypothesis developed by Lauren Alloy and Lyn Yvonne Abramson that depressed individuals make more realistic inferences than non-depressed individuals. Although depressed individuals are thought to have a negative cognitive bias that results in recurrent, negative automatic thoughts, maladaptive behaviors, and dysfunctional world beliefs, depressive realism argues not only that this negativity may reflect a more accurate appraisal of the world but also that non-depressed individuals' appraisals are positively biased.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Job interview</span> Type of interview

A job interview is an interview consisting of a conversation between a job applicant and a representative of an employer which is conducted to assess whether the applicant should be hired. Interviews are one of the most common methods of employee selection. Interviews vary in the extent to which the questions are structured, from an unstructured and informal conversation to a structured interview in which an applicant is asked a predetermined list of questions in a specified order; structured interviews are usually more accurate predictors of which applicants will make suitable employees, according to research studies.

Optimism bias or optimistic bias is a cognitive bias that causes someone to believe that they themselves are less likely to experience a negative event. It is also known as unrealistic optimism or comparative optimism.

Implicit personality theory describes the specific patterns and biases an individual uses when forming impressions based on a limited amount of initial information about an unfamiliar person. While there are parts of the impression formation process that are context-dependent, individuals also tend to exhibit certain tendencies in forming impressions across a variety of situations. There is not one singular implicit personality theory utilized by all; rather, each individual approaches the task of impression formation in his or her own unique way. However, there are some components of implicit personality theories that are consistent across individuals, or within groups of similar individuals. These components are of particular interest to social psychologists because they have the potential to give insight into what impression one person will form of another.

The negativity bias, also known as the negativity effect, is a cognitive bias that, even when positive or neutral things of equal intensity occur, things of a more negative nature have a greater effect on one's psychological state and processes than neutral or positive things. In other words, something very positive will generally have less of an impact on a person's behavior and cognition than something equally emotional but negative. The negativity bias has been investigated within many different domains, including the formation of impressions and general evaluations; attention, learning, and memory; and decision-making and risk considerations.

Social perception is the study of how people form impressions of and make inferences about other people as sovereign personalities. Social perception refers to identifying and utilizing social cues to make judgments about social roles, rules, relationships, context, or the characteristics of others. This domain also includes social knowledge, which refers to one's knowledge of social roles, norms, and schemas surrounding social situations and interactions. People learn about others' feelings and emotions by picking up information they gather from physical appearance, verbal, and nonverbal communication. Facial expressions, tone of voice, hand gestures, and body position or movement are a few examples of ways people communicate without words. A real-world example of social perception is understanding that others disagree with what one said when one sees them roll their eyes. There are four main components of social perception: observation, attribution, integration, and confirmation.

The Dr. Fox effect is a correlation observed between teacher expressiveness, content coverage, student evaluation and student achievement. This effect also allows insight to other related effects and relationships between student achievement and evaluations of the teacher.

In social psychology, illusory superiority is a cognitive bias wherein people overestimate their own qualities and abilities compared to others. Illusory superiority is one of many positive illusions, relating to the self, that are evident in the study of intelligence, the effective performance of tasks and tests, and the possession of desirable personal characteristics and personality traits. Overestimation of abilities compared to an objective measure is known as the overconfidence effect.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Negative affectivity</span> Personal tendency towards negative emotions and poor self-concept

In psychology, negative affectivity (NA), or negative affect, is a personality variable that involves the experience of negative emotions and poor self-concept. Negative affectivity subsumes a variety of negative emotions, including anger, contempt, disgust, guilt, fear, and nervousness. Low negative affectivity is characterized by frequent states of calmness and serenity, along with states of confidence, activeness, and great enthusiasm.

Thin-slicing is a term used in psychology and philosophy to describe the ability to find patterns in events based only on "thin slices", or narrow windows, of experience. The term refers to the process of making very quick inferences about the state, characteristics or details of an individual or situation with minimal amounts of information. Research has found that brief judgments based on thin-slicing are similar to those judgments based on much more information. Judgments based on thin-slicing can be as accurate, or even more so, than judgments based on much more information.

Impression formation in social psychology refers to the processes by which different pieces of knowledge about another are combined into a global or summary impression. Social psychologist Solomon Asch is credited with the seminal research on impression formation and conducted research on how individuals integrate information about personality traits. Two major theories have been proposed to explain how this process of integration takes place. The Gestalt approach views the formation of a general impression as the sum of several interrelated impressions. As an individual seeks to form a coherent and meaningful impression of another individual, previous impressions significantly influence the interpretation of subsequent information. In contrast to the Gestalt approach, the cognitive algebra approach asserts that individuals' experiences are combined with previous evaluations to form a constantly changing impression of a person. A related area to impression formation is the study of person perception, making dispositional attributions, and then adjusting those inferences based on the information available.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Assimilation and contrast effects</span>

The assimilation effect, assimilation bias or biased assimilation is a bias in evaluative judgments towards the position of a context stimulus, while contrast effects describe a negative correlation between a judgment and contextual information.

In psychology, a first impression is the event when one person first encounters another person and forms a mental image of that person. Impression accuracy varies depending on the observer and the target being observed. First impressions are based on a wide range of characteristics: age, race, culture, language, gender, physical appearance, accent, posture, voice, number of people present, economic status, and time allowed to process. The first impressions individuals give to others could greatly influence how they are treated and viewed in many contexts of everyday life.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Cheerleader effect</span> Psychological effect on perceptions of attractiveness

The cheerleader effect, also known as the group attractiveness effect or the friend effect, is a proposed cognitive bias which causes people to perceive individuals as 1.5–2.0% more attractive in a group than when seen alone. The first paper to report this effect was written by Drew Walker and Edward Vul, in 2013.

The horn effect, closely related to the halo effect, is a form of cognitive bias that causes one's perception of another to be unduly influenced by a single negative trait. An example of the horn effect may be that an observer is more likely to assume a physically unattractive person is morally inferior to an attractive person, despite the lack of relationship between morality and physical appearance.

References

  1. "Halo Effect | Definition of Halo Effect by Lexico". Archived from the original on August 2, 2017.
  2. Al Ries (17 Apr 2006). "Understanding Marketing Psychology and the Halo Effect". Advertising Age . Crain Publications. Retrieved 2017-07-31.
  3. Bethel, Ann; Knapp, T (2010). "Halo Effect". Dictionary of Nursing and Research. 4th via Credo Reference.
  4. 1 2 3 4 Thorndike 1920
  5. Nisbett, Richard E.; Wilson, Timothy D. (1977). "The halo effect: Evidence for unconscious alteration of judgments". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 35 (4): 250–256. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.35.4.250. hdl: 2027.42/92158 . S2CID   17867385.
  6. 1 2 3 4 Gibson, Jeremy L.; Gore, Jonathan S. (December 2016). "Is He a Hero or a Weirdo? How Norm Violations Influence the Halo Effect". Gender Issues. 33 (4): 299–310. doi:10.1007/s12147-016-9173-6. S2CID   151849084.
  7. 1 2 Lachman, Sheldon J.; Bass, Alan R. (November 1985). "A Direct Study of Halo Effect". The Journal of Psychology. 119 (6): 535–540. doi:10.1080/00223980.1985.9915460.
  8. Wade, T. Joel; DiMaria, Cristina (1 May 2003). "Weight Halo Effects: Individual Differences in Perceived Life Success as a Function of Women's Race and Weight". Sex Roles. 48 (9): 461–465. doi:10.1023/A:1023582629538. S2CID   141143275.
  9. 1 2 Greenwald, Anthony G.; Banaji, Mahzarin R. (1995). "Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes". Psychological Review. 102 (1): 4–27. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.411.2919 . doi:10.1037/0033-295x.102.1.4. PMID   7878162. S2CID   8194189.
  10. Levy, Leon H.; Dugan, Robert D. (July 1960). "A constant error approach to the study of dimensions of social perception". The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 61 (1): 21–24. doi:10.1037/h0042208. PMID   14416418.
  11. Roeckelein, Jon. E (2006). Elsevier's Dictionary of Psychological Theories (1st ed.). Elsevier Science & Technology.
  12. 1 2 McCornack, Steven. Choices & Connections (2nd ed.).
  13. 1 2 Daniel Kahneman (2013). Thinking, fast and slow (1st ed.). New York: Farrar, Straus, and Giroux. pp. 82–88. ISBN   978-0-374-53355-7.
  14. 1 2 Philip M. Rosenzweig (2014). The Halo Effect and the Eight Other Business Delusions That Deceive Managers. New York, NY: Free Press. ISBN   978-1-4767-8403-8.
  15. Zhang, Zhongheng; Poucke, Sven Van (2017-01-03). "Citations for Randomized Controlled Trials in Sepsis Literature: The Halo Effect Caused by Journal Impact Factor". PLOS ONE. 12 (1): e0169398. Bibcode:2017PLoSO..1269398Z. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0169398 . PMC   5207738 . PMID   28046105.
  16. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Sigall, Harold; Ostrove, Nancy (1975-03-01). "Beautiful but Dangerous: Effects of Offender Attractiveness and Nature of the Crime on Juridic Judgment". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 31 (3): 410–414. doi:10.1037/h0076472.
  17. "Halo Effect". 27 October 2008. Retrieved 2017-12-09.
  18. "Apple shares surfs on big profits". BBC News. 13 January 2005. Retrieved 18 January 2012.
  19. "Apple puts iPod halo to test with Shuffle and Mini". Advertising Age . 17 January 2005.
  20. Anthony Crupi (July 16, 2019). "For advertisers, the Emmy halo effect is a thing of the past". Advertising Age .
  21. Melanie Abrams (August 1, 2017). "The Wonder Woman Effect". The New York Times .
  22. Frank, Michael (31 August 2012). "What good is a halo car anyway?". Popular Mechanics. Hearst Communications, Inc. Retrieved 12 December 2018.
  23. Rowson, Paul; Thompson, Howard; Berry, Julian (1 June 2012). "Using a decision support optimisation software tool to maximise returns from an overall marketing budget: A case study from a B-to-C marketing company". Journal of Database Marketing & Customer Strategy Management. 19 (2): 138–142. doi: 10.1057/dbm.2012.10 . S2CID   167892664.
  24. Timothy Coombs, W.; Holladay, Sherry J. (April 2006). "Unpacking the halo effect: reputation and crisis management". Journal of Communication Management. 10 (2): 123–137. doi:10.1108/13632540610664698.
  25. Provencher, Véronique; Jacob, Raphaëlle (March 2016). "Impact of Perceived Healthiness of Food on Food Choices and Intake". Current Obesity Reports. 5 (1): 65–71. doi:10.1007/s13679-016-0192-0. hdl: 20.500.11794/13474 . PMID   26820622. S2CID   207474222.
  26. Polivy, Janet (December 2017). "What's that you're eating? Social comparison and eating behavior". Journal of Eating Disorders. 5 (1): 18. doi: 10.1186/s40337-017-0148-0 . PMC   5408479 . PMID   28465828.
  27. Nathan Jeffray (24 June 2010). "Interview: Gerald Steinberg". The Jewish Chronicle.
  28. Naftali Balanson (8 October 2008). "The 'halo effect' shields NGOs from media scrutiny". The Jerusalem Post.
  29. Nancy Jones. "Corporate Donors". Ronald House Durham. Archived from the original on 3 December 2013. Retrieved 26 November 2013.
  30. Coombs, Timothy W; Holladay, Sherry J (2006). "Unpacking the halo effect: reputation and crisis management". Journal of Communication Management. 10 (2): 123–37. doi:10.1108/13632540610664698.
  31. Klein, Jill; Dawar, Niraj (2004). "Evaluations in a Product-Harm Crisis". International Journal of Research in Marketing. 21 (3): 203–17. doi:10.1016/j.ijresmar.2003.12.003.
  32. Nancy Borkowski (2015). Organizational Behavior, Theory, and Design in Health Care. Burlington, MA: Jones & Bartlett Publishers. p. 63. ISBN   978-1-284-05088-2.
  33. Phil Rosenzweig (2014). The Halo Effect: . . . and the Eight Other Business Delusions That Deceive Managers. New York: Free Press. pp. xv. ISBN   978-0-7432-9125-5.
  34. "Do Metal Detectors Work Better on Wet Ground?".
  35. 1 2 Guha, Martin (December 2006). "Elsevier's Dictionary of Psychological Theories2006405Compiled by J.E. Roeckelein. Elsevier's Dictionary of Psychological Theories . Amsterdam: Elsevier 2006. xii+679 pp. £90; $143, ISBN: 0 444 51750 2". Reference Reviews. 20 (8): 10–11. doi:10.1108/09504120610709402.
  36. 1 2 Longe, Jacqueline (2016). The Gale Encyclopedia of Psychology. pp. 507–509.
  37. 1 2 3 Edward, Craighead (2004). "Rater Errors". The Concise Corsini Encyclopedia of Psychology and Behavioral Science.
  38. Thorndike (1920), p. 27.
  39. Jeni Mcray (2015). "Cognitive bias." Leadership Glossary: Essential Terms for the 21st Century (1st ed.). Mission Bell Media.
  40. Ellis, Geoffrey (2018). Cognitive Biases in Visualizations. Cham, Switzerland: Springer. p. 168. ISBN   978-3-319-95830-9.
  41. Horns and halo effect, The Free Dictionary, retrieved September 30, 2015
  42. 1 2 LeClaire, Anne (2017). The Halo Effect. Lake Union Publishing.
  43. Wade, T Joel; DiMaria, Cristina (2003). "Weight Halo Effects: Individual Differences in Perceived Life Success as a Function of Women's Race and Weight". Sex Roles. 48 (9/10): 461–465. doi:10.1023/A:1023582629538. S2CID   141143275.
  44. 1 2 Lasky, Jack (2020). "Halo Effect". Salem Press Encyclopedia of Health.
  45. SMES, HR for (2021-01-19). "How to avoid the Halo Effect in your company's decissions". HR for Small and Medium businesses. Archived from the original on 2021-06-24. Retrieved 2021-06-18.
  46. 1 2 Dion, Karen K. (1990). "Citations Classics - Commentary on 'What is beautiful is good' (1972)" (PDF). Current Contents. ISI.
  47. "Halo Effect". The New York Times . January 30, 1982.
  48. Surawski, Melissa K.; Ossoff, Elizabeth P. (March 2006). "The effects of physical and vocal attractiveness on impression formation of politicians". Current Psychology. 25 (1): 15–27. doi:10.1007/s12144-006-1013-5. S2CID   144978466.
  49. Herrmann, Michael; Shikano, Susumu (2016). "Attractiveness and Facial Competence Bias Face-Based Inferences of Candidate Ideology". Political Psychology. 37 (3): 401–417. doi:10.1111/pops.12256.
  50. Verhulst, Brad; Lodge, Milton; Lavine, Howard (June 2010). "The Attractiveness Halo: Why Some Candidates are Perceived More Favorably than Others". Journal of Nonverbal Behavior. 34 (2): 111–117. doi:10.1007/s10919-009-0084-z. S2CID   37559811.
  51. Poutvaara, Panu; Jordahl, Henrik; Berggren, Niclas (September 2009). "Faces of politicians: Babyfacedness predicts inferred competence but not electoral success". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 45 (5): 1132–1135. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2009.06.007. hdl: 10419/81455 . S2CID   3228508.
  52. Berggren, Niclas; Jordahl, Henrik; Poutvaara, Panu (February 2017). "The right look: Conservative politicians look better and voters reward it". Journal of Public Economics. 146: 79–86. doi: 10.1016/j.jpubeco.2016.12.008 . hdl: 10419/81356 .
  53. Dermer, Marshall; Thiel, Darrel L. (1975). "When beauty may fail". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 31 (6): 1168–1176. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.552.3491 . doi:10.1037/h0077085.
  54. Kaplan, Robert M. (1 April 1978). "Is beauty talent? Sex interaction in the attractiveness halo effect". Sex Roles. 4 (2): 195–204. doi:10.1007/BF00287500. S2CID   143581604.
  55. Moore, F. R.; Filippou, D.; Perrett, D. I. (September 2011). "Intelligence and attractiveness in the face: Beyond the attractiveness halo effect". Journal of Evolutionary Psychology. 9 (3): 205–217. doi: 10.1556/jep.9.2011.3.2 .
  56. 1 2 Feeley, Thomas Hugh (October 2002). "Comment on Halo Effects in Rating and Evaluation Research". Human Communication Research. 28 (4): 578–586. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.2002.tb00825.x.
  57. 1 2 Gräf, Michael; Unkelbach, Christian (August 2018). "Halo effects from agency behaviors and communion behaviors depend on social context: Why technicians benefit more from showing tidiness than nurses do: Halo effects depend on social context". European Journal of Social Psychology. 48 (5): 701–717. doi:10.1002/ejsp.2353.
  58. Eagly, Alice H.; Ashmore, Richard D.; Makhijani, Mona G.; Longo, Laura C. (1991). "What is beautiful is good, but . . .: A meta-analytic review of research on the physical attractiveness stereotype". Psychological Bulletin. 110 (1): 109–128. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.110.1.109.
  59. Forgas, Joseph P. (December 2011). "She just doesn't look like a philosopher…? Affective influences on the halo effect in impression formation: Mood and halo effects". European Journal of Social Psychology. 41 (7): 812–817. doi:10.1002/ejsp.842.
  60. "Halo and horns effects in rating errors". 2010.
  61. "Mental Model: Horns Effect and Halo Effect". 12 November 2011.
  62. Kurian, George Thomas (2013). The AMA Dictionary of Business and Management (1st ed.). Publishing Division o f the American Management Association.
  63. Richard E Nisbett; Timothy D Wilson (1977). "The halo effect: Evidence for unconscious alteration of judgments". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 35 (4): 250–56. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.35.4.250. hdl: 2027.42/92158 . S2CID   17867385.
  64. Glennie, Jonathan (3 May 2011). "Hugo Chávez's reverse-halo effect". The Guardian.
  65. Andrew Edward White; Douglas T. Kenrick (November 1, 2013). "Why Attractive Candidates Win". The New York Times .

Bibliography

Further reading