Moro language

Last updated
Moro
Dhimorong
Native to Sudan
Region South Kordofan
Ethnicity Moro Nuba
Native speakers
79,000 (2022) [1]
Dialects
  • Ulba
  • Laiyen
  • Nubwa
  • Werria
  • Nderre
  • Longorban
  • Thetogovela
Latin
Language codes
ISO 639-3 mor
Glottolog moro1285
ELP Moro
Lang Status 60-DE.svg
Moro is classified as Definitely Endangered by the UNESCO Atlas of the World's Languages in Danger

Moro is a Kordofanian language spoken in the Nuba Mountains of South Kordofan, Sudan. [2] It is part of the Western group of West Central Heiban Kordofonian languages and belongs to the Niger-Congo phylum. [3] In 1982 there were an estimated 30,000 Moro-speakers. This was before the second Sudan civil war and therefore the recent number of speakers might differ. There can be noted an influence of Arabic and it is suspected that today approximately a fourth of all Moro vocabulary has a relation or an origin in the Arabic language. [4]

Contents

Phonology

There are different intonations in the Moro language. Usually the vowels “e”, “a” and “o” have a lower tone, while the vowels “i”, “u” and “ʌ” have a higher tone. The vowel “ə” is a Schwa vowel and therefore neutral.

One can find vowel harmony, palatalization of dental stops, resistance to deletion in vowel hiatus resolution and imposition of a partially replaceable tone patterns within the language. However it is important to note that the latter two are unique to the causative in Moro (9;2,11). [5]

Segment Inventory

Moro has a seven vowel inventory, [6] [3] [7] [8] schematized in the table below. [ə] can be epenthetic or a reduced version of the peripheral vowels /i e o u/; it also appears in roots with no obvious source of reduction. [7] The behaviour of the schwa [ə] in Moro vowel harmony has been taken as a reason to posit two [ə] vowels in Moro - a higher one that raises vowels, and a lower one that does not. [8]

Vowel inventory
Front Central Back
High i u
Mid e ə ʌ o
Low a

In addition, light diphthongs such as [iə], [eə], [oa], and [uʌ] are also attested. [7] Light diphthongs count as a single tone-bearing unit. Vowel length is not contrastive, but lengthening is often observed in open penultimate or root-initial syllable. [7]

The consonantal inventory of Moro is given below. [7]

Consonantal inventory
Labial Dental Alveolar Retroflex Palatal Velar
Stop p b t d k g
Affricate
Fricative f v ð s
Nasal m n ɲ ŋ
Trill r
Flap ɾ ɽ
Lateral l
Glide w j

Consonants can be geminated, with the exception of [ɾ,ɽ,j]. Voiced stops and /v/ are realized as voiceless when geminate. [7]

Vowel Harmony

Moro has a 'one-step' height vowel harmony system, in which the lower vowels /e a o/ of affixes are raised to their high counterparts [i ʌ u] if the root vowel is high. [8] In addition to root- or stem-controlled harmony, Moro also exhibits a dominant-recessive harmony pattern in which certain extension suffixes trigger raising of preceding prefixes and root vowels, and a following suffix; these harmony-triggering suffixes are the causative -i, applicative -ət̪, and passive -ən. [8]

Schwa in Moro Vowel Harmony

Some Moro roots containing [ə] trigger vowel harmony, while others do not; similarly, while the applicative and passive suffix both contain [ə] and trigger vowel harmony, Moro exhibits other suffixes containing [ə], such as the antipassive -əđ, which do not. Accoustice studies confirm that the schwas which trigger vowel harmony in Moro show a significantly lower F1 formant than those which do not, and that schwas in raised vowel contexts in Moro also exhibit significantly lower F1 formants than those in non-raised contexts. [8] As such, some scholars argue that Moro includes two schwa vowels: a high schwa, which triggers vowel harmony, and a low schwa, which does not. [8]

Tone

Grammar

Nouns and Noun Phrases

Noun Classes

Moro can be categorized into 18 noun classes: 8 major, 5 minor and 5 unpaired. [3] Most of these classes have their individual class prefix, singular prefix and concord as well as plural prefix and concord. [9]

Semantic IDClass prefixSingular prefixSingular concordPlural prefixPlural concord
peopleg/l?/wGLL
animals/body partsl/ŋLLÑÑ
treesđ/gĐĐWG
common thingsg/n?ĐNN
Things of shapesl/ŋl/ɽ/ɽrl/ɽ/ɽrŊŊ
Long thingsđ/rĐĐRR
          /g/nĐĐ?G
Large and harmful thingsđ/jĐĐyy
Domestic/small animalsŋ/ŋŊ-Ŋ-ÑÑ
Liquids and abstract nounsŋŊ-Ŋ-
EmotionsđđĐ
Cow, goat, irregular nounsr/jrY
Foreign wordsj/jYy

There are some identified rules for the construction of the plural according to its noun class:

  • If the noun starts with either “e, i, o” or “u” and the following is a “đ”, those to letters in the plural will form “nđ-“
  • If the noun starts with either “e, i, o” or “u” and the following is a “r” or “ɽ”,  in the plural the vowel will be discarded
  • If the noun starts with “ɽ, ɽr” or “lɽr”, the first “ɽ” is dropped and the vowel that is inserted in the plural will most likely be “ə” [9]

Adjectives

Moro language has many compound adjectives, that have their origin in the same root. One can find comparative adjectives as well as superlatives however, these are normally translated in “very, very”. [10]

Compound adjectives are formed with a suffix as following:

GaiciaBad
GaiciaŋaiyoUgly
GaiciananoSad

To construct the comparative of a simple adjective, one must change the last vowel of the adjective into “-ə -“ and then add the suffix “-tu”:

GabəcoWhite
GabəcətuVery white

To form the comparative of compound adjectives one must first form the comparative of the first part of the adjective and then add the suffix:

GamenanoHappy
GamətunanoVery happy

To construct the superlative there are several opportunities:

  • Repeat one or two syllables of the word (in this case, sometimes the vowel of the repeated syllable changes and a “-đ” is added)
    Example: “geđe” (= green) turns into “geđeđe” (=very, very green)
    Example: “gapa” (= light) turns into “gapapeđ” (= very, very light)
  • Double a consonant or vowel
    Example: “gafalo” (= low) turns into “gaffalo” (= very, very low) (2.1; 1-2) [10]

Pronouns and agreement

There are eight Subject pronouns in Moro but each in two different variations. The exact meaning of these variations have not yet been determined.

Variation 1Variation 2
IñiIgënəñi
You (singular)ŋaAganəŋa
He/she/itŋuGënəŋu
We (dual)LəŋLëləŋ
We (inclusive)Nëndr/lëndrNëndr
We (exclusive)Nanda/landañagananda
You (plural)ñaŋñaganəñaŋ
Theyŋululënəŋulu

Each subject pronoun has its own agreement prefix. These prefixes can vary in accordance to the tense or the aspect of the verb. [11]

In the present and past tense they are attached to the particle “ga“ and used as (exemplified with the verb “tu” = “to drink”) following:

Ii-Ñi igatuI was drinking
You (singular)a-Ŋa agatuYou were drinking
He/she/it/ (concord agreem.)Ŋu gatuHe was drinking
We (dual)La-/lə-Ləŋ lagatuWe were drinking
We (inclusive)La-/lə-Nëndr lagatrWe were drinking
We (exclusive)ña-Nanda ñagatuWe were drinking
You (plural)ña-ñan ñagatuYou were drinking
They/ (concord agreem.)Ŋulu latuThey were drinking

The morphophonology changes when the suffix “-r” is added to the end of a verb:

  • A verb ending in either “o” or “u” in the past tense will have its vowel replaced by “-r”
  • A verb ending in either “e”, “a” or “i” will add “-r” to its end

For the future tense the subject prefixes that are mentioned above will are added to the auxiliary of the future verb. The main verb of the future tense has different subject prefixes.

There have been identified following rules:

  • The vowel of the subject prefix is dropped if a verb starts with a vowel
  • The prefixes for “we dual” and “we inclusive” become “aɽ-“ if the verb starts with a “r-“ or “ɽ-“
  • Compound verbs (to be happy; to be sad etc.) are handled differently

The subject prefix of the future tense is exemplified in the following using the auxiliary “-gidi” and the verb “tu” (to drink):

Future tense SPExampleTranslation
Iñi-/ ñe-Igiđi ñitiI will drink
You (singular)ŋa-Agiđi natiYou will drink
He/she/itAŋə-Giđi aŋətiHe will drink
We (dual)Alə-Lagiđi alətiWe will drink
We (inclusive)Alə + r-Lagiđr alətrWe will drink
We (exclusive)ña-ñagiđi ñatiWe will drink
You (plural)ña-ñagiđi ñatiYou will drink
TheyAlə-Liđi alətiThey will drink

Moro also has a set of eight object pronouns for each tense: In case of the past tense, the object infix will be added to the end of the verb, while in case of the present tense it is inserted between the tense marker “ga” and the verb stem. [11]

OP PresentOP PastOP Future
IIñañe/ ñ iñə
You (singular)ŋaAŋaŋa
He/she/itMaMaMa
We (dual)NdəNdeNdə
We (inclusive)NdəNdrNdə
We (exclusive)NdəNdeNdə
You (plural)NdəNdeNdə
TheyLo

Also with object pronouns there are some morphophonemic changes:

  • Verbs ending in “u” will turn their vowel into “i” (or “w” / “ə” before “aŋa”)
  • Verbs ending in “o” or “e” will turn their vowel into “ə” (in the case on “aŋa” it disappears)
  • Verbs ending in “a” will also drop their vowel in case of “aŋa”

In case of the past tense, the object infix will be added to the end of the verb, while in case of the present tense it is inserted between the tense marker “ga” and the verb stem:

Past tense; verb: “ma” – “gamənde” (= married us)
Present tense; verb: “ma” – “gandəma” (= marries us)
Future tense; verb: “ma” – “giđi aŋəndəme” (= will marry us)

Again, compound verbs are handled slightly differently. While in the present and future tense they can take the same agreements as simple verbs, in the past tense the object pronoun is inserted before the “-alo” or “-ano”. [12]

Adpositions

Researches could identify some pre- and postpositions in the Moro language. The postposition “-ŋa” can be translated to “with” and can be added at least to verbs:

Example: “ŋenŋa nəwujənu” means “talk with parables”

In other cases the prepositions “o-“ for singular and “lə-“ for plural can express that someone is from a certain place:

Example: “tofəgəlla” (= a place); “otofəgəlla” (= a person from that place); “tofəgəlla” (= people from that place” [13]

Verbs and Verb Phrases

Tense

In Moro there are so far three identified tenses: the past, the present and the future. While the future- and the past tense take tense markers, the future tense comes with an additional auxiliary. [14]

The past tense is usually used at the start of a story and often follows the word “when”. Additionally, it describes an action from the past that is still ongoing in the present. It is important to note that if a story is started with the past tense it indicates that other actions of the same time are in the past tense too. The present tense is used to describe actions that happened at the same time as the one before and if the word “when” is used, it changes its meaning to “while”. Finally, it describes new situations that have not been there before. The future tense is used for the expression of an intention to do something. [15]

The tense marker for the past tense is “ga” and it comes with little exceptions:

  • If the verb starts with a consonant: “ga-“ + verb root
  • If the verb starts with a vowel: “g-“ + verb root
  • If the verb starts with “ci”, “co” or “j”: “gai-“ + verb root
Verb rootVerb in past tenseTranslation
-tuGatuDrank
-eridoGeridoWalked
-naicoGanaicoGave

For the present tense, the tense marker remains “ga-“ but there are more rules regarding the changes of vowels:

  • If the verb starts with a consonant: “ga-“ + verb root + last vowel changes
  • If the verb starts with “i”: “ga-“ + “b” + (“i” turns into “ə”) + verb root + last vowel changes
  • If the verb starts with “e”, “ë” or “a”: “ga-“ + “b” + verb root + last vowel changes
  • If the verb starts with “w”, “u”, “o” or “a”: “g-“ + verb root + last vowel changes
  • The changes of the last vowel work according to the following: “e” remains “e”; “i” changes to “ia”; “o” changes to either “a”, “ia” or “wa”; “u” changes to either “ia”, “a” or “wa”
Verb rootVerb in the present tenseTranslation
-tuGatiaDrink
-eridoGaberidaWalk
-naicoganaicaGive

Within the present tense they are also more rules regarding the compound verbs (to be happy; to be sad etc.). In Moro these verbs end with “-alo” or “-ano” and follow other agreements than simple verbs:

  • They do not change their last vowel
  • To differ from the past tense, there is a variation in tone in which the past has a high tone and the present has a low tone (here: ´ = high tone; ` = low tone)
Verb rootPast tensePresent tenseTranslation
- ŋərananoGaŋəránanoGaŋərànanoTo be happy
-ciananoGaiciánanoGaiciànanoTo be sad
-eɽianoGeɽíanoGeɽìanoTo be untrustworthy

The future tense is constructed by using the auxiliary “giđi” which is then followed by the main verb. There are following rules:

  • Verbs starting with a consonant other than “ŋ”: “aŋə-” + verb root + last vowel changes
  • Verbs starting with “n”: “a-” + verb root + last vowel changes
  • Verbs starting with a vowel: “aŋ-“ + verb root + last vowel changes
  • The changes of the last vowel work according to the following: “u” becomes “i”; “o” becomes “e”; “a” and “i” remain the same [11]
Verb rootFuture tenseTranslation
-tu(giđi) AŋətiWill drink
-naico(giđi) AŋənaiceWill walk
-erido(giđi) AŋerideWill give

Syntax

Basic constituent order

A simple Moro clause consists of the Subject – Verb – Object:

Trwí

policeman

đʌdəri

stops

trʌmbílí.

car

Trwí đʌdəri trʌmbílí.

policeman stops car

The order of a noun phrase is in the order of: Noun – Demonstrative – Numeral – Adjective. Noun classes are common in Niger-Congo languages, especially in Bantu languages like Kikuyu or Swahili. [16]

NádámNətínəNəgətfanNóré
BooksThoseTwored

Derivational Morphology

Within the Moro language, several notations according to (verbal) derivations have been made. So far the following derivational morphemes have been described:

  1. Causative: add “-i”
  2. Passive/reflexive: “-ən-“ + change of the last vowel
    Example: “raico” = to pour; “raicenu” = to be poured
  3. Applicative: suffix “ət”
  4. Antipassive: “əđ” [5]
  5. Repetitive: add the prefix “ka-“
    Example: “arənđo” = to divide; “akarənđo” = to divide many times
  6. Simultaneous: add “ta-“ to the verb before the concord agreement
  7. Narrative/sequential: add “nə-“ to the verb before the concord agreement (2.1; 3)
  8. Adjective: if “-nano” is added to certain adjectives, they can turn into verbs
    Example: “gaicia” (=bad) + “nano” = gaicianano [17]
  9. Locative: use the suffix “e-“ or “i-“ in sccordance with the following vowels
    Example: é-lógopájá = in the cup; í-lútí = in the owl [5]

Writing system

Before the arrival of the first missionaries in 1936, Moro was written with Arabic letters, therefore Latin capital letters were not known by Moro speakers. [4] Missionaries created an orthography including capital letters and also translated the New Testament into Moro in the Ləŋorəban dialect. Today Moro consists of 22 consonants and 7 vowels with one of them being a Schwa. [18]

Moro alphabet [19]
A B C D Đ E Ë Ə F G I J K L M N Ñ Ŋ O P R S T U W Y
abcdđeëəfgijklmnñŋoprɽstuwy

Đ is typically used in its bowl-struck form, approximately

Dialects

There are 7 dialects of Moro (Guest 1997a). [20] Ethnologue names are given in parentheses. [1]

Each dialect corresponds to a separate clan, except for Ləmwarəŋ and Ləŋorəban, which are amalgamated into a single clan known as the Wërria. Thus, there is a total of 6 different clans. There is a lexical difference within the dialects, especially with the labials: “t” and “d” or “ɽ” and “t”. In other cases, the “b” of the Ləŋorəban dialect is noted as “f, v, w,” in other dialects.

Example: “vomit” in the Ləŋorəban dialect is: “biđu”, while in other dialects it is: “fiđu” or “wiđu”.

There are also significant semantic differences in the dialects.

Example: “majen” in Ləŋorəban means “now” but in other dialect refers to “a long time ago”.

This even leads to the fact, that speakers of the Tobəɽelda dialect are not able to understand the translation of the New Testament which was translated into the Ləŋorəban dialect. [18] An explanation for this might be, that the speakers of the Ləŋorəban dialect seem to be geographically isolated from speakers of other dialects and receive influence from other languages they are surrounded by such as Katcha or Utundi. [23]

Related Research Articles

Ganda or Luganda is a Bantu language spoken in the African Great Lakes region. It is one of the major languages in Uganda and is spoken by more than 5.56 million Baganda and other people principally in central Uganda, including the country's capital, Kampala. Typologically, it is an agglutinative, tonal language with subject–verb–object word order and nominative–accusative morphosyntactic alignment.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Halkomelem</span> Shalishan language

Halkomelem is a language of various First Nations peoples of the British Columbia Coast. It is spoken in what is now British Columbia, ranging from southeastern Vancouver Island from the west shore of Saanich Inlet northward beyond Gabriola Island and Nanaimo to Nanoose Bay and including the Lower Mainland from the Fraser River Delta upriver to Harrison Lake and the lower boundary of the Fraser Canyon.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Supyire language</span> Language

Supyire, or Suppire, is a Senufo language spoken in the Sikasso Region of southeastern Mali and in adjoining regions of Ivory Coast. In their native language, the noun sùpyìré means both "the people" and "the language spoken by the people".

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Klallam language</span> Salishan language of North America

Klallam,Clallam, Ns'Klallam or S'klallam, is a Straits Salishan language historically spoken by the Klallam people at Becher Bay on Vancouver Island in British Columbia and across the Strait of Juan de Fuca on the north coast of the Olympic Peninsula in Washington. The last native speaker of Klallam as a first language died in 2014, but there is a growing group of speakers of Klallam as a second language.

The Elgeyo language, or Kalenjin proper, are a dialect cluster of the Kalenjin branch of the Nilotic language family.

Tooro or Rutooro is a Bantu language spoken mainly by the Tooro people (Abatooro) from the Tooro Kingdom in western Uganda. There are three main areas where Tooro as a language is mainly used: Kabarole District, Kyenjojo District and Kyegegwa District. Tooro is unusual among Bantu languages as it lacks lexical tone. It is most closely related to Runyoro.

Dulong or Drung, Derung, Rawang, or Trung, is a Sino-Tibetan language in China. Dulong is closely related to the Rawang language of Myanmar (Burma). Although almost all ethnic Derung people speak the language to some degree, most are multilingual, also speaking Burmese, Lisu, and Mandarin Chinese except for a few very elderly people.

In Hebrew, verbs, which take the form of derived stems, are conjugated to reflect their tense and mood, as well as to agree with their subjects in gender, number, and person. Each verb has an inherent voice, though a verb in one voice typically has counterparts in other voices. This article deals mostly with Modern Hebrew, but to some extent, the information shown here applies to Biblical Hebrew as well.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Wayuu language</span> Major Arawakan language spoken in the Guajira Peninsula

Wayuu, or Guajiro, is a major Arawakan language spoken by 400,000 indigenous Wayuu people in northwestern Venezuela and northeastern Colombia on the Guajira Peninsula and surrounding Lake Maracaibo.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Nukak language</span> Endangered indigenous language of Colombia

The Nukak language is a language of uncertain classification, perhaps part of the macrofamily Puinave-Maku. It is very closely related to Kakwa.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Moloko language</span> Afro-Asiatic language spoken in Cameroon

Moloko (Məlokwo) is an Afro-Asiatic language spoken in northern Cameroon.

Universal is an Esperantido, a constructed language based on Esperanto. It has inclusive and exclusive pronouns, uses partial reduplication for the plural, and inversion for antonyms. Inversion can be seen in,

The grammar of the Otomi language displays a mixture of elements of synthetic and analytic structures. Particularly the phrase-level morphology is synthetic, whereas the sentence-level is analytic. Simultaneously, the language is head-marking in terms of its verbal morphology, but not in its nominal morphology, which is more analytic. Otomi recognizes three large open word classes of nouns, verbs, and particles. There is a small closed class of property words, variously analyzed as adjectives or stative verbs.

The Fwe language, also known as Chifwe, is a Bantu language spoken by the Fwe people in Namibia and Zambia. It is closely related to the Subia language, Chisubia, and is one of several Bantu languages that feature click consonants.

Old Norse has three categories of verbs and two categories of nouns. Conjugation and declension are carried out by a mix of inflection and two nonconcatenative morphological processes: umlaut, a backness-based alteration to the root vowel; and ablaut, a replacement of the root vowel, in verbs.

Zulu grammar is the way in which meanings are encoded into wordings in the Zulu language. Zulu grammar is typical for Bantu languages, bearing all the hallmarks of this language family. These include agglutinativity, a rich array of noun classes, extensive inflection for person, tense and aspect, and a subject–verb–object word order.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Paya language</span> Chibchan language of Honduras

Pech or Pesh is a Chibchan language spoken in Honduras. It was formerly known as Paya, and continues to be referred to in this manner by several sources, though there are negative connotations associated with this term. It has also been referred to as Seco. There are 300 speakers according to Yasugi (2007). It is spoken near the north-central coast of Honduras, in the Dulce Nombre de Culmí municipality of Olancho Department.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Jita language</span> Bantu language of Tanzania

Jita is a Bantu language of Tanzania, spoken on the southeastern shore of Lake Victoria/Nyanza and on the island of Ukerewe.

Turkmen grammar is the grammar of the Turkmen language, whose dialectal variants are spoken in Turkmenistan, Iran, Afghanistan, Russia, China, Uzbekistan, Tajikistan and others. Turkmen grammar, as described in this article, is the grammar of standard Turkmen as spoken and written by Turkmen people in Turkmenistan.

Levantine Arabic grammar is the set of rules by which Levantine Arabic creates statements, questions and commands. In many respects, it is quite similar to that of the other vernacular Arabic varieties.

References

  1. 1 2 Moro at Ethnologue (27th ed., 2024) Closed Access logo transparent.svg
  2. "Moro Language Project". moro.ucsd.edu. Retrieved 2023-01-31.
  3. 1 2 3 Gibbard, George, Rohde, Hannah, and Rose, Sharon (2009). 'Moro Noun Class Morphology'. In Masangu Matonodo et al. (eds.) Selected Proceedings of the 38th Annual Conference on African Linguistics, pp. 106-117. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings, p. 117. Project. http://www.lingref.com/cpp/acal/38/paper2139.pdf?q=moro
  4. 1 2 SUDAN LOCAL LANGUAGE CENTRE: Notes on Language Use in the Moro Community in Khartoum. p. 1.
  5. 1 2 3 Strabone, Andrew and Rose, Sharon (2012). 'Morphophonological properties of Moro causatives'. In Connell, Bruce and Rolle, Nicholas (eds.) Selected Proceedings of the 41st Annual Conference on African Linguistics: African Languages in Contact, pp. 92-103. Sommerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project. http://www.lingref.com/cpp/acal/41/paper2740.pdf
  6. Black, K.; Black, K. (1971). The Moro Language Grammar and Dictionary. Linguistic Monograph Series. Vol. 6. Khartoum: Sudan Research Unit, Faculty of Arts.
  7. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Jenks, Peter; Rose, Sharon (2011). "High Tone in Moro: Effects of Prosodic Categories and Morphological Domains". Natural Language and Linguistic Theory. 29: 211–250. doi: 10.1007/s11049-011-9120-x . S2CID   14067976.
  8. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Ritchart, Amanda; Rose, Sharon (2015). "Schwas in Moro Vowel Harmony". In Kramer, Ruth; Zsiga, Elizabeth; Tlale Boyer, One (eds.). Selected Proceedings of the 44th Annual Conference on African Linguistics (PDF). Cascadilla Proceedings Project. pp. 231–242.
  9. 1 2 Guest, Elizabeth. 1997: Moro Noun Classes. p. 1.
  10. 1 2 Guest, Elizabeth. 1998: Miscellaneous Moro Grammar. p. 1-2.
  11. 1 2 3 Guest, Elizabeth. 1997: Moro Verbs. p. 1-7.
  12. Guest, Elizabeth. 1997: Moro Verbs. p. 4-7.
  13. Guest, Elizabeth. 1998: Miscellaneous Moro Grammar. p. 5.
  14. Guest, Elizabeth. 1997: Moro Verbs. p. 1-3.
  15. Guest, Elizabeth. 1998: Miscellaneous Moro Grammar. p. 4.
  16. Jenks, Peter (2014). 'Head Movement in Moro DPs: Evidence for a Unified Theory of Movement'. In Santana-LaBarge, Robert E. (ed.) Proceedings of the 31st West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics, pp. 248-257. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project. https://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/document?repid=rep1&type=pdf&doi=3572bcf0d316e8ec04d2f68aebd218b1969ff915
  17. Guest, Elizabeth. 1998: Miscellaneous Moro Grammar. p. 3.
  18. 1 2 Guest, Elizabeth. 1997: Moro Phonology. p. 3.
  19. Guest 1997a.
  20. http://www.rogerblench.info/Language/Niger-Congo/Kordofanian/Moro/guest_moro-phonology1997.pdf [ bare URL PDF ]
  21. 1 2 Blench, Roger. 2005. A dictionary of the Moro language of the Nuba hills, Sudan. m.s.
  22. Moro Language Project
  23. Guest, Elizabeth. 1997: History of the Moro NT. p. 1.

Further reading