Moro | |
---|---|
Dhimorong | |
Native to | Sudan |
Region | South Kordofan |
Ethnicity | Moro Nuba |
Native speakers | 79,000 (2022) [1] |
Dialects |
|
Latin | |
Language codes | |
ISO 639-3 | mor |
Glottolog | moro1285 |
ELP | Moro |
Moro is classified as Definitely Endangered by the UNESCO Atlas of the World's Languages in Danger |
Moro is a Kordofanian language spoken in the Nuba Mountains of South Kordofan, Sudan. [2] It is part of the Western group of West Central Heiban Kordofonian languages and belongs to the Niger-Congo phylum. [3] In 1982 there were an estimated 30,000 Moro-speakers. This was before the second Sudan civil war and therefore the recent number of speakers might differ. There can be noted an influence of Arabic and it is suspected that today approximately a fourth of all Moro vocabulary has a relation or an origin in the Arabic language. [4]
There are different intonations in the Moro language. Usually the vowels “e”, “a” and “o” have a lower tone, while the vowels “i”, “u” and “ʌ” have a higher tone. The vowel “ə” is a Schwa vowel and therefore neutral.
One can find vowel harmony, palatalization of dental stops, resistance to deletion in vowel hiatus resolution and imposition of a partially replaceable tone patterns within the language. However it is important to note that the latter two are unique to the causative in Moro (9;2,11). [5]
Moro has a seven vowel inventory, [6] [3] [7] [8] schematized in the table below. [ə] can be epenthetic or a reduced version of the peripheral vowels /i e o u/; it also appears in roots with no obvious source of reduction. [7] The behaviour of the schwa [ə] in Moro vowel harmony has been taken as a reason to posit two [ə] vowels in Moro - a higher one that raises vowels, and a lower one that does not. [8]
Front | Central | Back | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
High | i | u | ||
Mid | e | ə | ʌ | o |
Low | a |
In addition, light diphthongs such as [iə], [eə], [oa], and [uʌ] are also attested. [7] Light diphthongs count as a single tone-bearing unit. Vowel length is not contrastive, but lengthening is often observed in open penultimate or root-initial syllable. [7]
The consonantal inventory of Moro is given below. [7]
Labial | Dental | Alveolar | Retroflex | Palatal | Velar | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Stop | p b | t̪ d̪ | t d | k g | ||
Affricate | tʃ dʒ | |||||
Fricative | f v | ð | s | |||
Nasal | m | n | ɲ | ŋ | ||
Trill | r | |||||
Flap | ɾ | ɽ | ||||
Lateral | l | |||||
Glide | w | j |
Consonants can be geminated, with the exception of [ɾ,ɽ,j]. Voiced stops and /v/ are realized as voiceless when geminate. [7]
Moro has a 'one-step' height vowel harmony system, in which the lower vowels /e a o/ of affixes are raised to their high counterparts [i ʌ u] if the root vowel is high. [8] In addition to root- or stem-controlled harmony, Moro also exhibits a dominant-recessive harmony pattern in which certain extension suffixes trigger raising of preceding prefixes and root vowels, and a following suffix; these harmony-triggering suffixes are the causative -i, applicative -ət̪, and passive -ən. [8]
Some Moro roots containing [ə] trigger vowel harmony, while others do not; similarly, while the applicative and passive suffix both contain [ə] and trigger vowel harmony, Moro exhibits other suffixes containing [ə], such as the antipassive -əđ, which do not. Accoustice studies confirm that the schwas which trigger vowel harmony in Moro show a significantly lower F1 formant than those which do not, and that schwas in raised vowel contexts in Moro also exhibit significantly lower F1 formants than those in non-raised contexts. [8] As such, some scholars argue that Moro includes two schwa vowels: a high schwa, which triggers vowel harmony, and a low schwa, which does not. [8]
Moro can be categorized into 18 noun classes: 8 major, 5 minor and 5 unpaired. [3] Most of these classes have their individual class prefix, singular prefix and concord as well as plural prefix and concord. [9]
Semantic ID | Class prefix | Singular prefix | Singular concord | Plural prefix | Plural concord |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
people | g/l | ?/w | G | L | L |
animals/body parts | l/ŋ | L | L | Ñ | Ñ |
trees | đ/g | Đ | Đ | W | G |
common things | g/n | ? | Đ | N | N |
Things of shapes | l/ŋ | l/ɽ/ɽr | l/ɽ/ɽr | Ŋ | Ŋ |
Long things | đ/r | Đ | Đ | R | R |
/ | g/n | Đ | Đ | ? | G |
Large and harmful things | đ/j | Đ | Đ | y | y |
Domestic/small animals | ŋ/ŋ | Ŋ- | Ŋ- | Ñ | Ñ |
Liquids and abstract nouns | ŋ | Ŋ- | Ŋ- | — | — |
Emotions | đ | đ | Đ | — | — |
Cow, goat, irregular nouns | r/j | — | r | — | Y |
Foreign words | j/j | — | Y | — | y |
There are some identified rules for the construction of the plural according to its noun class:
Moro language has many compound adjectives, that have their origin in the same root. One can find comparative adjectives as well as superlatives however, these are normally translated in “very, very”. [10]
Compound adjectives are formed with a suffix as following:
Gaicia | Bad |
Gaiciaŋaiyo | Ugly |
Gaicianano | Sad |
To construct the comparative of a simple adjective, one must change the last vowel of the adjective into “-ə -“ and then add the suffix “-tu”:
Gabəco | White |
Gabəcətu | Very white |
To form the comparative of compound adjectives one must first form the comparative of the first part of the adjective and then add the suffix:
Gamenano | Happy |
Gamətunano | Very happy |
To construct the superlative there are several opportunities:
There are eight Subject pronouns in Moro but each in two different variations. The exact meaning of these variations have not yet been determined.
Variation 1 | Variation 2 | |
---|---|---|
I | ñi | Igënəñi |
You (singular) | ŋa | Aganəŋa |
He/she/it | ŋu | Gënəŋu |
We (dual) | Ləŋ | Lëləŋ |
We (inclusive) | Nëndr/lëndr | Nëndr |
We (exclusive) | Nanda/landa | ñagananda |
You (plural) | ñaŋ | ñaganəñaŋ |
They | ŋulu | lënəŋulu |
Each subject pronoun has its own agreement prefix. These prefixes can vary in accordance to the tense or the aspect of the verb. [11]
In the present and past tense they are attached to the particle “ga“ and used as (exemplified with the verb “tu” = “to drink”) following:
I | i- | Ñi igatu | I was drinking |
You (singular) | a- | Ŋa agatu | You were drinking |
He/she/it | / (concord agreem.) | Ŋu gatu | He was drinking |
We (dual) | La-/lə- | Ləŋ lagatu | We were drinking |
We (inclusive) | La-/lə- | Nëndr lagatr | We were drinking |
We (exclusive) | ña- | Nanda ñagatu | We were drinking |
You (plural) | ña- | ñan ñagatu | You were drinking |
They | / (concord agreem.) | Ŋulu latu | They were drinking |
The morphophonology changes when the suffix “-r” is added to the end of a verb:
For the future tense the subject prefixes that are mentioned above will are added to the auxiliary of the future verb. The main verb of the future tense has different subject prefixes.
There have been identified following rules:
The subject prefix of the future tense is exemplified in the following using the auxiliary “-gidi” and the verb “tu” (to drink):
Future tense SP | Example | Translation | |
---|---|---|---|
I | ñi-/ ñe- | Igiđi ñiti | I will drink |
You (singular) | ŋa- | Agiđi nati | You will drink |
He/she/it | Aŋə- | Giđi aŋəti | He will drink |
We (dual) | Alə- | Lagiđi aləti | We will drink |
We (inclusive) | Alə + r- | Lagiđr alətr | We will drink |
We (exclusive) | ña- | ñagiđi ñati | We will drink |
You (plural) | ña- | ñagiđi ñati | You will drink |
They | Alə- | Liđi aləti | They will drink |
Moro also has a set of eight object pronouns for each tense: In case of the past tense, the object infix will be added to the end of the verb, while in case of the present tense it is inserted between the tense marker “ga” and the verb stem. [11]
OP Present | OP Past | OP Future | |
---|---|---|---|
I | Iña | ñe/ ñ i | ñə |
You (singular) | ŋa | Aŋa | ŋa |
He/she/it | Ma | Ma | Ma |
We (dual) | Ndə | Nde | Ndə |
We (inclusive) | Ndə | Ndr | Ndə |
We (exclusive) | Ndə | Nde | Ndə |
You (plural) | Ndə | Nde | Ndə |
They | lə | Lo | lə |
Also with object pronouns there are some morphophonemic changes:
In case of the past tense, the object infix will be added to the end of the verb, while in case of the present tense it is inserted between the tense marker “ga” and the verb stem:
Again, compound verbs are handled slightly differently. While in the present and future tense they can take the same agreements as simple verbs, in the past tense the object pronoun is inserted before the “-alo” or “-ano”. [12]
Researches could identify some pre- and postpositions in the Moro language. The postposition “-ŋa” can be translated to “with” and can be added at least to verbs:
In other cases the prepositions “o-“ for singular and “lə-“ for plural can express that someone is from a certain place:
In Moro there are so far three identified tenses: the past, the present and the future. While the future- and the past tense take tense markers, the future tense comes with an additional auxiliary. [14]
The past tense is usually used at the start of a story and often follows the word “when”. Additionally, it describes an action from the past that is still ongoing in the present. It is important to note that if a story is started with the past tense it indicates that other actions of the same time are in the past tense too. The present tense is used to describe actions that happened at the same time as the one before and if the word “when” is used, it changes its meaning to “while”. Finally, it describes new situations that have not been there before. The future tense is used for the expression of an intention to do something. [15]
The tense marker for the past tense is “ga” and it comes with little exceptions:
Verb root | Verb in past tense | Translation |
---|---|---|
-tu | Gatu | Drank |
-erido | Gerido | Walked |
-naico | Ganaico | Gave |
For the present tense, the tense marker remains “ga-“ but there are more rules regarding the changes of vowels:
Verb root | Verb in the present tense | Translation |
---|---|---|
-tu | Gatia | Drink |
-erido | Gaberida | Walk |
-naico | ganaica | Give |
Within the present tense they are also more rules regarding the compound verbs (to be happy; to be sad etc.). In Moro these verbs end with “-alo” or “-ano” and follow other agreements than simple verbs:
Verb root | Past tense | Present tense | Translation |
---|---|---|---|
- ŋəranano | Gaŋəránano | Gaŋərànano | To be happy |
-cianano | Gaiciánano | Gaiciànano | To be sad |
-eɽiano | Geɽíano | Geɽìano | To be untrustworthy |
The future tense is constructed by using the auxiliary “giđi” which is then followed by the main verb. There are following rules:
Verb root | Future tense | Translation |
---|---|---|
-tu | (giđi) Aŋəti | Will drink |
-naico | (giđi) Aŋənaice | Will walk |
-erido | (giđi) Aŋeride | Will give |
A simple Moro clause consists of the Subject – Verb – Object:
Trwí
policeman
đʌdəri
stops
trʌmbílí.
car
The order of a noun phrase is in the order of: Noun – Demonstrative – Numeral – Adjective. Noun classes are common in Niger-Congo languages, especially in Bantu languages like Kikuyu or Swahili. [16]
Nádám | Nətínə | Nəgətfan | Nóré |
Books | Those | Two | red |
Within the Moro language, several notations according to (verbal) derivations have been made. So far the following derivational morphemes have been described:
Before the arrival of the first missionaries in 1936, Moro was written with Arabic letters, therefore Latin capital letters were not known by Moro speakers. [4] Missionaries created an orthography including capital letters and also translated the New Testament into Moro in the Ləŋorəban dialect. Today Moro consists of 22 consonants and 7 vowels with one of them being a Schwa. [18]
A | B | C | D | Ḏ | Đ | E | Ë | Ə | F | G | I | J | K | L | M | N | Ñ | Ŋ | O | P | R | Ɽ | S | T | Ṯ | U | W | Y |
a | b | c | d | ḏ | đ | e | ë | ə | f | g | i | j | k | l | m | n | ñ | ŋ | o | p | r | ɽ | s | t | ṯ | u | w | y |
Đ is typically used in its bowl-struck form, approximately ⟨d̶⟩
There are 7 dialects of Moro (Guest 1997a). [20] Ethnologue names are given in parentheses. [1]
Each dialect corresponds to a separate clan, except for Ləmwarəŋ and Ləŋorəban, which are amalgamated into a single clan known as the Wërria. Thus, there is a total of 6 different clans. There is a lexical difference within the dialects, especially with the labials: “t” and “d” or “ɽ” and “t”. In other cases, the “b” of the Ləŋorəban dialect is noted as “f, v, w,” in other dialects.
There are also significant semantic differences in the dialects.
This even leads to the fact, that speakers of the Tobəɽelda dialect are not able to understand the translation of the New Testament which was translated into the Ləŋorəban dialect. [18] An explanation for this might be, that the speakers of the Ləŋorəban dialect seem to be geographically isolated from speakers of other dialects and receive influence from other languages they are surrounded by such as Katcha or Utundi. [23]
Ganda or Luganda is a Bantu language spoken in the African Great Lakes region. It is one of the major languages in Uganda and is spoken by more than 5.56 million Baganda and other people principally in central Uganda, including the country's capital, Kampala. Typologically, it is an agglutinative, tonal language with subject–verb–object word order and nominative–accusative morphosyntactic alignment.
Halkomelem is a language of various First Nations peoples of the British Columbia Coast. It is spoken in what is now British Columbia, ranging from southeastern Vancouver Island from the west shore of Saanich Inlet northward beyond Gabriola Island and Nanaimo to Nanoose Bay and including the Lower Mainland from the Fraser River Delta upriver to Harrison Lake and the lower boundary of the Fraser Canyon.
Supyire, or Suppire, is a Senufo language spoken in the Sikasso Region of southeastern Mali and in adjoining regions of Ivory Coast. In their native language, the noun sùpyìré means both "the people" and "the language spoken by the people".
Klallam,Clallam, Ns'Klallam or S'klallam, is a Straits Salishan language historically spoken by the Klallam people at Becher Bay on Vancouver Island in British Columbia and across the Strait of Juan de Fuca on the north coast of the Olympic Peninsula in Washington. The last native speaker of Klallam as a first language died in 2014, but there is a growing group of speakers of Klallam as a second language.
The Elgeyo language, or Kalenjin proper, are a dialect cluster of the Kalenjin branch of the Nilotic language family.
Tooro or Rutooro is a Bantu language spoken mainly by the Tooro people (Abatooro) from the Tooro Kingdom in western Uganda. There are three main areas where Tooro as a language is mainly used: Kabarole District, Kyenjojo District and Kyegegwa District. Tooro is unusual among Bantu languages as it lacks lexical tone. It is most closely related to Runyoro.
Dulong or Drung, Derung, Rawang, or Trung, is a Sino-Tibetan language in China. Dulong is closely related to the Rawang language of Myanmar (Burma). Although almost all ethnic Derung people speak the language to some degree, most are multilingual, also speaking Burmese, Lisu, and Mandarin Chinese except for a few very elderly people.
In Hebrew, verbs, which take the form of derived stems, are conjugated to reflect their tense and mood, as well as to agree with their subjects in gender, number, and person. Each verb has an inherent voice, though a verb in one voice typically has counterparts in other voices. This article deals mostly with Modern Hebrew, but to some extent, the information shown here applies to Biblical Hebrew as well.
Wayuu, or Guajiro, is a major Arawakan language spoken by 400,000 indigenous Wayuu people in northwestern Venezuela and northeastern Colombia on the Guajira Peninsula and surrounding Lake Maracaibo.
The Nukak language is a language of uncertain classification, perhaps part of the macrofamily Puinave-Maku. It is very closely related to Kakwa.
Moloko (Məlokwo) is an Afro-Asiatic language spoken in northern Cameroon.
Universal is an Esperantido, a constructed language based on Esperanto. It has inclusive and exclusive pronouns, uses partial reduplication for the plural, and inversion for antonyms. Inversion can be seen in,
The grammar of the Otomi language displays a mixture of elements of synthetic and analytic structures. Particularly the phrase-level morphology is synthetic, whereas the sentence-level is analytic. Simultaneously, the language is head-marking in terms of its verbal morphology, but not in its nominal morphology, which is more analytic. Otomi recognizes three large open word classes of nouns, verbs, and particles. There is a small closed class of property words, variously analyzed as adjectives or stative verbs.
The Fwe language, also known as Chifwe, is a Bantu language spoken by the Fwe people in Namibia and Zambia. It is closely related to the Subia language, Chisubia, and is one of several Bantu languages that feature click consonants.
Old Norse has three categories of verbs and two categories of nouns. Conjugation and declension are carried out by a mix of inflection and two nonconcatenative morphological processes: umlaut, a backness-based alteration to the root vowel; and ablaut, a replacement of the root vowel, in verbs.
Zulu grammar is the way in which meanings are encoded into wordings in the Zulu language. Zulu grammar is typical for Bantu languages, bearing all the hallmarks of this language family. These include agglutinativity, a rich array of noun classes, extensive inflection for person, tense and aspect, and a subject–verb–object word order.
Pech or Pesh is a Chibchan language spoken in Honduras. It was formerly known as Paya, and continues to be referred to in this manner by several sources, though there are negative connotations associated with this term. It has also been referred to as Seco. There are 300 speakers according to Yasugi (2007). It is spoken near the north-central coast of Honduras, in the Dulce Nombre de Culmí municipality of Olancho Department.
Jita is a Bantu language of Tanzania, spoken on the southeastern shore of Lake Victoria/Nyanza and on the island of Ukerewe.
Turkmen grammar is the grammar of the Turkmen language, whose dialectal variants are spoken in Turkmenistan, Iran, Afghanistan, Russia, China, Uzbekistan, Tajikistan and others. Turkmen grammar, as described in this article, is the grammar of standard Turkmen as spoken and written by Turkmen people in Turkmenistan.
Levantine Arabic grammar is the set of rules by which Levantine Arabic creates statements, questions and commands. In many respects, it is quite similar to that of the other vernacular Arabic varieties.