Psychological barriers to effective altruism

Last updated
Peter Singer is one of the prominent philosophers of effective altruism. Peter Singer no Fronteiras do Pensamento Porto Alegre (9616423447).jpg
Peter Singer is one of the prominent philosophers of effective altruism.

In the philosophy of effective altruism, an altruistic act such as charitable giving is considered more effective, or cost-effective, if it uses a set of resources to do more good per unit of resource than other options, with the goal of trying to do the most good. [1] Following this definition of effectiveness, researchers in psychology and related fields have identified psychological barriers to effective altruism that can cause people to choose less effective options when they engage in altruistic activities such as charitable giving. [2] [3] [4] These barriers can include evolutionary influences as well as motivational and epistemic obstacles.

Contents

Overview

In general, humans are motivated to do good things in the world, whether that is through donations to charity, volunteering time for a cause, or just lending a hand to someone who needs help. [5] [6] In 2022, approximately 4.2 billion people donated their money, time, or helped a stranger. [5] Donating money to charity is especially substantial. For instance, 2% of the GDP of the United States goes to charitable organizations—a total of more than $450 billion in annual donations. [7] Despite the human tendency and motivation to give and engage in altruistic behavior, research has shed light on an unequal motivation to give effectively. [8]

Humans are motivated to give, but often not motivated to give most effectively. [3] In the title of an article published in Nature Human Behaviour in 2020, Bethany Burum, Martin Nowak, and Moshe Hoffman termed this phenomenon ineffective altruism, that is, relatively less sensitivity to cost-effectiveness in altruistic behaviour. [3] [9] In the domain of business decisions, investors look for how much return they will get for each dollar they invest. However, when it comes to the domain of altruistic decision-making, this line of thinking is far less common. [2] Most donors seem to prioritize giving to charitable organizations that spend the least possible amount on running costs in the hopes of having more of their donation reach the destination. [10] [11]

Evolutionary explanations

Martin Nowak is a professor at Harvard University whose research contributions include the theory of evolution and cooperation. Dr. Martin Nowak.jpg
Martin Nowak is a professor at Harvard University whose research contributions include the theory of evolution and cooperation.

While plenty of studies in the behavioral sciences have demonstrated the cognitive and emotional limitations in charitable giving, some argue that the reasons behind ineffective giving run deeper. [12] [3] A study by Martin Nowak and fellow academics at Harvard University and the Massachusetts Institute of Technology suggested that the human tendency to ineffective altruism can be explained through evolutionary motives and evolutionary game theory. [3] They argue that society rewards the act of giving but generally provides no motivation or incentive to give effectively. Past research suggests that altruistic motives are distorted by, among other things, parochialism, status seeking and conformity. [13] [14] [12]

Parochialism

People are sensitive to effectiveness when they or their kin are at stake, [13] but not so much when confronted with a needy stranger. [3] [15] [16] Donors have been shown to respond to impact and efficacy when giving to themselves, but less so when donating to charity. [3] [12] While cost-effectiveness information of charities tends to be hard to evaluate, [17] [18] studies have shown that people are less scope insensitive when the beneficiaries are family members. [3]

Throughout human evolutionary history, residing in small, tightly-knit groups has given rise to prosocial emotions and intentions towards kin and ingroup members, rather than universally extending to those outside the group boundaries. [19] [20] Humans tend to exhibit parochial tendencies, showing concern for their in-groups, but not out-groups. [12] [3] This parochial inclination can hinder effective altruism, especially as a significant portion of human suffering occurs in distant regions. [18] [21] Despite the potential impact of donations in different parts of the world, individuals in rich and developed countries often view assistance to physically distant others as less important than helping those in close proximity. [21] [12] [3] Contrary to maximizing impact and effectiveness with their donations, many individuals commit to donating money to local charities and organizations to which they have a personal connection, thus living by the notion of "charity begins at home." [8] [22] Similarly, people are more inclined to help a needy child from their neighborhood rather than their city or country. [23]

Status seeking

Humans assign value to their social status within a group for survival and reproduction. [16] People tend to pursue high-status positions to enjoy benefits, such as desirable mating partners. [24] Therefore, behaviors that can produce reputational benefits are desirable to enhance one's standing in society. [25] Altruistic acts are generally viewed positively, [26] yield social rewards, [13] [3] and are cumulative. [27] However, effective altruism, that is, altruistic behavior that focuses on maximizing others' welfare, is often not socially rewarded. [3] [28] Evidence-based reasoning in charitable giving may be perceived negatively, as amoral, and so will reduce a person's likability. [29] Some have even argued that the reputational costs incurred for engaging in effective giving explain people's aversion to prioritizing some causes over more impactful ones. [4]

Conformity

Many living organisms have demonstrated conformity, [30] [31] that is, the tendency to use dominant group norms (or descriptive norms) as guiding rules of behavior. Research on humans has also shown that social norms have the power to influence what others do. [32] In the judgment and decision-making research, this observation has come to be known as the bandwagon effect. The power of this bias has also been demonstrated in the field of charitable giving. In fact, people have been shown to donate more, or to exhibit an increased likelihood to donate, when they perceived donating to charity as the social norm or the default choice. [33] Therefore, the fact that many people become increasingly in favor of donating to ineffective options, then society will see the creation of a norm for people to give ineffectively. [12] As a result, people rely more strongly on their intuitions [34] which lead them to choosing to give ineffectively simply because they know that most others would do the same thing. [12]

Motivational obstacles

Subjective preferences

People often prioritize giving to charities that align with their subjectively preferred causes. [8] Commonly, people believe charity to be a subjective decision which should not be motivated by numbers, but by care for the cause given the lack of responsibility attributed to the effects of donations. [35] This aligns with the theory of warm-glow giving originally proposed by the economist James Andreoni. According to Andreoni (1990), individuals gain satisfaction from the act of giving but are not concerned about the benefits generated by their act. [36] [8]

Narrow moral circle

Moral circle expansion is the concept of increasing one's number and kind of subjects deserving of moral concern over time. [37] The establishment of one's moral circle depends on spatial, biological, and temporal proximity. [37] For instance, many donors in WEIRD countries tend to favor charities that conduct work within their respective geographical boundaries. [2] In terms of biological distance, people favor donating money to help humans instead of animals, even in cases when animals can have equal cognitive and suffering capacities. [38] [39] The idea of temporal proximity relates to people's tendency to prefer helping current generations over future ones. [40] [41]

Scope neglect (insensitivity)

Scope neglect (or scope insensitivity) is the idea that people are numb to the number of victims in large, high-stake humanitarian situations. [42] [43] Some research has compared this cognitive bias to the economic concept of diminishing marginal utility wherein people demonstrate a decreasing non-linear concern for individuals as the number of people increases. [43]

Epistemic obstacles

Overhead aversion

Donors are averse to giving to charities that devote a lot of their expenses to administration [44] or running costs. [11] [17] Several studies have demonstrated the ubiquitous effect of overhead aversion which is commonly attributed to people's conflation between overhead spending and charity cost-effectiveness (or impact). [44] [11] [17] Furthermore, some have argued that when donors learn that a charity uses their donation to fund running costs, donors experience a diminished feeling of warm-glow, [36] which is a significant driver of donation behavior. [44]

Quantifiability scepticism

Demonstration of quality-adjusted life years (QALYs) for two individuals. Individual A (who did not receive an intervention) has fewer QALYs than individual B (who received an intervention). QALY graph-en.svg
Demonstration of quality-adjusted life years (QALYs) for two individuals. Individual A (who did not receive an intervention) has fewer QALYs than individual B (who received an intervention).

Intangible outcomes (such as health interventions, charity effectiveness) are hard to quantify, and many people doubt that they can every be quantified and compared. [4] However, in disciplines such as health economics, health outcomes and interventions are quantified and evaluated using metrics such as quality-adjusted life years (QALYs). [45] In a similar vein, happiness economists have developed the concept of wellbeing-years (WELLBYs) which evaluates effectiveness in terms of life-years lived up to full life satisfaction. [46] Put simply, a WELLBY is given by:

Where is the number of lives remaining from the region's life expectancy and is the change in life satisfaction expected to result from a particular action or intervention. [47] Thus, charity cost-effectiveness analyses use a number of different measures grounded in academic research to quantify their impact, allowing direct comparisons of charities that address multiple causes. [17] [4]

Limited awareness

The effective altruism movement does substantial work on identifying the world's most effective charities through charity evaluators such as GiveWell, Giving What We Can, and Animal Charity Evaluators. However, many people are unaware of these organizations and the charities they evaluate, [17] and are strongly driven by emotional responses when estimating the effectiveness of a charity; [17] choosing instead to prioritize those causes to which they have a personal connection. [8]

See also

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Altruism</span> Principle or practice of concern for the welfare of others

Altruism is the principle and practice of concern for the well-being and/or happiness of other humans or animals above oneself. While objects of altruistic concern vary, it is an important moral value in many cultures and religions. It may be considered a synonym of selflessness, the opposite of selfishness.

Sympathy is the perception of, understanding of, and reaction to the distress or need of another life form.

Reciprocity is a crucial aspect of how people interact and live in society but researchers who study these interactions have often overlooked its importance. Reciprocity, as a fundamental principle in social psychology, revolves around the concept that individuals tend to respond to the actions of others in a manner that mirrors the positive or negative nature of those actions. It involves a mutual exchange of behaviors and reactions, where individuals reciprocate the same type of behavior they have received from others. People's choices in how they behave are mostly based on what they can gain from others in return, while feelings of trust, liking, and togetherness are strongly influenced by the idea of giving and receiving equally

The norm of reciprocity requires that people repay in kind what others have done for them. It can be understood as the expectation that people will respond to each other by returning benefits for benefits, and with either indifference or hostility to harms. The social norm of reciprocity may take different forms in different areas of social life, or in different societies. This is distinct from related ideas such as gratitude, the Golden Rule, or mutual goodwill. See reciprocity for an analysis of the concepts involved.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Charity (practice)</span> Voluntary giving of help to those in need

The practice of charity, which is the voluntary provision of assistance to those in need, serves as a humanitarian act, and is unmotivated by self-interest. Various philosophies about charity exist, with frequent associations with religion.

Prosocial behavior, or intent to benefit others, is a social behavior that "benefit[s] other people or society as a whole", "such as helping, sharing, donating, co-operating, and volunteering". Obeying the rules and conforming to socially accepted behaviors are also regarded as prosocial behaviors. These actions may be motivated by empathy and by concern about the welfare and rights of others, as well as for egoistic or practical concerns, such as one's social status or reputation, hope for direct or indirect reciprocity, or adherence to one's perceived system of fairness. It may also be motivated by altruism, though the existence of pure altruism is somewhat disputed, and some have argued that this falls into philosophical rather than psychological realm of debate. Evidence suggests that pro sociality is central to the well-being of social groups across a range of scales, including schools. Prosocial behavior in the classroom can have a significant impact on a student's motivation for learning and contributions to the classroom and larger community. In the workplace, prosocial behaviour can have a significant impact on team psychological safety, as well as positive indirect effects on employee's helping behaviors and task performance. Empathy is a strong motive in eliciting prosocial behavior, and has deep evolutionary roots.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Dean Karlan</span> American economist

Dean Karlan is an American development economist. He is Professor of Economics and Finance at Northwestern University where, alongside Christopher Udry, he co-founded and co-directs the Global Poverty Research Lab at Kellogg School of Management. Karlan is the president and founder of Innovations for Poverty Action (IPA), a New Haven, Connecticut, based research outfit dedicated to creating and evaluating solutions to social and international development problems. He is also a Research Fellow and member of the Executive Committee of the board of directors at the Abdul Latif Jameel Poverty Action Lab (J-PAL) at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Along with economists Jonathan Morduch and Sendhil Mullainathan, Karlan served as director of the Financial Access Initiative (FAI), a consortium of researchers focused on substantially expanding access to quality financial services for low-income individuals.

Humanity is a virtue linked with altruistic ethics derived from the human condition. It signifies human love and compassion towards each other. Humanity differs from mere justice in that there is a level of altruism towards individuals included in humanity more so than in the fairness found in justice. That is, humanity, and the acts of love, altruism, and social intelligence are typically individual strengths while fairness is generally expanded to all. Humanity is one of six virtues that are consistent across all cultures.

Warm-glow giving is an economic theory describing the emotional reward of giving to others. According to the original warm-glow model developed by James Andreoni, people experience a sense of joy and satisfaction for "doing their part" to help others. This satisfaction - or "warm glow" - represents the selfish pleasure derived from "doing good", regardless of the actual impact of one's generosity. Within the warm-glow framework, people may be "impurely altruistic", meaning they simultaneously maintain both altruistic and egoistic (selfish) motivations for giving. This may be partially due to the fact that "warm glow" sometimes gives people credit for the contributions they make, such as a plaque with their name or a system where they can make donations publicly so other people know the "good" they are doing for the community.

Social preferences describe the human tendency to not only care about one's own material payoff, but also the reference group's payoff or/and the intention that leads to the payoff. Social preferences are studied extensively in behavioral and experimental economics and social psychology. Types of social preferences include altruism, fairness, reciprocity, and inequity aversion. The field of economics originally assumed that humans were rational economic actors, and as it became apparent that this was not the case, the field began to change. The research of social preferences in economics started with lab experiments in 1980, where experimental economists found subjects' behavior deviated systematically from self-interest behavior in economic games such as ultimatum game and dictator game. These experimental findings then inspired various new economic models to characterize agent's altruism, fairness and reciprocity concern between 1990 and 2010. More recently, there are growing amounts of field experiments that study the shaping of social preference and its applications throughout society.

Third-party punishment, or altruistic punishment, is punishment of a transgressor which is administered, not by a victim of the transgression, but rather by a third party not directly affected by the transgression. It has been argued that third-party punishments are the essence of social norms, as they are an evolutionarily stable strategy, unlike second-party punishments. It has also been shown that third-party punishments are exhibited in all examined populations, though the magnitude of the punishments varies greatly, and that costly punishment co-varies with altruistic behavior. Differences between within-group and inter-group altruistic punishments have also been observed.

Nudge theory is a concept in behavioral economics, decision making, behavioral policy, social psychology, consumer behavior, and related behavioral sciences that proposes adaptive designs of the decision environment as ways to influence the behavior and decision-making of groups or individuals. Nudging contrasts with other ways to achieve compliance, such as education, legislation or enforcement.

Effective altruism is a 21st-century philosophical and social movement that advocates "using evidence and reason to figure out how to benefit others as much as possible, and taking action on that basis". People who pursue the goals of effective altruism, sometimes called effective altruists, may choose careers based on the amount of good that they expect the career to achieve or donate to charities based on the goal of maximising positive impact. They may work on the prioritization of scientific projects, entrepreneurial ventures, and policy initiatives estimated to save the most lives or reduce the most suffering.

Earning to give involves deliberately pursuing a high-earning career for the purpose of donating a significant portion of earned income, typically because of a desire to do effective altruism. Advocates of earning to give contend that maximizing the amount one can donate to charity is an important consideration for individuals when deciding what career to pursue.

Elevation is an emotion elicited by witnessing actual or imagined virtuous acts of remarkable moral goodness. It is experienced as a distinct feeling of warmth and expansion that is accompanied by appreciation and affection for the individual whose exceptional conduct is being observed. Elevation motivates those who experience it to open up to, affiliate with, and assist others. Elevation makes an individual feel lifted up and optimistic about humanity.

<i>The Most Good You Can Do</i> 2015 book by Peter Singer

The Most Good You Can Do: How Effective Altruism Is Changing Ideas About Living Ethically is a 2015 Yale University Press book by moral philosopher and bioethicist Peter Singer describing and arguing for the ideas of effective altruism. As a follow-up to The Life You Can Save, which makes the moral argument for donating money to improve the lives of people in extreme poverty, the new book focuses on the broader question of how to do the most good.

Moral emotions are a variety of social emotions that are involved in forming and communicating moral judgments and decisions, and in motivating behavioral responses to one's own and others' moral behavior. As defined by Jonathan Haidt, moral emotions "are linked to the interests or welfare either of a society as a whole or at least of persons other than the judge or agent". A person may not always have clear words to articulate, yet simultaneously, that same person knows it to be true deep down inside.

Compassion fade is the tendency to experience a decrease in empathy as the number of people in need of aid increase. As a type of cognitive bias, it has a significant effect on the prosocial behaviour from which helping behaviour generates. The term was developed by psychologist and researcher Paul Slovic.

Do-gooder derogation is a phenomenon where a person's morally motivated behavior leads to them being perceived negatively by others. The term "do-gooder" refers to a person who deviates from the majority in terms of behavior, because of their morality.

Parochial altruism is a concept in the fields of social psychology, evolutionary biology, and anthropology that describes altruism towards an in-group, often accompanied by hostility towards an out-group. It is a combination of altruism, defined as behavior done for the benefit of others without direct effect to the self, and parochialism, which refers to having a limited viewpoint. Together, these concepts create parochial altruism, or altruism that is limited in scope to one's in-group. Parochial altruism is closely related to the concepts of in-group favoritism and out-group discrimination. Research has suggested that parochial altruism may have evolved in humans to promote high levels of in-group cooperation, which is advantageous for group survival. Parochial altruism is often evoked to explain social behaviors within and between groups, such as why people are cooperative within their social groups and why they may be aggressive towards other social groups.

References

  1. Pummer, Theron; MacAskill, William (June 2020). "Effective altruism". In LaFollette, Hugh (ed.). International Encyclopedia of Ethics. Hoboken, New Jersey: John Wiley & Sons. pp. 1–9. doi:10.1002/9781444367072.wbiee883. ISBN   9781444367072. OCLC   829259960. S2CID   241220220.
  2. 1 2 3 Baron, Jonathan; Szymanska, Ewa (2011). "Heuristics and Biases in Charity". In Oppenheimer, Daniel M.; Olivola, Christopher Yves (eds.). The Science of Giving: Experimental Approaches to the Study of Charity. The Society for Judgment and Decision Making series. New York: Psychology Press. pp. 215–235. doi:10.4324/9780203865972-24. ISBN   9781138981430. OCLC   449889661.
  3. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 Burum, Bethany; Nowak, Martin A.; Hoffman, Moshe (December 2020). "An evolutionary explanation for ineffective altruism". Nature Human Behaviour. 4 (12): 1245–1257. doi:10.1038/s41562-020-00950-4. ISSN   2397-3374. PMID   33046859. S2CID   222318993.
  4. 1 2 3 4 Caviola, Lucius; Schubert, Stefan; Greene, Joshua D. (July 2021). "The Psychology of (In)Effective Altruism". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 25 (7): 596–607. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2021.03.015 . ISSN   1364-6613. PMID   33962844.
  5. 1 2 "CAF World Giving Index 2023" (PDF). Charities Aid Foundation (CAF). 2023. Retrieved 14 November 2023.
  6. "World Giving Index 2022: A global view of giving trends" (PDF). Charities Aid Foundation (CAF). 2022. Retrieved 10 November 2023.
  7. Giving USA 2020: The Annual Report on Philanthropy for the Year 2019. Giving USA Foundation. 2020. ISBN   9780998746654.
  8. 1 2 3 4 5 Berman, Jonathan Z.; Barasch, Alixandra; Levine, Emma E.; Small, Deborah A. (May 2018). "Impediments to Effective Altruism: The Role of Subjective Preferences in Charitable Giving". Psychological Science. 29 (5): 834–844. doi:10.1177/0956797617747648. ISSN   0956-7976. PMID   29659341. S2CID   4901791 via Association for Psychological Science.
  9. Pummer, Theron (2023). The Rules of Rescue: Cost, Distance and Effective Altruism. Oxford: Oxford University Press. pp. 206–207. ISBN   9780190884147.
  10. Lewis, Joshua; Small, Deborah (2018). Gershoff, Andrew; Kozinets, Robert; White, Tiffany (eds.). "Ineffective Altruism: Giving Less When Donations Do More". NA - Advances in Consumer Research. Duluth, Minnesota: Association for Consumer Research. 46: 194–198.
  11. 1 2 3 Caviola, Lucius; Faulmüller, Nadira; Everett, Jim A. C.; Savulescu, Julian; Kahane, Guy (July 2014). "The evaluability bias in charitable giving: Saving administration costs or saving lives?". Judgment and Decision Making. 9 (4): 303–315. doi: 10.1017/S1930297500006185 . S2CID   18730753. ProQuest   1548669952.
  12. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Jaeger, Bastian; van Vugt, Mark (April 2022). "Psychological barriers to effective altruism: An evolutionary perspective". Current Opinion in Psychology. 44: 130–134. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2021.09.008 . PMID   34628365. S2CID   238582556 via Elsevier Science Direct.
  13. 1 2 3 Nowak, M. A. (2006). "Five rules for the evolution of cooperation". Science. 314 (5805): 1560–1563. Bibcode:2006Sci...314.1560N. doi:10.1126/science.1133755. PMC   3279745 . PMID   17158317.
  14. Panchanathan, Karthik; Boyd, Robert (November 2004). "Indirect reciprocity can stabilize cooperation without the second-order free rider problem". Nature. 432 (7016): 499–502. Bibcode:2004Natur.432..499P. doi:10.1038/nature02978. ISSN   1476-4687. PMID   15565153. S2CID   4373929.
  15. Hamilton, W. D. (September 1963). "The Evolution of Altruistic Behavior". The American Naturalist. 97 (896): 354–356. doi:10.1086/497114. ISSN   0003-0147. S2CID   84216415 via The University of Chicago Press Journals.
  16. 1 2 Darwin, C. (1859). On the Origin of Species by Means of Natural Selection, or, the Preservation of Favoured Races in the Struggle for Life. P. F. Collier & Son.
  17. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Caviola, Lucius; Schubert, Stefan; Nemirow, Jason (March 2020). "The many obstacles to effective giving". Judgment and Decision Making. 15 (2): 159–172. doi: 10.1017/S1930297500007312 . ISSN   1930-2975.
  18. 1 2 Singer, Peter (2009). The Life You Can Save: Acting Now to End World Poverty (1st ed.). United States: Random House. ISBN   978-1-4000-6710-7.
  19. Aktipis, Athena; Cronk, Lee; Alcock, Joe; Ayers, Jessica D.; Baciu, Cristina; Balliet, Daniel; Boddy, Amy M.; Curry, Oliver Scott; Krems, Jaimie Arona; Muñoz, Andrés; Sullivan, Daniel; Sznycer, Daniel; Wilkinson, Gerald S.; Winfrey, Pamela (July 2018). "Understanding cooperation through fitness interdependence". Nature Human Behaviour. 2 (7): 429–431. doi:10.1038/s41562-018-0378-4. hdl: 1871.1/72e0524e-788d-4f93-90f8-a6f04369a2a7 . ISSN   2397-3374. PMID   31097813. S2CID   49667807.
  20. Greene, Joshua (2013). Moral Tribes: Emotion, Reason, and the Gap Between Us and Them. New York, NY: Penguin Press. ISBN   978-0-14-312605-8.
  21. 1 2 Bloom, Paul (January 2017). "Empathy and Its Discontents". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 21 (1): 24–31. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2016.11.004. ISSN   1364-6613. PMID   27916513. S2CID   3863278 via Elsevier Science Direct.
  22. Adleberg, Toni; Surani, Faiz; GWWC, Team (May 2021). "Charity begins at home; shouldn't we solve our own problems before helping others?". Giving What We Can. Retrieved 31 October 2023.
  23. Kogut, Tehila; Ritov, Ilana; Rubaltelli, Enrico; Liberman, Nira (September 2018). "How far is the suffering? The role of psychological distance and victims' identifiability in donation decisions". Judgment and Decision Making. 13 (5): 458–466. doi: 10.1017/S1930297500008731 . hdl: 11577/3286801 . ISSN   1930-2975.
  24. von Rueden, Christopher R.; Jaeggi, Adrian V. (2016-09-27). "Men's status and reproductive success in 33 nonindustrial societies: Effects of subsistence, marriage system, and reproductive strategy". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 113 (39): 10824–10829. Bibcode:2016PNAS..11310824V. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1606800113 . ISSN   0027-8424. PMC   5047206 . PMID   27601650.
  25. Schaller, Mark; Kenrick, Douglas T.; Neel, Rebecca; Neuberg, Steven L. (June 2017). "Evolution and human motivation: A fundamental motives framework". Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 11 (6). doi:10.1111/spc3.12319. ISSN   1751-9004 via Wiley.
  26. Durkee, Patrick K.; Lukaszewski, Aaron W.; Buss, David M. (September 2020). "Psychological foundations of human status allocation". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 117 (35): 21235–21241. Bibcode:2020PNAS..11721235D. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2006148117 . ISSN   0027-8424. PMC   7474695 . PMID   32817486.
  27. Ashraf, Nava; Bandiera, Oriana (May 2017). "Altruistic Capital". American Economic Review. 107 (5): 70–75. doi:10.1257/aer.p20171097. ISSN   0002-8282 via American Economic Association.
  28. Yudkin, Daniel A.; Prosser, Annayah M. B.; Crockett, Molly J. (October 2019). "Actions speak louder than outcomes in judgments of prosocial behavior". Emotion. 19 (7): 1138–1147. doi:10.1037/emo0000514. ISSN   1931-1516. PMID   30475039. S2CID   53746918.
  29. Montealegre, Andres; Bush, Lance; Moss, David; Pizarro, David; Jimenez-Leal, William (2023). "Does Maximizing Good Make People Look Bad?". osf.io. Retrieved 2023-11-22.
  30. Boyd, Robert; Richerson, Peter J.; Henrich, Joseph (2011-06-28). "The cultural niche: Why social learning is essential for human adaptation". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 108 (Suppl 2): 10918–10925. Bibcode:2011PNAS..10810918B. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1100290108 . ISSN   0027-8424. PMC   3131818 . PMID   21690340.
  31. Muthukrishna, Michael; Morgan, Thomas J. H.; Henrich, Joseph (2016-01-01). "The when and who of social learning and conformist transmission". Evolution and Human Behavior. 37 (1): 10–20. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2015.05.004. ISSN   1090-5138.
  32. Pike, Thomas W.; Laland, Kevin N. (2010-08-23). "Conformist learning in nine-spined sticklebacks' foraging decisions". Biology Letters. 6 (4): 466–468. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2009.1014. ISSN   1744-9561. PMC   2936200 . PMID   20129948.
  33. Everett, Jim A.C.; Caviola, Lucius; Kahane, Guy; Savulescu, Julian; Faber, Nadira S. (March 2015). "Doing good by doing nothing? The role of social norms in explaining default effects in altruistic contexts". European Journal of Social Psychology. 45 (2): 230–241. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2080 . ISSN   0046-2772.
  34. Croson, Rachel; Handy, Femida; Shang, Jen (June 2009). "Keeping up with the Joneses: The relationship of perceived descriptive social norms, social information, and charitable giving". Nonprofit Management and Leadership. 19 (4): 467–489. doi:10.1002/nml.232. ISSN   1048-6682.
  35. Lerner, J.S.; Tetlock, P.E. (1999). "Accounting for the effects of accountability". Psychol Bull. 125 (2): 255–275. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.125.2.255. PMID   10087938 via PubMed.
  36. 1 2 Andreoni, James (1990). "Impure altruism and donations to public goods: A theory of warm-glow giving". The Economic Journal. 100 (401): 464–477. doi:10.2307/2234133. JSTOR   2234133. S2CID   6001457.
  37. 1 2 Anthis, Jacy Reese; Paez, Eze (2021-06-01). "Moral circle expansion: A promising strategy to impact the far future". Futures. 130: 102756. doi: 10.1016/j.futures.2021.102756 . ISSN   0016-3287.
  38. Caviola, Lucius; Everett, Jim A. C.; Faber, Nadira S. (June 2019). "The moral standing of animals: Towards a psychology of speciesism". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 116 (6): 1011–1029. doi:10.1037/pspp0000182. ISSN   1939-1315. PMID   29517258. S2CID   3818419.
  39. Caviola, Lucius; Kahane, Guy; Everett, Jim A. C.; Teperman, Elliot; Savulescu, Julian; Faber, Nadira S. (May 2021). "Utilitarianism for animals, Kantianism for people? Harming animals and humans for the greater good". Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 150 (5): 1008–1039. doi:10.1037/xge0000988. ISSN   1939-2222. PMID   33074696.
  40. Schubert, Stefan; Caviola, Lucius; Faber, Nadira S. (2019). "The Psychology of Existential Risk: Moral Judgments about Human Extinction". Scientific Reports. 9 (15100). Bibcode:2019NatSR...915100S. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-50145-9 . PMC   6803761 . PMID   31636277 via Nature.
  41. MacAskill, William (2022). What We Owe the Future (1st ed.). Basic Books. ISBN   978-1541618626.
  42. Yudkowsky, Eliezer (13 May 2007). "Scope Insensitivity". lesswrong.com. Retrieved 16 October 2023.
  43. 1 2 Dickert, Stephan; Västfjäll, Daniel; Kleber, Janet; Slovic, Paul (September 2015). "Scope insensitivity: The limits of intuitive valuation of human lives in public policy". Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition. 4 (3): 248–255. doi: 10.1016/j.jarmac.2014.09.002 . hdl: 1794/19441 . ISSN   2211-369X.
  44. 1 2 3 Gneezy, U.; Keenan, E. A.; Gneezy, A. (2014-10-30). "Avoiding overhead aversion in charity". Science. 346 (6209): 632–635. Bibcode:2014Sci...346..632G. doi:10.1126/science.1253932. ISSN   0036-8075. PMID   25359974. S2CID   206557384.
  45. Banerjee, Abhijit V.; Duflo, Esther (2011). Poor Economics: A Radical Rethinking of the Way to Fight Global Poverty. United States: PublicAffairs. ISBN   978-1-58648-798-0.
  46. De Neve, Jan-Emmanuel; Clark, Andrew E.; Krekel, Christian; Layard, Richard; O’Donnell, Gus (2020-10-05). "Taking a wellbeing years approach to policy choice". BMJ. 371: m3853. doi:10.1136/bmj.m3853. ISSN   1756-1833. PMID   33020062. S2CID   222125497.
  47. Treasury, HM (2021). "Wellbeing Guidance for Appraisal: Supplementary Green Book Guidance" (PDF). HM Treasury. Retrieved 11 November 2023.