Paternal care

Last updated

In biology, paternal care is parental investment provided by a male to his own offspring. It is a complex social behaviour in vertebrates associated with animal mating systems, life history traits, and ecology. [1] Paternal care may be provided in concert with the mother (biparental care) or, more rarely, by the male alone (so called exclusive paternal care).

Contents

The provision of care, by either males or females, is presumed to increase growth rates, quality, and/or survival of young, and hence ultimately increase the inclusive fitness of parents. [2] [3] [4] In a variety of vertebrate species (e.g., about 80% of birds [5] and about 6% of mammals), [6] both males and females invest heavily in their offspring. Many of these biparental species are socially monogamous, so individuals remain with their mate for at least one breeding season.

Exclusive paternal care has evolved multiple times in a variety of organisms, including invertebrates, fishes, and amphibians. [7] [8] [9]

Mammals

Pack of African wild dogs (Lycaon pictus) resting. A mammal in which males remain as care helpers. African Wild Dog (Lycaon pictus pictus) (21513301410).jpg
Pack of African wild dogs ( Lycaon pictus ) resting. A mammal in which males remain as care helpers.

Male mammals employ different behaviors to enhance their reproductive success (e.g. courtship displays, mate choice). However, the benefits of paternal care have rarely been studied in mammals, largely because only 5-10% of mammals exhibit such care (mostly present in primates, rodents and canids). [11] [12] In those species in which males provide extensive care for their offspring, indirect evidence suggests that its costs can be substantial. [13] [14] For example, mammalian fathers that care for their young may undergo changes in body mass and an increase in production of a number of costly hormones (e.g. androgens, glucocorticoids, leptin). [15] [16] [17] Nonetheless, there is evidence that suggest that across all mammals, when males carry and groom their offspring their female partner fecundity increases, and if males provision the females, their litter size tend to be larger. [18]

Humans

Human cultures and societies vary widely in the expression of paternal care. Some cultures recognize paternal care via celebration of Father's Day. Human paternal care is a derived characteristic (evolved in humans or our recent ancestors) and one of the defining characteristics of Homo sapiens . [19] Different aspects of human paternal care (direct, indirect, fostering social or moral development) may have evolved at different points in our history, and together they form a unique suite of behaviors as compared with the great apes. One study of humans has found evidence suggesting a possible evolutionary trade-off between mating success and parenting involvement; specifically, fathers with smaller testes tend to be more involved in care of their children. [20]

Research on the effects of paternal care on human happiness have yielded conflicting results. However, one recent study concluded that fathers generally report higher levels of happiness, positive emotion, and meaning in life as compared with non-fathers. [21]

According to the United States Census Bureau, approximately one third of children in the U.S. grow up without their biological father in their home. Numerous studies have documented negative consequences of being raised in a home that lacks a father, including increased likelihood of living in poverty, having behavioral problems, committing crimes, spending time in prison, abusing drugs or alcohol, becoming obese, and dropping out of school. [22] [23]

Non-human primates

In non-human primates, paternal investment is often dependent on the type of mating system exhibited by each species. Mating systems influence paternity certainty and the likelihood that a male is providing care towards his own biological offspring. Paternal certainty is high in monogamous pair-bonded species and males are less likely to be at risk for caring for unrelated offspring and not contributing to their own fitness. [24] [25] In contrast, polygamous primate societies create paternity uncertainty and males are more at risk of providing care for unrelated offspring and compromising their own fitness. [26] [27] Paternal care by male non-human primates motivated by biological paternity utilize past mating history and phenotypic matching in order to recognize their own offspring. [28] [29] Comparing male care efforts exhibited by the same species can provide insight on the significant relationship between paternity certainty and the amount of paternal care exhibited by a male. For example, Siamangs (Symphalangus syndactylus) utilize both polyandrous and monogamous mating systems but, it was found that monogamous males are more likely to carry infants and contribute to parental duties compared to those in promiscuous mating systems. [30] Studies in Primatology have used primate mating systems and social organization to help theorize the evolutionary significance of paternal care in Primates.

Strepsirrhines

Ring tailed Lemur Lenur At Monkey World (190899369).jpeg
Ring tailed Lemur

Strepsirrhini is a suborder of the order Primates and includes lemurs, lorises, and bush babies. In this sub-order, males exhibit the lowest levels of paternal care for infants among primates. [25] Examples of observed male care in this group include playing, grooming, and occasionally transporting infants. Males have also been observed interacting with infants while mothers park them and temporarily leave in order to feed. [31] [25] When female strepsirrhines park or nest their infants in nearby trees, males frequently use this as an opportunity to play with the unattended infants. [31] In this suborder, male care and affection is directed toward multiple infants including non-biological offspring, and young strepsirrhines can be found interacting with various males. [25] Paternal care does not influence infant growth rates or shorten inter-birth intervals of mothers as it can in haplorrhines. [31] Strepsirrhini males exhibit the lowest intensity of care towards infants in non-human primates.

Strepsirrhines are constrained by their life history traits and reproductive rates are not flexible within this group of primates. This group of primates are programmed to give birth when food is abundant resulting in strict seasonal breeding periods. [31] Shortening inter-birth intervals, which is theorized to be a possible outcome of increased male care, is not beneficial for Strepsirrhine mothers and can decrease infant survival. [31] Studies also show that paternity can be highly skewed in Strepsirrhines, with only one or few male members being the only biological father within a single group. [32] Instead of relying on a singular paternal figure, female mothers in this group rely on alloparenting from other group members. Infant parking and strict reproductive schedules are more beneficial for successful infant development in Strepsirrhines.

Haplorrhines

Chimpanzee infant Chimpanzee VIII (13945323981).jpg
Chimpanzee infant

Haplorhini, a sub-order of the order Primate, includes tarsiers, New World Monkeys, Old World monkeys, apes, and humans.[ citation needed ] Haplorrhini is broken into two sister groups which are commonly distinguished by the characteristic of the primate nose: Catarrhini (narrow turned down nose) and Platyrrhini (flat nose). Paternal care is highly variable between the two sister groups and the species within them.[ citation needed ]

Catarrhines

Catarrhini is composed of Old World Monkeys (Cercopithecidae) and apes (Hylobatidae and Hominoidea).[ citation needed ] These primates are geographically located in Africa, Asia, and Madagascar.

Cercopithecines, the largest primate family, include primates species such as baboons, macaques, colobus, and vervet monkeys.[ citation needed ]

Apes consist of species of gibbons, siamangs, bonobos, chimpanzees, gorillas, orangutans and humans.[ citation needed ]

Catarrhines (non-human) are often organized into a multimale-multifemale social systems and utilize polygamous mating systems which results in paternity uncertainty. It is predicted that males in promiscuous mating systems do not engage in infant care due to the high costs of caring for an infant and missing opportunities to mate with receptive females. [27] Male care in this group of primates is often portrayed through actions such as grooming, carrying, tolerance of the infant, as well as protection against agonistic interactions and infanticide. [33] [27] High ranking males can also provide access to food for developing infants. [34] Direct care such as grooming and playing is not as common compared to male intervention on behalf of the infant when it is being harassed by conspecifics. [27]

Baboon and her infant Olive baboon (Papio anubis) with juvenile.jpg
Baboon and her infant

In Cercopiths, male involvement in the infant's interactions with others is common in many species of baboons but between species paternal care is not always biased towards biological offspring. Male Savannah baboons (Papio cynocephalus) direct care towards their own biological offspring. [35] [33] Males in this species are more likely to intervene and protect infants from harassment against other group members when the infant is predicted to be their own. Studies have shown that male Savannah baboons selectively choose to remain in closer proximity to their own offspring and engage in long-term investment beyond early infancy, when the infant is at greatest risk for infanticide. [36] [33] Infants receiving paternal investment in Savannah baboons have shown enhanced fitness and accelerated maturation through males creating a safe zone for infants to exist in. [35] [36] Similarly to Savannah Baboons, Yellow baboon (Papio cynocephalus) males provide elevated care for their own offspring. Long-term care and investment beyond early infancy is better linked to paternity in this species and affecting infant growth and development. [28] Male baboons also direct care towards unrelated offspring based on male affiliations with female mothers. Baboon males and females within a social group often exhibit “friendships” with females which begin during birth of her infant and has been observed to end abruptly if the infant dies. [37] [26] [33] Males establish associations with females in which they have previously mated resulting in affiliative behaviour and protection towards her offspring. Relationships created by male and female members are significant for infant survival in Chacma baboons (Papio ursinus) because the risk of infanticide in early infancy is higher in this species. [37] [26] Paternal care in the form of protection for the infant is therefore more beneficial than long term investment in Chacma baboons and is believed to be directed towards both biological and non-biological infants in the group.

Rhesus Macaque 20171128 - Macaca fascicularis - Angkor Wat - 5758 DxO.jpg
Rhesus Macaque

Similarly to baboons, paternal roles and the underlying mechanisms as to why paternal care evolved vary within macaque species. In Sulawesi crested macaques (Macaca nigra) both male rank and the relationship to the mother predicted male care towards an infant instead of true biological paternity. [27] In both Sulawesi and Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus) males adopted a “care-then-mate” strategy, in which care is provided to infants regardless of paternity in order for the male to increase future mating opportunities with the mother. [28] [27] In both species, it was observed that male macaques are more likely to initiate care towards and positively interact with the infant in the presence of the mother. [27] In Assamese macaques (Macaca assamensis) biological paternity was the most significant predictor of male affiliations with infants and therefore males biased care towards infants presumed to be their own. [38] [27] Observers found that Assamese males were more likely to engage and provide care for infants in the absence of their mothers reducing the likelihood that care provided to infants will impress the mother and secure access to mating possibilities. [38] In Rhesus macaques, male's providing protection and greater access to food resulted in higher weight gain for both male and female infants. [34] This had a positive effect on infant survival and was significant in the first year of infancy when the risk of infanticide is the highest. [27] [34]

Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) are organized into fission-fusion social groups and provide an example of a polygamous mating society. Male chimpanzees often engage with infants in the form of grooming, playing, and providing protection towards other group members. In both Western and Eastern chimpanzees it was found that males were more likely to engage with their own biological offspring meaning that male care is directed by paternity in this species. [28] [29] In both chimpanzee and bonobo social groups, high ranking alpha males sire approximately half of the offspring within their social group. [39] [40] More research needs to be done addressing how reproductive skew affects paternal care and infant-male relationships in non-human primates including chimpanzees and bonobos.

Platyrrhines

Titi Monkey Goeldi's marmoset, Smithsonian National Zoo.jpg
Titi Monkey

Platyrrhini is a sub-order of the order Primate and are commonly referred to as the New World Monkeys. [41] These primates occupy Central and South America, and Mexico. This group is broken into five families, range in body size, and include species such as spider monkeys, capuchins, and howler monkeys.

Among primate species, the highest levels of male care found in New World monkeys are observed in Owl monkeys (Aotus azarai ) and Titi monkeys (Callicebus caligatus). In both of these species, males and females are monogamous, pair-bonded, and exhibit bi-parental care for their offspring. [25] [42] [24] The social group in both these species consists of female and male parents along with their offspring. [43] [24] Males in these species serve as the primary caregivers and play a major role in infant survival. [24]

Male Titi monkeys are more involved than the mother in all aspects of male care except nursing, and engage in more social activities such as grooming, food sharing, play, and transportation of the infant. [42] [24] The bond between an infant and its father is established right after birth and maintained into adolescence making the father the infant's predominant attachment figure. Similarly, the male Owl monkey acts as the main caregiver and is crucial to the survival of his offspring. If a female gives birth to twins, the male is still responsible for transporting both the infants. [24] In the absence of a father, infant mortality increases in both these species and it is unlikely that the infant will survive. One study found that the replacement of a male enacting as the role of the father resulted in higher mortality during infancy emphasizing the importance of the social bond created between father and offspring at birth. [25]

White-faced Capuchin Capuchin monkey (13434730224).jpg
White-faced Capuchin

In White‐faced Capuchins (Cebus capucinus) one study found that parental care was exhibited in the form of playful behaviour, proximity to, inspection of, and collecting discarded food items from infants as determined by male rank and dominance status rather than biological relatedness to the infant. [44] Scientists believe that future research on kin recognition needs to be done on capuchins to determine if males choose to bias their care as well as in other non-human primates relying on phenotypic matching to distinguish biological offspring. [28] [44]

Evolutionary Perspectives on Paternal Care in Primates

Squirrel monkeys Squirrelmonkfamily1.jpg
Squirrel monkeys

The Theory of Paternal Investment: Differences in infant care between sexes stems from females investing more time and energy in their offspring than males, while males compete with one another for access to females. [27] Although paternal care is rare among mammalians, males across many primate species still play a paternal role in infant care.

Hypotheses addressing the rise of paternal care in several primate species

The Paternal Care hypothesis: Paternal care and investment will be designated to biological offspring, increasing the infant's chance of survival, and therefore increasing the male's own fitness. [33] [27] This hypothesis requires the on male to use recognition and behavioural cues to distinguish their own offspring from other infants. [29] Paternal uncertainty is high in multimale-multifemale primate groups so males must use these cues to recognize and bias care towards their own offspring. This allows males to provide both short and long-term investment for infants. [26] Primates living in monogamous pairs or single-male groups exhibit high paternity certainty and assist with the Paternal Care hypothesis.

The Mating Effort hypothesis: Males provide care for infants in order to increase mating opportunities with females. [27] [38] This means that males are more likely to engage in affiliative behaviours with the infant in the presence of the mother as a form of male mating effort in order to enhance future reproductive success. [27] This theory is independent of genetics and evolved independent of paternity.

The Maternal Relief hypothesis: Males provide care infants to help reduce reproductive burdens of the female, ultimately resulting shorter inter-birth intervals and more successful offspring. [27] This stems from the male alleviating the female from her parental duties in order to keep her resources from becoming depleted and subsequently allowing her to produce high quality milk for the infant. [25] Similarly to the mating effort hypothesis, the maternal relief hypothesis is independent of genetics and does not require the male to be the biological father to take part in infant care.

Rodents

California mice (Peromyscus californicus) are well known for have intensive and sustained paternal behavior. California mouse 5 Peromyscus californicus.jpg
California mice ( Peromyscus californicus ) are well known for have intensive and sustained paternal behavior.

Several species of rodents have been studied as models of paternal care, including prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster), Campbell's dwarf hamster, the Mongolian gerbil, and the African striped mouse. The California mouse (Peromyscus californicus) is a monogamous rodent that exhibits extensive and essential paternal care, and hence has been studied as a model organism for this phenomenon. [45] [46] One study of this species found that fathers had larger hindlimb muscles than did non-breeding males. [14] Quantitative genetic analysis has identified several genomic regions that affect paternal care. [47]

Birds

Fathers contribute equally with mothers to the care of offspring in as many as 90% of bird species, sometimes including incubating the eggs. Most paternal care is associated with biparental care in socially monogamous mating systems (about 81% of species), but in approximately 1% of species, fathers provide all care after eggs are laid. [5] The unusually high incidence of paternal care in birds compared to other vertebrate taxa is often assumed to stem from the extensive resource requirements for production of flight-capable offspring. By contrast, in bats (the other extant flying vertebrate lineage), care of offspring is provided by females (although males may help guard pups in some species [48] ). In contrast to the large clutch sizes found in many bird species with biparental care, bats typically produce single offspring, which may be a limitation related to lack of male help. It has been suggested, though not without controversy, that paternal care is the ancestral form of parental care in birds. [9]

Amphibians

Paternal care occurs in a number of species of anuran amphibians, [49] including glass frogs.

Fish

According to the Encyclopedia of Fish Physiology: From Genome to Environment:

About 30% of the 500 known fish families show some form of parental care, and most often (78% of the time) care is provided by only one parent (usually the male). Male care (50%) is much more common than female care (30%) with biparental care accounting for about 20%, although a more recent comparative analysis suggests that male care may be more common (84%). [50]

There are three common theoretical explanations for the high levels of paternal care in fish, with the third one currently favoured. First, external fertilization protects against paternity loss; however, sneaker tactics and strong sperm competition have evolved many times. Second, the earlier release of eggs than sperm gives females an opportunity to flee; however, in many paternal care species, eggs and sperm are released simultaneously. Third, if a male is already protecting a valuable spawning territory in order to attract females, defending young adds minimal parental investment, giving males a lower relative cost of parental care. [50]

One well-known example of paternal care is in seahorses, where males brood the eggs in a brood pouch until they are ready to hatch.

Males from the Centrarchidae (sunfish) family exhibit paternal parental care of their eggs and fry through a variety of behaviors such as nest guarding and nest fanning (aerating eggs). [51]

In jawfish, the female lays the eggs and the male then takes them in his mouth. A male can have up to 400 eggs in his mouth at one time. The male can't feed while he hosts the young, but as the young get older, they spend more time out of the mouth. [52] This is sometimes termed mouthbrooding.

During the breeding season, male three-spined sticklebacks defend nesting territories. Males attract females to spawn in their nests and defend their breeding territory from intruders and predators. After spawning, the female leaves the male's territory and the male is solely responsible for the care of the eggs. During the ~6-day incubation period, the male 'fans' (oxygenates) the eggs, removes rotten eggs and debris, and defends the territory. Even after embryos hatch, father sticklebacks continue to tend their newly hatched offspring for ~7 days, chasing and retrieving fry that stray from the nest and spitting them back into the nest. [53]

Arthropods

Paternal care is rare in arthropods, [54] but occurs in some species, including the giant water bug [55] [56] and the arachnid Iporangaia pustulosa, a harvestman. [57] In several species of crustaceans, males provide care of offspring by building and defending burrows or other nest sites. [58] Exclusive paternal care, where males provide the sole investment after egg-laying, is the rarest form, and is known in only 13 taxa: giant water bugs, sea spiders, two genera of leaf-footed bugs, two genera of assassin bugs, three genera of phlaeothripid thrips, three genera of harvestmen, and in millipedes of the family Andrognathidae. [59]

Theoretical models of the evolution of paternal care

Mathematical models related to the prisoner's dilemma suggest that when female reproductive costs are higher than male reproductive costs, males cooperate with females even when they do not reciprocate. In this view, paternal care is an evolutionary achievement that compensates for the higher energy demands that reproduction typically involves for mothers. [60] [61]

Other models suggest that basic life-history differences between males and females are adequate to explain the evolutionary origins of maternal, paternal, and bi-parental care. Specifically, paternal care is more likely if male adult mortality is high, and maternal care is more likely to evolve if female adult mortality is high. [62] Basic life-history differences between the sexes can also cause evolutionary transitions among different sex-specific patterns of parental care. [63]

Consequences for offspring survival and development

Care by fathers can have important consequences for survival and development of offspring in both humans [64] and other species. Mechanisms underlying such effects may include protecting offspring from predators or environmental extremes (e.g., heat or cold), feeding them or, in some species, direct teaching of skills. Moreover, some studies indicate a potential epigenetic germline inheritance of paternal effects. [65]

The effects of paternal care on offspring can be studied in various ways. One way is to compare species that vary in the degree of paternal care. For example, an extended duration of paternal care occurs in the gentoo penguin, as compared with other Pygoscelis species. It was found that their fledging period, the time between a chick's first trip to sea and its absolute independence from the group, was longer than other penguins of the same genus. The authors hypothesized that this was because it allowed chicks to better develop their foraging skills before becoming completely independent from their parents. By doing so, a chick may have a higher chance of survival and increase the population's overall fitness. [66]

Proximate mechanisms

The proximate mechanisms of paternal care are not well understood for any organism. In vertebrates, at the level of hormonal control, vasopressin apparently underlies the neurochemical basis of paternal care; prolactin and testosterone may also be involved. As with other behaviors that affect Darwinian fitness, reward pathways [67] in the brain may reinforce the expression of paternal care and may be involved in the formation of attachment bonds.

The mechanisms that underlie the onset of parental behaviors in female mammals have been characterized in a variety of species. In mammals, females undergo endocrine changes during gestation and lactation that "prime" mothers to respond maternally towards their offspring. [68] [69]

Paternal males do not undergo these same hormonal changes and so the proximate causes of the onset of parental behaviors must differ from those in females. There is little consensus regarding the processes by which mammalian males begin to express parental behaviors. [16] In humans, evidence ties oxytocin to sensitive care-giving in both women and men, and with affectionate infant contact in women and stimulatory infant contact in men. In contrast, testosterone decreases in men who become involved fathers and testosterone may interfere with aspects of paternal care. [70]

Placentophagia (the behavior of ingesting the afterbirth after parturition) has been proposed to have physiological consequences that could facilitate a male's responsiveness to offspring [71] [72] [73] [74] Non-genomic transmission of paternal behavior from fathers to their sons has been reported to occur in laboratory studies of the biparental California mouse, but whether this involves (epigenetic) modifications or other mechanisms is not yet known. [75]

See also

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Father</span> Male parent

A father is the male parent of a child. Besides the paternal bonds of a father to his children, the father may have a parental, legal, and social relationship with the child that carries with it certain rights and obligations. A biological father is the male genetic contributor to the creation of the infant, through sexual intercourse or sperm donation. A biological father may have legal obligations to a child not raised by him, such as an obligation of monetary support. An adoptive father is a man who has become the child's parent through the legal process of adoption. A putative father is a man whose biological relationship to a child is alleged but has not been established. A stepfather is a non-biological male parent married to a child's preexisting parent, and may form a family unit but generally does not have the legal rights and responsibilities of a parent in relation to the child.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Olive baboon</span> Also called the Anubis baboon, is a member of the family Cercopithecidae (Old World monkeys)

The olive baboon, also called the Anubis baboon, is a member of the family Cercopithecidae Old World monkeys. The species is the most wide-ranging of all baboons, being native to 25 countries throughout Africa, extending from Mali eastward to Ethiopia and Tanzania. Isolated populations are also present in some mountainous regions of the Sahara. It inhabits savannahs, steppes, and forests. The common name is derived from its coat colour, which is a shade of green-grey at a distance. A variety of communications, vocal and non-vocal, facilitate a complex social structure.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Concealed ovulation</span> Lack of visible changes in ovulating females

Concealed ovulation or hidden estrus in a species is the lack of any perceptible change in an adult female when she is fertile and near ovulation. Some examples of perceptible changes are swelling and redness of the genitalia in baboons and bonobos, and pheromone release in the feline family. In contrast, the females of humans and a few other species that undergo hidden estrus have few external signs of fecundity, making it difficult for a mate to consciously deduce, by means of external signs only, whether or not a female is near ovulation.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Chacma baboon</span> Species of baboon from the Old World monkey family

The chacma baboon, also known as the Cape baboon, is, like all other baboons, from the Old World monkey family. It is one of the largest of all monkeys. Located primarily in southern Africa, the chacma baboon has a wide variety of social behaviours, including a dominance hierarchy, collective foraging, adoption of young by females, and friendship pairings. These behaviors form parts of a complex evolutionary ecology. In general, the species is not threatened, but human population pressure has increased contact between humans and baboons. Hunting, trapping, and accidents kill or remove many baboons from the wild, thereby reducing baboon numbers and disrupting their social structure.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Parental investment</span> Parental expenditure (e.g. time, energy, resources) that benefits offspring

Parental investment, in evolutionary biology and evolutionary psychology, is any parental expenditure that benefits offspring. Parental investment may be performed by both males and females, females alone or males alone. Care can be provided at any stage of the offspring's life, from pre-natal to post-natal.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Pair bond</span> Biological term

In biology, a pair bond is the strong affinity that develops in some species between a mating pair, often leading to the production and rearing of young and potentially a lifelong bond. Pair-bonding is a term coined in the 1940s that is frequently used in sociobiology and evolutionary biology circles. The term often implies either a lifelong socially monogamous relationship or a stage of mating interaction in socially monogamous species. It is sometimes used in reference to human relationships.

Monogamous pairing in animals refers to the natural history of mating systems in which species pair bond to raise offspring. This is associated, usually implicitly, with sexual monogamy.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Reproductive synchrony</span>

Reproductive synchrony is a term used in evolutionary biology and behavioral ecology. Reproductive synchrony—sometimes termed "ovulatory synchrony"—may manifest itself as "breeding seasonality". Where females undergo regular menstruation, "menstrual synchrony" is another possible term.

Extra-pair copulation (EPC) is a mating behaviour in monogamous species. Monogamy is the practice of having only one sexual partner at any one time, forming a long-term bond and combining efforts to raise offspring together; mating outside this pairing is extra-pair copulation. Across the animal kingdom, extra-pair copulation is common in monogamous species, and only a very few pair-bonded species are thought to be exclusively sexually monogamous. EPC in the animal kingdom has mostly been studied in birds and mammals. Possible benefits of EPC can be investigated within non-human species, such as birds.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Infanticide (zoology)</span> Killing of young offspring by an adult animal of the same species

In animals, infanticide involves the intentional killing of young offspring by a mature animal of the same species. Animal infanticide is studied in zoology, specifically in the field of ethology. Ovicide is the analogous destruction of eggs. The practice has been observed in many species throughout the animal kingdom, especially primates but including microscopic rotifers, insects, fish, amphibians, birds and mammals. Infanticide can be practiced by both males and females.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Parental care</span>

Parental care is a behavioural and evolutionary strategy adopted by some animals, involving a parental investment being made to the evolutionary fitness of offspring. Patterns of parental care are widespread and highly diverse across the animal kingdom. There is great variation in different animal groups in terms of how parents care for offspring, and the amount of resources invested by parents. For example, there may be considerable variation in the amount of care invested by each sex, where females may invest more in some species, males invest more in others, or investment may be shared equally. Numerous hypotheses have been proposed to describe this variation and patterns in parental care that exist between the sexes, as well as among species.

Allomothering, allomaternal infant care/handling, or non-maternal infant care/handling is performed by any group member other than the mother. Alloparental care is provided by group members other than the genetic father or the mother and thus is distinguished from parental care. Both are widespread phenomena among social insects, birds and mammals.

Basic requirements of individual primates include obtaining food, avoiding predators, and reproducing. When these basic requirements are best pursued in the company of others, groups will form. Multi-male groups, also known as multi-male/multi-female, are a type of social organization where the group contains more than one adult male, more than one adult female, and offspring. This structure is characterized by group living, polygynandrous, and strong reproductive competition among males, which could result in an uneven division of male reproductive success. There are no stable heterosexual bonds as both males and females have a number of different mates. Multi-male groups are common among semi-terrestrial primates, like savanna baboons, macques, colobus and some New World monkeys.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Sexual swelling</span> Swelling of genital and perineal skin in some mammals as a sign of fertility

Sexual swelling, sexual skin, or anogenital tumescence refers to localized engorgement of the anus and vulva region of some female primates that vary in size over the course of the menstrual cycle. Thought to be an honest signal of fertility, male primates are attracted to these swellings; preferring, and competing for, females with the largest swellings.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Social monogamy in mammalian species</span> Monogamy in mammals

Social monogamy in mammals is defined as sexually mature adult organisms living in pairs. While there are many definitions of social monogamy, this social organization can be found in invertebrates, reptiles and amphibians, fish, birds, mammals, and humans.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Polyandry in animals</span> Class of mating system in non-human species

In behavioral ecology, polyandry is a class of mating system where one female mates with several males in a breeding season. Polyandry is often compared to the polygyny system based on the cost and benefits incurred by members of each sex. Polygyny is where one male mates with several females in a breeding season . A common example of polyandrous mating can be found in the field cricket of the invertebrate order Orthoptera. Polyandrous behavior is also prominent in many other insect species, including the red flour beetle and the species of spider Stegodyphus lineatus. Polyandry also occurs in some primates such as marmosets, mammal groups, the marsupial genus' Antechinus and bandicoots, around 1% of all bird species, such as jacanas and dunnocks, insects such as honeybees, and fish such as pipefish.

Endocrinology of parenting has been the subject of considerable study with focus both on human females and males and on females and males of other mammalian species. Parenting as an adaptive problem in mammals involves specific endocrine signals that were naturally selected to respond to infant cues and environmental inputs. Infants across species produce a number of cues to inform caregivers of their needs. These include visual cues, like facial characteristics, or in some species smiling, auditory cues, such as vocalizations, olfactory cues, and tactile stimulation. A commonly mentioned hormone in parenting is oxytocin, however many other hormones relay key information that results in variations in behavior. These include estrogen, progesterone, prolactin, cortisol, and testosterone. While hormones are not necessary for the expression of maternal behavior, they may influence it.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Extended female sexuality</span>

Extended female sexuality is where the female of a species mates despite being infertile. In most species, the female only engages in copulation when she is fertile. However, extended sexuality has been documented in Old World primates, pair bonded birds and some insects. Extended sexuality is most prominent in human females who exhibit no change in copulation rate across the ovarian cycle.

Infanticide in non-human primates occurs when an individual kills its own or another individual's dependent young. Five hypotheses have been proposed to explain infanticide in non-human primates: exploitation, resource competition, parental manipulation, sexual selection, and social pathology.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Primate sociality</span>

Primate sociality is an area of primatology that aims to study the interactions between three main elements of a primate social network: the social organisation, the social structure and the mating system. The intersection of these three structures describe the socially complex behaviours and relationships occurring among adult males and females of a particular species. Cohesion and stability of groups are maintained through a confluence of factors, including: kinship, willingness to cooperate, frequency of agonistic behaviour, or varying intensities of dominance structures.

References

  1. Stockley P, Hobson L (April 2016). "Paternal care and litter size coevolution in mammals". Proceedings. Biological Sciences. 283 (1829): 20160140. doi:10.1098/rspb.2016.0140. PMC   4855383 . PMID   27097924.
  2. Lack L (1968). Ecological Adaptations for Breeding in Birds . London: Methuen.
  3. Trivers RL (1972). "Parental investment and sexual selection" . In Campbell B (ed.). Sexual selection and the descent of Man 1871–1971. Chicago: Aldine. pp.  136–179. ISBN   9780202020051.
  4. Westneat DF, Sherman PW (1993). "Parentage and the evolution of parental behavior". Behavioral Ecology. 4: 66–77. doi: 10.1093/beheco/4.1.66 .
  5. 1 2 Cockburn A (June 2006). "Prevalence of different modes of parental care in birds". Proceedings. Biological Sciences. 273 (1592): 1375–83. doi:10.1098/rspb.2005.3458. PMC   1560291 . PMID   16777726.
  6. Kleiman DG, Malcolm JR (1981). "The evolution of male parental investment in mammals.". In Gubernick DJ, Klopfer PH (eds.). Parental care in mammals . New York: Plenum Press. pp.  347–387. ISBN   9780306405334.
  7. Clutton-Brock TH (1991). The evolution of parental care. Princeton, New Jersey: Princeton University Press.
  8. Reynolds JD, Goodwin NB, Freckleton RP (March 2002). "Evolutionary transitions in parental care and live bearing in vertebrates". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. 357 (1419): 269–81. doi:10.1098/rstb.2001.0930. PMC   1692951 . PMID   11958696.
  9. 1 2 Wesołowski T (2004). "The origin of parental care in birds: a reassessment". Behavioral Ecology. 15 (3): 520–523. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arh039 .
  10. Creel S, Creel NM, Monfort SL (October 1998). "Birth order, estrogens and sex-ratio adaptation in African wild dogs (Lycaon pictus)". Animal Reproduction Science. 53 (1–4): 315–320. doi:10.1016/S0378-4320(98)00121-3. PMID   9835385.
  11. Kleiman DG, Malcolm JR. 1981. The evolution of male parental investment in mammals. Pp 347-387 in Parental Care in Mammals, Gubernick DJ, Klopfer PH, eds. Plenum Press, New York.
  12. Woodroffe R, Vincent A (August 1994). "Mother's little helpers: Patterns of male care in mammals". Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 9 (8): 294–7. doi:10.1016/0169-5347(94)90033-7. PMID   21236858.
  13. Careau V, Garland T (2012). "Performance, personality, and energetics: correlation, causation, and mechanism" (PDF). Physiological and Biochemical Zoology. 85 (6): 543–71. doi:10.1086/666970. hdl: 10536/DRO/DU:30056093 . PMID   23099454. S2CID   16499109.
  14. 1 2 Andrew JR, Saltzman W, Chappell MA, Garland T (2016). "Consequences of Fatherhood in the Biparental California Mouse (Peromyscus californicus): Locomotor Performance, Metabolic Rate, and Organ Masses". Physiological and Biochemical Zoology. 89 (2): 130–40. doi:10.1086/685435. PMC   5013725 . PMID   27082723.
  15. Campbell JC, Laugero KD, Van Westerhuyzen JA, Hostetler CM, Cohen JD, Bales KL (September 2009). "Costs of pair-bonding and paternal care in male prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster)". Physiology & Behavior. 98 (3): 367–73. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.06.014. PMC   2755084 . PMID   19576236.
  16. 1 2 Wynne-Edwards KE, Timonin ME (June 2007). "Paternal care in rodents: weakening support for hormonal regulation of the transition to behavioral fatherhood in rodent animal models of biparental care". Hormones and Behavior. 52 (1): 114–21. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.03.018. PMID   17482188. S2CID   24293885.
  17. Ziegler TE, Prudom SL, Schultz-Darken NJ, Kurian AV, Snowdon CT (June 2006). "Pregnancy weight gain: marmoset and tamarin dads show it too". Biology Letters. 2 (2): 181–3. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2005.0426. PMC   1483903 . PMID   16810338.
  18. West HE, Capellini I (June 2016). "Male care and life history traits in mammals". Nature Communications. 7 (1): 11854. Bibcode:2016NatCo...711854W. doi:10.1038/ncomms11854. PMC   4911630 . PMID   27299954.
  19. Jejurikar R. "Paternal care". Center for Academic Research & Training in Anthropogeny (CARTA).
  20. Zhang S (9 September 2013). "Better fathers have smaller testicles". Nature. doi:10.1038/nature.2013.13701. S2CID   184406805.
  21. Nelson SK, Kushlev K, English T, Dunn EW, Lyubomirsky S (January 2013). "In defense of parenthood: children are associated with more joy than misery". Psychological Science. 24 (1): 3–10. doi:10.1177/0956797612447798. PMID   23201970. S2CID   3017204.
  22. Sanders R (Nov 12, 2013). "The Father Absence Crisis in America [Infographic]". The Father Factor: The Official Blog of National Fatherhood Initiative. National Fatherhood Initiative. Archived from the original on 2013-11-21. Retrieved 17 December 2013.
  23. Kim S, Glassgow AE (2018-07-04). "The effect of father's absence, parental adverse events, and neighborhood disadvantage on children's aggression and delinquency: A multi-analytic approach". Journal of Human Behavior in the Social Environment. 28 (5): 570–587. doi:10.1080/10911359.2018.1443866. PMC   6516459 . PMID   31097910.
  24. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Fernandez-Duque E, Juárez CP, Di Fiore A (January 2008). "Adult male replacement and subsequent infant care by male and siblings in socially monogamous owl monkeys (Aotus azarai)". Primates; Journal of Primatology. 49 (1): 81–4. doi:10.1007/s10329-007-0056-z. PMID   17805482. S2CID   2665652.
  25. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Huck M, Fernandez-Duque E (2012-07-24). "When Dads Help: Male Behavioral Care During Primate Infant Development". Building Babies. Springer New York. pp. 361–385. doi:10.1007/978-1-4614-4060-4_16. hdl:10545/610534. ISBN   978-1-4614-4059-8.
  26. 1 2 3 4 Moscovice LR, Heesen M, Di Fiore A, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (August 2009). "Paternity alone does not predict long-term investment in juveniles by male baboons". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 63 (10): 1471–1482. doi:10.1007/s00265-009-0781-y. PMC   2755737 . PMID   19816527.
  27. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 Minge C, Berghänel A, Schülke O, Ostner J (June 2016). "Macaca assamensis". International Journal of Primatology. 37 (3): 350–370. doi:10.1007/s10764-016-9904-2. PMC   4978776 . PMID   27546937.
  28. 1 2 3 4 5 Murray CM, Stanton MA, Lonsdorf EV, Wroblewski EE, Pusey AE (November 2016). "Chimpanzee fathers bias their behaviour towards their offspring". Royal Society Open Science. 3 (11): 160441. Bibcode:2016RSOS....360441M. doi:10.1098/rsos.160441. PMC   5180124 . PMID   28018626.
  29. 1 2 3 Boesch C, Lehmann J, Fickenscher G (January 2006). "Kin biased investment in wild chimpanzees". Behaviour. 143 (8): 931–955. doi:10.1163/156853906778623635.
  30. Lappan S (April 2008). "Male care of infants in a siamang (Symphalangus syndactylus) population including socially monogamous and polyandrous groups" (PDF). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 62 (8): 1307–1317. doi:10.1007/s00265-008-0559-7. S2CID   5857416.
  31. 1 2 3 4 5 Tecot SR, Baden AL, Romine NK, Kamilar JM (October 2012). "Infant parking and nesting, not allomaternal care, influence Malagasy primate life histories". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 66 (10): 1375–1386. doi:10.1007/s00265-012-1393-5. S2CID   15946699.
  32. Jacobs RL, Frankel DC, Rice RJ, Kiefer VJ, Bradley BJ (February 2018). "Parentage complexity in socially monogamous lemurs (Eulemur rubriventer): Integrating genetic and observational data". American Journal of Primatology. 80 (2): e22738. doi:10.1002/ajp.22738. PMID   29405330. S2CID   3408109.
  33. 1 2 3 4 5 Buchan JC, Alberts SC, Silk JB, Altmann J (September 2003). "True paternal care in a multi-male primate society". Nature. 425 (6954): 179–81. Bibcode:2003Natur.425..179B. doi:10.1038/nature01866. PMID   12968180. S2CID   1348221.
  34. 1 2 3 Langos D, Kulik L, Ruiz-Lambides A, Widdig A (2015-09-14). "Does Male Care, Provided to Immature Individuals, Influence Immature Fitness in Rhesus Macaques?". PLOS ONE. 10 (9): e0137841. Bibcode:2015PLoSO..1037841L. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0137841 . PMC   4569174 . PMID   26367536.
  35. 1 2 Onyango PO, Gesquiere LR, Altmann J, Alberts SC (March 2013). "Testosterone positively associated with both male mating effort and paternal behavior in Savanna baboons (Papio cynocephalus)". Hormones and Behavior. 63 (3): 430–6. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.11.014. PMC   3637985 . PMID   23206991.
  36. 1 2 Langos D, Kulik L, Mundry R, Widdig A (July 2013). "The impact of paternity on male-infant association in a primate with low paternity certainty". Molecular Ecology. 22 (13): 3638–51. Bibcode:2013MolEc..22.3638L. doi:10.1111/mec.12328. PMC   3800749 . PMID   23682587.
  37. 1 2 Ostner J, Vigilant L, Bhagavatula J, Franz M, Schülke O (September 2013). "Stable heterosexual associations in a promiscuous primate". Animal Behaviour. 86 (3): 623–31. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.07.004. S2CID   53157437.
  38. 1 2 3 Kerhoas D, Kulik L, Perwitasari-Farajallah D, Agil M, Engelhardt A, Widdig A (August 2016). "Mother-male bond, but not paternity, influences male-infant affiliation in wild crested macaques". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 70 (8): 1117–1130. doi:10.1007/s00265-016-2116-0. PMC   4954837 . PMID   27478299.
  39. Inoue E, Inoue-Murayama M, Vigilant L, Takenaka O, Nishida T (November 2008). "Relatedness in wild chimpanzees: influence of paternity, male philopatry, and demographic factors". American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 137 (3): 256–62. doi:10.1002/ajpa.20865. PMID   18512686.
  40. Ishizuka S, Kawamoto Y, Sakamaki T, Tokuyama N, Toda K, Okamura H, Furuichi T (January 2018). "Paternity and kin structure among neighbouring groups in wild bonobos at Wamba". Royal Society Open Science. 5 (1): 171006. Bibcode:2018RSOS....571006I. doi:10.1098/rsos.171006. PMC   5792889 . PMID   29410812.
  41. Vargas-Pinilla P, Babb P, Nunes L, Paré P, Rosa G, Felkl A, Longo D, Salzano FM, Paixão-Côrtes VR, Gonçalves GL, Bortolini MC (January 2017). "Progesterone Response Element Variation in the OXTR Promoter Region and Paternal Care in New World Monkeys". Behavior Genetics. 47 (1): 77–87. doi:10.1007/s10519-016-9806-2. PMID   27562397. S2CID   42320457.
  42. 1 2 Spence-Aizenberg A, Di Fiore A, Fernandez-Duque E (January 2016). "Social monogamy, male-female relationships, and biparental care in wild titi monkeys (Callicebus discolor)". Primates; Journal of Primatology. 57 (1): 103–12. doi:10.1007/s10329-015-0489-8. PMID   26386712. S2CID   2457638.
  43. Deluycker AM (January 2014). "Observations of a daytime birthing event in wild titi monkeys (Callicebus oenanthe): implications of the male parental role". Primates; Journal of Primatology. 55 (1): 59–67. doi:10.1007/s10329-013-0368-0. PMID   23783761. S2CID   1251931.
  44. 1 2 Sargeant EJ, Wikberg EC, Kawamura S, Jack KM, Fedigan LM (June 2016). "Paternal kin recognition and infant care in white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus)". American Journal of Primatology. 78 (6): 659–68. doi:10.1002/ajp.22530. PMID   26815856. S2CID   3802355.
  45. Trainor BC, Pride MC, Villalon Landeros R, Knoblauch NW, Takahashi EY, Silva AL, Crean KK (February 2011). "Sex differences in social interaction behavior following social defeat stress in the monogamous California mouse (Peromyscus californicus)". PLOS ONE. 6 (2): e17405. Bibcode:2011PLoSO...617405T. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0017405 . PMC   3045459 . PMID   21364768.
  46. de Jong TR, Korosi A, Harris BN, Perea-Rodriguez JP, Saltzman W (2012). "Individual variation in paternal responses of virgin male California mice (Peromyscus californicus): behavioral and physiological correlates". Physiological and Biochemical Zoology. 85 (6): 740–51. doi:10.1086/665831. PMID   23099470. S2CID   7579901.
  47. Bendesky A, Kwon YM, Lassance JM, Lewarch CL, Yao S, Peterson BK, He MX, Dulac C, Hoekstra HE (April 2017). "The genetic basis of parental care evolution in monogamous mice". Nature. 544 (7651): 434–439. Bibcode:2017Natur.544..434B. doi:10.1038/nature22074. PMC   5600873 . PMID   28424518.
  48. Kunz TH, Hood WR (2000). "Parental care and postnatal growth in the Chiroptera." (PDF). In Chrichton EG, Krutzsch PH (eds.). Reproductive Biology of Bats. Academic Press. pp. 416–510.
  49. Brown JL, Morales V, Summers K (April 2010). "A key ecological trait drove the evolution of biparental care and monogamy in an amphibian". The American Naturalist. 175 (4): 436–46. doi:10.1086/650727. PMID   20180700. S2CID   20270737.
  50. 1 2 Balshine S, Sloman KA (1 June 2011). "Parental Care in Fishes" (PDF). In Farrell AP (ed.). Encyclopedia of Fish Physiology: From Genome to Environment. Academic Press. p. 670–677 (672). ISBN   978-0-08-092323-9.
  51. Blumer LS (1982-05-01). "A bibliography and categorization of bony fishes exhibiting parental care" (PDF). Zoological Journal of the Linnean Society. 75 (1): 1–22. doi:10.1111/j.1096-3642.1982.tb01939.x. hdl: 2027.42/71841 .
  52. Taylor AL (2012). "Shells, trees and bottoms: Strange places fish live". BBC. Retrieved February 26, 2015.
  53. Wootton RJ (15 November 2023). A Functional Biology of Sticklebacks. Berkeley: University of California Press. ISBN   978-0-520-33515-8.
  54. Wong JW, Meunier J, Koelliker M (April 2013). "The evolution of parental care in insects: the roles of ecology, life history and the social environment". Ecological Entomology. 38 (2): 123–37. Bibcode:2013EcoEn..38..123W. doi: 10.1111/een.12000 . S2CID   82267208.
  55. Smith RL (1997). "Evolution of paternal care in the giant water bugs (Heteroptera: Belostomatidae)". In Choe JC, Crespi BJ (eds.). The Evolution of Social Behavior in Insects and Arachnids . Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press. pp.  116–149.
  56. Ohba SY, Hidaka K, Sasaki M (2006). "Notes on paternal care and sibling cannibalism in the giant water bug, Lethocerus deyrolli (Heteroptera: Belostomatidae)". Entomological Science. 9: 1–5. doi:10.1111/j.1479-8298.2006.00147.x. S2CID   84609130.
  57. Requena GS, Buzatto BA, Martins EG, Machado G (2012). "Paternal care decreases foraging activity and body condition, but does not impose survival costs to caring males in a Neotropical arachnid". PLOS ONE. 7 (10): e46701. Bibcode:2012PLoSO...746701R. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0046701 . PMC   3468633 . PMID   23071616.
  58. Duffy SE (2010). "Crustacean social evolution". Encyclopedia of Animal Behavior. Elsevier. pp. 421–429.
  59. Tallamy DW (2001). "Evolution of exclusive paternal care in arthropods". Annual Review of Entomology. 46 (1): 139–65. doi:10.1146/annurev.ento.46.1.139. PMID   11112166.
  60. Salgado M (April 2013). "The Evolution of Paternal Care". In Greenberg AM, Kennedy WG, Bos ND (eds.). International Conference on Social Computing, Behavioral-Cultural Modeling, and Prediction. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag. pp. 1–10.
  61. Salgado M (September 2015). "The evolution of paternal care can lead to population growth in artificial societies". Journal of Theoretical Biology. 380: 192–202. Bibcode:2015JThBi.380..192S. doi:10.1016/j.jtbi.2015.05.034. PMID   26051195.
  62. Klug H, Bonsall MB, Alonzo SH (April 2013). "The origin of parental care in relation to male and female life history". Ecology and Evolution. 3 (4): 779–91. Bibcode:2013EcoEv...3..779K. doi:10.1002/ece3.493. PMC   3631394 . PMID   23610624.
  63. Klug H, Bonsall MB, Alonzo SH (April 2013). "Sex differences in life history drive evolutionary transitions among maternal, paternal, and bi-parental care". Ecology and Evolution. 3 (4): 792–806. Bibcode:2013EcoEv...3..792K. doi:10.1002/ece3.494. PMC   3631395 . PMID   23610625.
  64. Pruett KD (18 April 1997). "How Men and Children Affect Each Other's Development". Zero to Three.
  65. Curley JP, Mashoodh R, Champagne FA (March 2011). "Epigenetics and the origins of paternal effects". Hormones and Behavior. 59 (3): 306–14. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.06.018. PMC   2975825 . PMID   20620140.
  66. Polito MJ, Trivelpiece WE (2008). "Transition to Independence and Evidence of Extended Parental Care in the Gentoo Penguin (Pygoscelis Papua).". Marine Biology.
  67. "Natural reward pathways exist in the brain". Learn Genetics. University of Utah. Archived from the original on 2013-06-29. Retrieved 2013-05-11.
  68. Brunton PJ, Russell JA, Douglas AJ (June 2008). "Adaptive responses of the maternal hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis during pregnancy and lactation". Journal of Neuroendocrinology. 20 (6): 764–76. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2826.2008.01735.x . PMID   18601699. S2CID   28123845.
  69. Numan M, Insel TR (2003). The Neurobiology of Parental Behavior. New York: Springer.
  70. Rilling JK (March 2013). "The neural and hormonal bases of human parental care". Neuropsychologia. 51 (4): 731–47. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.12.017. PMID   23333868. S2CID   23008342.
  71. Gregg JK, Wynne-Edwards KE (September 2005). "Placentophagia in naïve adults, new fathers, and new mothers in the biparental dwarf hamster, Phodopus campbelli". Developmental Psychobiology. 47 (2): 179–88. doi:10.1002/dev.20079. PMID   16136563.
  72. Gregg JK, Wynne-Edwards KE (November 2006). "In uniparental Phodopus sungorus, new mothers, and fathers present during the birth of their offspring, are the only hamsters that readily consume fresh placenta". Developmental Psychobiology. 48 (7): 528–36. doi:10.1002/dev.20174. PMID   17016837.
  73. Lévy F, Keller M (June 2009). "Olfactory mediation of maternal behavior in selected mammalian species". Behavioural Brain Research. 200 (2): 336–45. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2008.12.017. PMID   19146885. S2CID   8420915.
  74. Lévy F, Keller M, Poindron P (September 2004). "Olfactory regulation of maternal behavior in mammals". Hormones and Behavior. 46 (3): 284–302. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.02.005. PMID   15325229. S2CID   31209629.
  75. Gleason ED, Marler CA (July 2013). "Non-genomic transmission of paternal behaviour between fathers and sons in the monogamous and biparental California mouse". Proceedings. Biological Sciences. 280 (1763): 20130824. doi:10.1098/rspb.2013.0824. PMC   4043185 . PMID   23698012.

Further reading