Altruism

Last updated

Giving alms to the poor is often considered an altruistic action. Belisaire demandant l'aumone Jacques-Louis David.jpg
Giving alms to the poor is often considered an altruistic action.

Altruism is the principle and practice of concern for the well-being and/or happiness of other humans or animals above oneself. While objects of altruistic concern vary, it is an important moral value in many cultures and religions. It may be considered a synonym of selflessness, the opposite of selfishness.

Contents

The word altruism was popularized (and possibly coined) by the French philosopher Auguste Comte in French, as altruisme, for an antonym of egoism. [1] He derived it from the Italian altrui, which in turn was derived from Latin alteri, meaning "other people" or "somebody else". [2]

Altruism, as observed in populations of organisms, is when an individual performs an action at a cost to themselves (in terms of e.g. pleasure and quality of life, time, probability of survival or reproduction) that benefits, directly or indirectly, another individual, without the expectation of reciprocity or compensation for that action.

Altruism can be distinguished from feelings of loyalty or concern for the common good. The latter are predicated upon social relationships, whilst altruism does not consider relationships. Whether "true" altruism is possible in human psychology is a subject of debate. The theory of psychological egoism suggests that no act of sharing, helping, or sacrificing can be truly altruistic, as the actor may receive an intrinsic reward in the form of personal gratification. The validity of this argument depends on whether such intrinsic rewards qualify as "benefits".

The term altruism may also refer to an ethical doctrine that claims that individuals are morally obliged to benefit others. Used in this sense, it is usually contrasted with egoism, which claims individuals are morally obligated to serve themselves first.

Effective altruism is the use of evidence and reason to determine the most effective ways to benefit others.

The notion of altruism

The concept of altruism has a history in philosophical and ethical thought. The term was coined in the 19th century by the founding sociologist and philosopher of science Auguste Comte, and has become a major topic for psychologists (especially evolutionary psychology researchers), evolutionary biologists, and ethologists. Whilst ideas about altruism from one field can affect the other fields, the different methods and focuses of these fields always lead to different perspectives on altruism. In simple terms, altruism is caring about the welfare of other people and acting to help them, above oneself.

Scientific viewpoints

Anthropology

Marcel Mauss's essay The Gift contains a passage called "Note on alms". This note describes the evolution of the notion of alms (and by extension of altruism) from the notion of sacrifice. In it, he writes:

Alms are the fruits of a moral notion of the gift and of fortune on the one hand, and of a notion of sacrifice, on the other. Generosity is an obligation, because Nemesis avenges the poor and the gods for the superabundance of happiness and wealth of certain people who should rid themselves of it. This is the ancient morality of the gift, which has become a principle of justice. The gods and the spirits accept that the share of wealth and happiness that has been offered to them and had been hitherto destroyed in useless sacrifices should serve the poor and children.

Evolutionary explanations

Giving alms to beggar children Manner of Francis Wheatley, Giving alms to beggar children.jpg
Giving alms to beggar children

In the Science of ethology (the study of animal behaviour), and more generally in the study of social evolution, altruism refers to behavior by an individual that increases the fitness of another individual while decreasing the fitness of the actor. [3] In evolutionary psychology this term may be applied to a wide range of human behaviors such as charity, emergency aid, help to coalition partners, tipping, courtship gifts, production of public goods, and environmentalism. [4]

Theories of apparently altruistic behavior were accelerated[ clarification needed ] by the need to produce ideas compatible with evolutionary origins. Two related strands of research on altruism have emerged from traditional evolutionary analyses and evolutionary game theory: a mathematical model and analysis of behavioral strategies.

Some of the proposed mechanisms are:

Hunters who share meat display a costly signal of ability. The research found that good hunters have higher reproductive success and more adulterous relations even if they receive no more of the hunted meat than anyone else. Similarly, holding large feasts and giving large donations are ways of demonstrating one's resources. Heroic risk-taking has also been interpreted as a costly signal of ability. [4]
Volunteers assist Hurricane victims at the Houston Astrodome, following Hurricane Katrina. FEMA - 15337 - Photograph by Andrea Booher taken on 09-10-2005 in Texas.jpg
Volunteers assist Hurricane victims at the Houston Astrodome, following Hurricane Katrina.
Both indirect reciprocity and costly signaling depend on reputation value and tend to make similar predictions. One is that people will be more helpful when they know that their helping behavior will be communicated to people they will interact with later, publicly announced, discussed, or observed by someone else. This has been documented in many studies. The effect is sensitive to subtle cues, such as people being more helpful when there were stylized eyespots instead of a logo on a computer screen. [ dubious ] Weak reputational cues such as eyespots may become unimportant if there are stronger cues present and may lose their effect with continued exposure unless reinforced with real reputational effects. [4] Public displays such as public weeping for dead celebrities and participation in demonstrations may be influenced by a desire to be seen as generous. People who know that they are publicly monitored sometimes even wastefully donate the money they know is not needed by the recipient because of reputational concerns. [12]
Typically, women find altruistic men to be attractive partners. When women look for a long-term partner, altruism may be a trait they prefer as it may indicate that the prospective partner is also willing to share resources with her and her children. Men perform charitable acts in the early stages of a romantic relationship or simply when in the presence of an attractive woman. While both sexes state that kindness is the most preferable trait in a partner, there is some evidence that men place less value on this than women and that women may not be more altruistic in the presence of an attractive man. Men may even avoid altruistic women in short-term relationships, which may be because they expect less success. [4] [12]
People may compete for the social benefit of a burnished reputation, which may cause competitive altruism. On the other hand, in some experiments, a proportion of people do not seem to care about reputation and do not help more, even if this is conspicuous. This may be due to reasons such as psychopathy or that they are so attractive that they need not be seen as altruistic. The reputational benefits of altruism occur in the future compared to the immediate costs of altruism. While humans and other organisms generally place less value on future costs/benefits as compared to those in the present, some have shorter time horizons than others, and these people tend to be less cooperative. [4]
Explicit extrinsic rewards and punishments have sometimes been found to have a counterintuitively inverse effect on behaviors when compared to intrinsic rewards. This may be because such extrinsic incentives may replace (partially or in whole) intrinsic and reputational incentives, motivating the person to focus on obtaining the extrinsic rewards, which may make the thus-incentivized behaviors less desirable. People prefer altruism in others when it appears to be due to a personality characteristic rather than overt reputational concerns; simply pointing out that there are reputational benefits of action may reduce them. This may be used as a derogatory tactic against altruists ("you're just virtue signalling"), especially by those who are non-cooperators. A counterargument is that doing good due to reputational concerns is better than doing no good. [4]
Helping the homeless in New York City Helping the homeless (cropped).jpg
Helping the homeless in New York City

Such explanations do not imply that humans consciously calculate how to increase their inclusive fitness when doing altruistic acts. Instead, evolution has shaped psychological mechanisms, such as emotions, that promote certain altruistic behaviors. [4]

The benefits for the altruist may be increased, and the costs reduced by being more altruistic towards certain groups. Research has found that people are more altruistic to kin than to no-kin, to friends than strangers, to those attractive than to those unattractive, to non-competitors than competitors, and to members in-groups than to members of out-groups. [4]

The study of altruism was the initial impetus behind George R. Price's development of the Price equation, a mathematical equation used to study genetic evolution. An interesting example of altruism is found in the cellular slime moulds, such as Dictyostelium mucoroides. These protists live as individual amoebae until starved, at which point they aggregate and form a multicellular fruiting body in which some cells sacrifice themselves to promote the survival of other cells in the fruiting body. [14]

Selective investment theory proposes that close social bonds, and associated emotional, cognitive, and neurohormonal mechanisms, evolved to facilitate long-term, high-cost altruism between those closely depending on one another for survival and reproductive success. [15]

Such cooperative behaviors have sometimes been seen as arguments for left-wing politics, for example, by the Russian zoologist and anarchist Peter Kropotkin in his 1902 book Mutual Aid: A Factor of Evolution and Moral Philosopher Peter Singer in his book A Darwinian Left .

Neurobiology

Jorge Moll and Jordan Grafman, neuroscientists at the National Institutes of Health and LABS-D'Or Hospital Network, provided the first evidence for the neural bases of altruistic giving in normal healthy volunteers, using functional magnetic resonance imaging. In their research, [16] they showed that both pure monetary rewards and charitable donations activated the mesolimbic reward pathway, a primitive part of the brain that usually responds to food and sex. However, when volunteers generously placed the interests of others before their own by making charitable donations, another brain circuit was selectively activated: the subgenual cortex/septal region. These structures are related to[ vague ] social attachment and bonding in other species. The experiment suggested that altruism is not a higher moral faculty overpowering innate selfish desires, but a fundamental, ingrained, and enjoyable trait in the brain. [17] One brain region, the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex/basal forebrain, contributes to learning altruistic behavior, especially in people with trait[ clarification needed ] empathy. [18] The same study identified a link between[ vague ] giving to charity and the promotion[ clarification needed ] of social bonding. [19]

Bill Harbaugh, a University of Oregon economist, in an fMRI scanner test conducted with his psychologist colleague Dr. Ulrich Mayr, reached the same conclusions as Jorge Moll and Jordan Grafman about giving to charity, although they were able to divide the study group into two groups: "egoists" and "altruists". One of their discoveries was that, though rarely, even some of the considered "egoists" sometimes gave more than expected because that would help others, leading to the conclusion that there are other factors in charity, such as a person's environment and values. [19]

A recent meta-analysis of fMRI studies conducted by Shawn Rhoads, Jo Cutler, and Abigail Marsh analyzed the results of prior studies of generosity in which participants could freely choose to give or not give resources to someone else. [20] The results of this study confirmed that altruism is supported by distinct mechanisms from giving motivated by reciprocity or by fairness. This study also confirmed that the right ventral striatum is recruited during altruistic giving, as well as the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, bilateral anterior cingulate cortex, and bilateral anterior insula, which are regions previously implicated in empathy.

Abigail Marsh has conducted studies of real-world altruists that have also identified an important role for the amygdala in human altruism. In real-world altruists, such as people who have donated kidneys to strangers, the amygdala is larger than in typical adults. Altruists' amygdalas are also more responsive than those of typical adults to the sight of others' distress, which is thought to reflect an empathic response to distress. [21] [22] This structure may also be involved in altruistic choices due to its role in encoding the value of outcomes for others. [23] This is consistent with the findings of research in non-human animals, which has identified neurons within the amygdala that specifically encode the value of others' outcomes, activity in which appears to drive altruistic choices in monkeys. [24] [25]

Psychology

The International Encyclopedia of the Social Sciences defines psychological altruism as "a motivational state to increase another's welfare". Psychological altruism is contrasted with psychological egoism, which refers to the motivation to increase one's welfare. [26] In keeping with this, research in real-world altruists, including altruistic kidney donors, bone marrow donors, humanitarian aid workers, and heroic rescuers findings that these altruists are primarily distinguished from other adults by unselfish traits and decision-making patterns. This suggests that human altruism reflects genuinely high valuation of others' outcomes. [27]

There has been some debate on whether humans are capable of psychological altruism. [28] Some definitions specify a self-sacrificial nature to altruism and a lack of external rewards for altruistic behaviors. [29] However, because altruism ultimately benefits the self in many cases, the selflessness of altruistic acts is difficult to prove. The social exchange theory postulates that altruism only exists when the benefits outweigh the costs to the self. [30]

Daniel Batson, a psychologist, examined this question and argued against the social exchange theory. He identified four significant motives: to ultimately benefit the self (egoism), to ultimately benefit the other person (altruism), to benefit a group (collectivism), or to uphold a moral principle (principlism). Altruism that ultimately serves selfish gains is thus differentiated from selfless altruism, but the general conclusion has been that empathy-induced altruism can be genuinely selfless. [31] The empathy-altruism hypothesis states that psychological altruism exists and is evoked by the empathic desire to help someone suffering. Feelings of empathic concern are contrasted with personal distress, which compels people to reduce their unpleasant emotions and increase their positive ones by helping someone in need. Empathy is thus not selfless since altruism works either as a way to avoid those negative, unpleasant feelings and have positive, pleasant feelings when triggered by others' need for help or as a way to gain social reward or avoid social punishment by helping. People with empathic concern help others in distress even when exposure to the situation could be easily avoided, whereas those lacking in empathic concern avoid allowing it unless it is difficult or impossible to avoid exposure to another's suffering. [26]

Helping behavior is seen in humans from about two years old when a toddler can understand subtle emotional cues. [32]

Peace Corps trainees swearing in as volunteers in Cambodia, 4 April 2007 Peace Corps Volunteer swearing in Cambodia, 2007.jpg
Peace Corps trainees swearing in as volunteers in Cambodia, 4 April 2007

In psychological research on altruism, studies often observe altruism as demonstrated through prosocial behaviors such as helping, comforting, sharing, cooperation, philanthropy, and community service. [29] People are most likely to help if they recognize that a person is in need and feel personal responsibility for reducing the person's distress. The number of bystanders witnessing pain or suffering affects the likelihood of helping (the Bystander effect ). More significant numbers of bystanders decrease individual feelings of responsibility. [26] [33] However, a witness with a high level of empathic concern is likely to assume personal responsibility entirely regardless of the number of bystanders. [26]

Many studies have observed the effects of volunteerism (as a form of altruism) on happiness and health and have consistently found that those who exhibit volunteerism also have better current and future health and well-being. [34] [35] In a study of older adults, those who volunteered had higher life satisfaction and will to live, and less depression, anxiety, and somatization. [36] Volunteerism and helping behavior have not only been shown to improve mental health but physical health and longevity as well, attributable to the activity and social integration it encourages. [34] [37] [38] One study examined the physical health of mothers who volunteered over 30 years and found that 52% of those who did not belong to a volunteer organization experienced a major illness while only 36% of those who did volunteer experienced one. [39] A study on adults aged 55 and older found that during the four-year study period, people who volunteered for two or more organizations had a 63% lower likelihood of dying. After controlling for prior health status, it was determined that volunteerism accounted for a 44% reduction in mortality. [40] Merely being aware of kindness in oneself and others is also associated with greater well-being. A study that asked participants to count each act of kindness they performed for one week significantly enhanced their subjective happiness. Happier people are kinder and more grateful, kinder people are happier and more grateful and more grateful people are happier and kinder, the study suggests. [41]

While research supports the idea that altruistic acts bring about happiness, it has also been found to work in the opposite direction—that happier people are also kinder. The relationship between altruistic behavior and happiness is bidirectional. Studies found that generosity increases linearly from sad to happy affective states. [42]

Feeling over-taxed by the needs of others has negative effects on health and happiness. [38] For example, one study on volunteerism found that feeling overwhelmed by others' demands had an even stronger negative effect on mental health than helping had a positive one (although positive effects were still significant). [43]

Pathological altruism

Pathological altruism is altruism taken to an unhealthy extreme, such that it either harms the altruistic person or the person's well-intentioned actions cause more harm than good.

The term "pathological altruism" was popularised by the book Pathological Altruism .

Examples include depression and burnout seen in healthcare professionals, an unhealthy focus on others to the detriment of one's own needs, hoarding of animals, and ineffective philanthropic and social programs that ultimately worsen the situations they are meant to aid. [44]

Sociology

"Sociologists have long been concerned with how to build the good society". [45] The structure of our societies and how individuals come to exhibit charitable, philanthropic, and other pro-social, altruistic actions for the common good is a commonly researched topic within the field. The American Sociology Association (ASA) acknowledges public sociology saying, "The intrinsic scientific, policy, and public relevance of this field of investigation in helping to construct 'good societies' is unquestionable". [45] This type of sociology seeks contributions that aid popular and theoretical understandings of what motivates altruism and how it is organized, and promotes an altruistic focus in order to benefit the world and people it studies.

How altruism is framed, organized, carried out, and what motivates it at the group level is an area of focus that sociologists investigate in order to contribute back to the groups it studies and "build the good society". The motivation of altruism is also the focus of study; for example, one study links the occurrence of moral outrage to altruistic compensation of victims. [46] Studies show that generosity in laboratory and in online experiments is contagious – people imitate the generosity they observe in others. [47]

Religious viewpoints

Most, if not all, of the world's religions promote altruism as a very important moral value. Buddhism, Christianity, Hinduism, Islam, Jainism, Judaism, and Sikhism, etc., place particular emphasis on altruistic morality.

Buddhism

Monks collecting alms Monks collecting alms - Bun Vat Phu.JPG
Monks collecting alms

Altruism figures prominently in Buddhism. Love and compassion are components of all forms of Buddhism, and are focused on all beings equally: love is the wish that all beings be happy, and compassion is the wish that all beings be free from suffering. "Many illnesses can be cured by the one medicine of love and compassion. These qualities are the ultimate source of human happiness, and the need for them lies at the very core of our being" (Dalai Lama). [48] [49]

The notion of altruism is modified in such a world-view, since the belief is that such a practice promotes the practitioner's own happiness: "The more we care for the happiness of others, the greater our own sense of well-being becomes" (Dalai Lama). [48]

In the context of larger ethical discussions on moral action and judgment, Buddhism is characterized by the belief that negative (unhappy) consequences of our actions derive not from punishment or correction based on moral judgment, but from the law of karma, which functions like a natural law of cause and effect. A simple illustration of such cause and effect is the case of experiencing the effects of what one causes: if one causes suffering, then as a natural consequence one would experience suffering; if one causes happiness, then as a natural consequence one would experience happiness.[ citation needed ]

Jainism

Sculpture depicting the Jain concept of ahimsa (non-injury) Ahinsa.jpg
Sculpture depicting the Jain concept of ahimsa (non-injury)

The fundamental principles of Jainism revolve around altruism, not only for[ ambiguous ] humans but for all sentient beings. Jainism preaches ahimsa – to live and let live, not harming sentient beings, i.e. uncompromising reverence for all life. It also considers all living things to be equal[ specify ]. The first Tirthankara , Rishabhdev, introduced the concept of altruism for all living beings, from extending knowledge and experience to others to donation, giving oneself up for others, non-violence, and compassion for all living things.[ citation needed ]

The principle of nonviolence seeks to minimize karmas which limit the capabilities of the soul. Jainism views every soul as worthy of respect because it has the potential to become Siddha (God in Jainism). Because all living beings possess a soul, great care and awareness is essential in one's actions. Jainism emphasizes the equality of all life, advocating harmlessness towards all, whether the creatures are great or small. This policy extends even to microscopic organisms. Jainism acknowledges that every person has different capabilities and capacities to practice and therefore accepts different levels of compliance for ascetics and householders.[ citation needed ]

Christianity

Thomas Aquinas interprets "You should love your neighbour as yourself" [50] as meaning that love for ourselves is the exemplar of love for others. [51] Considering that "the love with which a man loves himself is the form and root of friendship" he quotes Aristotle that "the origin of friendly relations with others lies in our relations to ourselves",. [52] Aquinas concluded that though we are not bound to love others more than ourselves, we naturally seek the common good, the good of the whole, more than any private good, the good of a part. However, he thought we should love God more than ourselves and our neighbours, and more than our bodily life—since the ultimate purpose of loving our neighbour is to share in eternal beatitude: a more desirable thing than bodily well-being. In coining the word "altruism", as stated above, Comte was probably opposing this Thomistic doctrine, which is present in some theological schools within Catholicism. The aim and focus of Christian life is a life that glorifies God, with obeying christ's command to treat others equally, caring for them and understanding eternity in heaven is what Jesus Resurrection at calvary was all about.

Many biblical authors draw a strong connection between love of others and love of God. 1 John 4 states that for one to love God one must love his fellowman, and that hatred of one's fellowman is the same as hatred of God. Thomas Jay Oord has argued in several books that altruism is but one possible form of love. An altruistic action is not always a loving action. Oord defines altruism as acting for the other's good, and he agrees with feminists who note that sometimes love requires acting for one's own good when the other's demands undermine overall well-being.

German philosopher Max Scheler distinguishes two ways in which the strong can help the weak. One way is a sincere expression of Christian love, "motivated by a powerful feeling of security, strength, and inner salvation, of the invincible fullness of one's own life and existence". [53] :88–89 Another way is merely "one of the many modern substitutes for love,... nothing but the urge to turn away from oneself and to lose oneself in other people's business". [53] :95–96 At its worst, Scheler says, "love for the small, the poor, the weak, and the oppressed is really disguised hatred, repressed envy, an impulse to detract, etc., directed against the opposite phenomena: wealth, strength, power, largesse." [53] :96–97

Islam

In the Arabic language, "'iythar" (إيثار) means "preferring others to oneself". [54]

On the topic of donating blood to non-Muslims (a controversial topic within the faith), the Shia religious professor, Fadhil al-Milani has provided theological evidence that makes it positively justifiable. In fact, he considers it a form of religious sacrifice and ithar (altruism). [55]

For Sufis, 'iythar means devotion to others through complete forgetfulness of one's own concerns, where concern for others is deemed as a demand made by God on the human body, considered to be property of God alone. The importance of 'iythar (aka īthār) lies in sacrifice for the sake of the greater good; Islam considers those practicing īthār as abiding by the highest degree of nobility. [56] This is similar to the notion of chivalry. A constant concern for God results in a careful attitude towards people, animals, and other things in this world. [57]

Judaism

Judaism defines altruism as the desired goal of creation. Rabbi Abraham Isaac Kook stated that love is the most important attribute in humanity. [58] Love is defined as bestowal, or giving, which is the intention of altruism. This can be altruism towards humanity that leads to altruism towards the creator or God. Kabbalah defines God as the force of giving in existence. Rabbi Moshe Chaim Luzzatto focused on the "purpose of creation" and how the will of God was to bring creation into perfection and adhesion with this force of giving. [59]

Modern Kabbalah developed by Rabbi Yehuda Ashlag, in his writings about the future generation, focuses on how society could achieve an altruistic social framework. [60] :120–130 Ashlag proposed that such a framework is the purpose of creation, and everything that happens is to raise humanity to the level of altruism, love for one another. Ashlag focused on society and its relation to divinity. [60] :175–180

Sikhism

Altruism is essential to the Sikh religion. The central faith in Sikhism is that the greatest deed anyone can do is to imbibe and live the godly qualities like love, affection, sacrifice, patience, harmony, and truthfulness. Sevā , or selfless service to the community for its own sake, is an important concept in Sikhism. [61]

The fifth Guru, Arjun Dev, sacrificed his life to uphold "22 carats of pure truth, the greatest gift to humanity", the Guru Granth. The ninth Guru, Tegh Bahadur, sacrificed his head to protect weak and defenseless people against atrocity.

In the late seventeenth century, Guru Gobind Singh (the tenth Guru in Sikhism), was at war with the Mughal rulers to protect the people of different faiths when a fellow Sikh, Bhai Kanhaiya, attended the troops of the enemy. [62] He gave water to both friends and foes who were wounded on the battlefield. Some of the enemy began to fight again and some Sikh warriors were annoyed by Bhai Kanhaiya as he was helping their enemy. Sikh soldiers brought Bhai Kanhaiya before Guru Gobind Singh, and complained of his action that they considered counterproductive to their struggle on the battlefield. "What were you doing, and why?" asked the Guru. "I was giving water to the wounded because I saw your face in all of them", replied Bhai Kanhaiya. The Guru responded, "Then you should also give them ointment to heal their wounds. You were practicing what you were coached in the house of the Guru."

Under the tutelage of the Guru, Bhai Kanhaiya subsequently founded a volunteer corps for altruism, which is still engaged today in doing good to others and in training new recruits for this service. [63]

Hinduism

In Hinduism Selflessness (Atmatyag), Love (Prema), Kindness (Daya), and Forgiveness (Kshama) are considered as the highest acts of humanity or "Manushyattva". Giving alms to the beggars or poor people is considered as a divine act or "Punya" and Hindus believe it will free their souls from guilt or "Paapa" and will led them to heaven or "Swarga" in afterlife. Altruism is also the central act of various Hindu mythology and religious poems and songs. Mass donation of clothes to poor people (Vastraseva), or blood donation camp or mass food donation (Annaseva) for poor people is common in various Hindu religious ceremonies.[ citation needed ]

The Bhagavad Gita supports the doctrine of karma yoga (achieving oneness with God through action) & "Nishkam Karma" or action without expectation / desire for personal gain which can be said to encompass altruism. Altruistic acts are generally celebrated and very well received in Hindu literature and are central to Hindu morality. [64]

Philosophy

There is a wide range of philosophical views on humans' obligations or motivations to act altruistically. Proponents of ethical altruism maintain that individuals are morally obligated to act altruistically. [65] The opposing view is ethical egoism, which maintains that moral agents should always act in their own self-interest. Both ethical altruism and ethical egoism contrast with utilitarianism, which maintains that each agent should act in order to maximise the efficacy of their function and the benefit to both themselves and their co-inhabitants.

A related concept in descriptive ethics is psychological egoism, the thesis that humans always act in their own self-interest and that true altruism is impossible. Rational egoism is the view that rationality consists in acting in one's self-interest (without specifying how this affects one's moral obligations).

Effective altruism

Effective altruism is a philosophy and social movement that uses evidence and reasoning to determine the most effective ways to benefit others. [66] Effective altruism encourages individuals to consider all causes and actions and to act in the way that brings about the greatest positive impact, based upon their values. [67] It is the broad, evidence-based, and cause-neutral approach that distinguishes effective altruism from traditional altruism or charity. [68] Effective altruism is part of the larger movement towards evidence-based practices.

While a substantial proportion of effective altruists have focused on the nonprofit sector, the philosophy of effective altruism applies more broadly to prioritizing the scientific projects, companies, and policy initiatives which can be estimated to save lives, help people, or otherwise have the biggest benefit. [69] People associated with the movement include philosopher Peter Singer, [70] Facebook co founder Dustin Moskovitz, [71] Cari Tuna, [72] Oxford-based researchers William MacAskill [73] and Toby Ord, [74] and professional poker player Liv Boeree. [75]

Extreme Altruism

Extreme altruism also known as costly altruism, extraordinary altruism, or heroic behaviours (shall be distinguished from heroism), refers to selfless acts directed to a stranger which significantly exceed the normal altruistic behaviours, often involving risks or great cost to the altruists themselves. [21] Since acts of extreme altruism are often directed towards strangers, many commonly accepted models of simple altruism appear inadequate in explaining this phenomenon. [76]

One of the initial concepts was introduced by Wilson in 1976, which he referred to as “hard-core” altruism. [77] This form is characterised by impulsive actions directed towards others, typically a stranger and lacking incentives for reward. Since then, several papers have mentioned the possibility of such altruism. [78] [79]

The current slow progress in the field is due to general ethical guidelines that restrict exposing research participants to costly or risky decisions. [80] Consequently, much research has based their studies on living organ donations and the actions of Carnegie Hero medal Recipients, actions which involve high risk, high cost, and are of infrequent occurrences. [80] A typical example of extreme altruism would be non-directed kidney donation - a living person donating one of their kidneys to a stranger without any benefits or knowing the recipient.

However, current research can only be carried out on a small population that meets the requirements of extreme altruism. Most of the time the research is also via the form of self-report which could lead to self-report biases. [80] Due to the limitations, the current gap between high stakes and normal altruism remains unknown. [81]

Characteristics of Extreme Altruists

In 1970, Schwartz hypothesised that extreme altruism is positively related to a person’s moral norms and is not influenced by the cost associated with the action. [81] This hypothesis was supported in the same study examining bone marrow donors. Schwartz discovered that individuals with strong personal norms and those who attribute more responsibility to themselves are more inclined to participate in bone marrow donation. [81] Similar findings were observed in a 1986 study by Piliavin and Libby focusing on blood donors. [82] These studies suggest that personal norms lead to the activation of moral norms, leading individuals to feel compelled to help others. [81]

Abigail Marsh has described psychopaths as the “opposite” group of people to extreme altruists [82] and has conducted a few research, comparing these two groups of individuals. Utilising techniques such as brain imaging and behavioural experiments, Marsh’s team observed that kidney donors tend to have larger amygdala sizes and exhibit better abilities in recognizing fearful expressions compared to psychopathic individuals. [21] Furthermore, an improved ability to recognize fear has been associated with an increase in prosocial behaviours, including greater charity contribution. [83]

Rand and Epstein explored the behaviours of 51 Carnegie Hero Medal Recipients, demonstrating how extreme altruistic behaviours often stem from system I of the Dual Process Theory, which leads to rapid and intuitive behaviours. [84] Additionally, a separate by Carlson et al. indicated that such prosocial behaviours are prevalent in emergencies where immediate actions are required. [85]

This discovery has led to ethical debates, particularly in the context of living organ donation, where laws regarding this issue differ by country. [86] As observed in extreme altruists, these decisions are made intuitively, which may reflect insufficient consideration. Critics are concerned about whether this rapid decision encompasses a thorough cost-benefit analysis and question the appropriateness of exposing donors to such risk. [87]

One finding suggests how extreme altruists exhibit lower levels of social discounting as compared to others. With that meaning extreme altruists place a higher value on the welfare of strangers than a typical person does. [27] [88]

Analysis of 676 Carnegie Hero Award Recipients [89] and another study on 243 rescuing acts [90] reveal that a significant proportion of rescuers come from lower socio-economic backgrounds. Johnson attributes the distribution to the high-risk occupations that are more prevalent between lower socioeconomic groups. [89] Another hypothesis proposed by Lyons is that individuals from these groups may perceive they have less to lose when engaging in high-risk extreme altruistic behaviours. [90]

Possible Explanations

Evolutionary theories such as the kin-selection, reciprocity, vested interest and punishment either contradict or do not fully explain the concept of extreme altruism. [91] As a result, considerable research has attempted for a separate explanation for this behaviour.  

Research suggests that males are more likely to engage in heroic and risk-taking behaviours due to a preference among females for such traits. [92] These extreme altruistic behaviours could serve to act as an unconscious “signal” to showcase superior power and ability compared to ordinary individuals. [91] When an extreme altruist survives a high-risk situation, they send an “honest signal” of quality. [91] Three qualities hypothesized to be exhibited by extreme altruists, which could be interpreted as "signals," are: (1) traits that are difficult to fake, (2) a willingness to help, and (3) generous behaviours. [91]

The empathy altruism hypothesis appears to align with the concept of extreme altruism without contradiction. The hypothesis was supported with further brain scanning research, which indicates how this group of people demonstrate a higher level of empathy concern. The level of empathy concern then triggers activation in specific brain regions, urging the individual to engage in heroic behaviours. [93]

While most altruistic behaviours offer some form of benefit, extreme altruism may sometimes result from a mistake where the victim does not reciprocate. [91] Considering the impulsive characteristic of extreme altruists, some researchers suggest that these individuals have made a wrong judgement during the cost-benefit analysis. [84] Furthermore, extreme altruism might be a rare variation of altruism where they lie towards to ends of a normal distribution. [91] In the US, the annual prevalence rate per capita is less than 0.00005%, this shows the rarity of such behaviours. [27]

Genetics

OXTR, CD38, COMT, DRD4, DRD5, IGF2, and GABRB2 are candidate genes for influencing altruistic behavior. [94] [ further explanation needed ]

Digital altruism

Digital altruism is the notion that some are willing to freely share information based on the principle of reciprocity and in the belief that in the end, everyone benefits from sharing information via the Internet. [95]

There are three types of digital altruism: (1) "everyday digital altruism", involving expedience, ease, moral engagement, and conformity; (2) "creative digital altruism", involving creativity, heightened moral engagement, and cooperation; and (3) "co-creative digital altruism" involving creativity, moral engagement, and meta cooperative efforts. [95]

See also

Notes

    • "altruism (n .)". Online Etymology Dictionary. Douglas Harper. Retrieved 27 May 2021.
    • Teske, Nathan (2009). Political Activists in America: The Identity Construction Model of Political Participation. University Park, Pa.: Pennsylvania State University Press. p. 101. ISBN   978-0-271-03546-8.
  1. Ciciloni, Ferdinando (1825). A Grammar of the Italian Language. London: John Murray. p.  64.
  2. Bell, Graham (2008). Selection: the mechanism of evolution . Oxford: Oxford University Press. pp.  367–368. ISBN   978-0-19-856972-5.
  3. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 Barcaly, Pat (2011). "The evolution of charitable behaviour and the power of reputation". In Roberts, S. Craig (ed.). Applied Evolutionary Psychology. Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199586073.001.0001. ISBN   978-0-19-958607-3.
  4. Okasha, Samir (2013). "Kin Selection and Inclusive Fitness". Biological Altruism. Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy.
  5. Trivers, R.L. (1971). "The evolution of reciprocal altruism". Quarterly Review of Biology. 46: 35–57. doi:10.1086/406755. S2CID   19027999.
  6. Axelrod, R; Hamilton, W.D. (27 March 1981). "The evolution of cooperation". Science . 211 (4489): 1390–1396. Bibcode:1981Sci...211.1390A. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.147.9644 . doi:10.1126/science.7466396. PMID   7466396.
  7. Nowak, Martin; Sigmund, Karl (October 2005). "Evolution of indirect reciprocity" (PDF). Nature . 437 (27): 1291–1298. Bibcode:2005Natur.437.1291N. doi:10.1038/nature04131. PMID   16251955. S2CID   3153895.
  8. Gintis, Herbert (September 2000). "Strong Reciprocity and Human Sociality". Journal of Theoretical Biology . 206 (2): 169–179. Bibcode:2000JThBi.206..169G. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.335.7226 . doi:10.1006/jtbi.2000.2111. hdl: 10419/105717 . PMID   10966755. S2CID   9260305.
  9. Hammerstein, Peter, ed. (2003). "By-product Benefits, Reciprocity, and Pseudoreciprocity in Mutualism". Genetic and Cultural Evolution of Cooperation. MIT Press. pp. 203–222. doi:10.7551/mitpress/3232.003.0013. ISBN   978-0-262-08326-3.
  10. Zahavi, A. (1995). "Altruism as a handicap – The limitations of kin selection and reciprocity". Journal of Avian Biology. 26 (1): 1–3. doi:10.2307/3677205. JSTOR   3677205.
  11. 1 2 Iredal, Wendy; van Vugt, Mark (2011). "Altruism as showing off: a signaling perspective on promoting green behavior and acts of kindness". In Roberts, S. Craig (ed.). Applied Evolutionary Psychology. Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199586073.001.0001. ISBN   978-0-19-958607-3.
  12. Neyfakh, Leon (17 April 2011). "Where does good come from?". Boston Globe. Archived from the original on 19 July 2011.
  13. Hudson, Richard Ellis; Aukema, Juliann Eve; Rispe, Claude; Roze, Denis (July 2002). "Altruism, cheating, and anticheater adaptations in cellular slime molds" (PDF). The American Naturalist. 160 (1): 31–43. doi:10.1086/340613. ISSN   1537-5323. PMID   18707497 . Retrieved 1 February 2024.
  14. Brown, S.L.; Brown, R.M. (2006). "Selective investment theory: Recasting the functional significance of close relationships" (PDF). Psychological Inquiry. 17: 1–29. doi:10.1207/s15327965pli1701_01. S2CID   144718661. Archived from the original (PDF) on 14 July 2014.
  15. Moll, Jorge; Krueger, Frank; Zahn, Roland; Pardini, Matteo; de Oliveira-Souza, Ricardo; Grafman, Jordan (17 October 2006). "Human fronto–mesolimbic networks guide decisions about charitable donation". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 103 (42): 15623–15628. Bibcode:2006PNAS..10315623M. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0604475103 . ISSN   0027-8424. PMC   1622872 . PMID   17030808.
  16. Vedantam, Shankar (28 May 2007). "If It Feels Good to Be Good, It Might Be Only Natural". The Washington Post. Retrieved 23 April 2010.
  17. 1 2 Svoboda, Elizabeth (5 September 2013). "Scientists Are Finding That We Are Hard-Wired for Giving". University of Notre Dame. Retrieved 7 August 2017.
  18. Rhoads, S. A. (2021). "A feature-based network analysis and fMRI meta-analysis reveal three distinct types of prosocial decisions". Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 16 (12): 1214–1233. doi:10.1093/scan/nsab079. PMC   8717062 . PMID   34160604 . Retrieved 30 December 2023.
  19. 1 2 3 Marsh, Abigail A.; Stoycos, Sarah A.; Brethel-Haurwitz, Kristin M.; Robinson, Paul; VanMeter, John W.; Cardinale, Elise M. (21 October 2014). "Neural and cognitive characteristics of extraordinary altruists". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 111 (42): 15036–15041. Bibcode:2014PNAS..11115036M. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1408440111 . ISSN   0027-8424. PMC   4210306 . PMID   25225374.
  20. Brethel-Haurwitz, Kristin M.; O'Connell, Katherine; Cardinale, Elise M.; Stoianova, Maria; Stoycos, Sarah A.; Lozier, Leah M.; VanMeter, John W.; Marsh, Abigail A. (25 October 2017). "Amygdala–midbrain connectivity indicates a role for the mammalian parental care system in human altruism". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 284 (1865): 20171731. doi:10.1098/rspb.2017.1731. ISSN   0962-8452. PMC   5666102 . PMID   29070724.
  21. Rhoads, Shawn A; O'Connell, Katherine; Berluti, Kathryn; Ploe, Montana L; Elizabeth, Hannah S; Amormino, Paige; Li, Joanna L; Dutton, Mary Ann; VanMeter, Ashley Skye; Marsh, Abigail A (13 June 2023). "Neural responses underlying extraordinary altruists' generosity for socially distant others". PNAS Nexus. 2 (7): pgad199. doi:10.1093/pnasnexus/pgad199. ISSN   2752-6542. PMC   10321390 . PMID   37416875.
  22. Dal Monte, Olga; Chu, Cheng C. J.; Fagan, Nicholas A.; Chang, Steve W. C. (April 2020). "Specialized medial prefrontal–amygdala coordination in other-regarding decision preference". Nature Neuroscience. 23 (4): 565–574. doi:10.1038/s41593-020-0593-y. hdl: 2318/1730693 . ISSN   1546-1726. PMC   7131896 . PMID   32094970.
  23. Putnam, P. T. (2023). "Dissociation of vicarious and experienced rewards by coupling frequency within the same neural pathway". Neuron. 111 (16): 2513–2522. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2023.05.020. PMC  10527039. PMID   37348507.
  24. 1 2 3 4 Darity, William A. Jr., ed. (2008). "Altruism". International Encyclopedia of the Social Sciences. Vol. 1 (2nd ed.). Detroit: Macmillan Reference USA. pp. 87–88.
  25. 1 2 3 Rhoads, Shawn A.; Vekaria, Kruti M.; O’Connell, Katherine; Elizabeth, Hannah S.; Rand, David G.; Kozak Williams, Megan N.; Marsh, Abigail A. (31 March 2023). "Unselfish traits and social decision-making patterns characterize six populations of real-world extraordinary altruists". Nature Communications. 14 (1): 1807. doi: 10.1038/s41467-023-37283-5 . ISSN   2041-1723. PMC   10066349 . PMID   37002205.
  26. Batson, C. (2011). Altruism in humans. New York, N.Y. U.S.: Oxford University Press.
  27. 1 2 Batson, C. Daniel (2012). "A history of prosocial behavior research". In Kruglanski, Arie W.; Stroebe, Wolfgang (eds.). Handbook of the history of social psychology. New York, NY: Psychology Press. pp. 243–264. ISBN   978-1-84872-868-4.
  28. Maner, J. K.; Luce, C. L.; Neuberg, S. L.; Cialdini, R. B.; Brown, S.; Sagarin, B. J. (2002). "The effects of perspective taking on motivations for helping: Still no evidence for altruism". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 28 (11): 1601–1610. doi:10.1177/014616702237586. S2CID   146125891.
  29. Batson, C. Daniel; Ahmad, Nadia; Stocks, E. L. (2011). "Four forms of prosocial motivation: Egoism, altruism, collectivism, and principlism". In Dunning, David (ed.). Social motivation. New York, NY: Psychology Press. pp. 103–126. ISBN   978-1-136-84720-2.
  30. Svetlova, M.; Nichols, S. R.; Brownell, C. A. (2010). "Toddlers prosocial behavior: From instrumental to empathic to altruistic helping". Child Development. 81 (6): 1814–1827. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01512.x. PMC   3088085 . PMID   21077866.
  31. Hudson, James M.; Bruckman, Amy S. (2004). "The Bystander Effect: A Lens for Understanding Patterns of Participation". Journal of the Learning Sciences. 13 (2): 165–195. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.72.4881 . doi:10.1207/s15327809jls1302_2. S2CID   16442298.
  32. 1 2 Musick, M. A.; Wilson, J. (2003). "Volunteering and depression: The role of psychological and social resources in different age groups". Social Science & Medicine. 56 (2): 259–269. doi:10.1016/S0277-9536(02)00025-4. PMID   12473312.
  33. Koenig, L. B.; McGue, M.; Krueger, R. F.; Bouchard (2007). "Religiousness, antisocial behavior, and altruism: Genetic and environmental mediation". Journal of Personality. 75 (2): 265–290. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2007.00439.x. PMID   17359239.
  34. Hunter, K. I.; Hunter, M. W. (1980). "Psychosocial differences between elderly volunteers and non-volunteers". The International Journal of Aging & Human Development. 12 (3): 205–213. doi:10.2190/0H6V-QPPP-7JK4-LR38. PMID   7216525. S2CID   42991434.
    • Kayloe, J. C.; Krause, M. (1985). "RARE FIND: or The value of volunteerism". Psychosocial Rehabilitation Journal. 8 (4): 49–56. doi:10.1037/h0099659.
    • Brown, S. L.; Brown, R.; House, J. S.; Smith, D. M. (2008). "Coping with spousal loss: Potential buffering effects of self-reported helping behavior". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 34 (6): 849–861. doi:10.1177/0146167208314972. PMID   18344495. S2CID   42983453.
  35. 1 2 Post, S. G. (2005). "Altruism, Happiness, and Health: It's Good to Be Good". International Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 12 (2): 66–77. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.485.8406 . doi:10.1207/s15327558ijbm1202_4. PMID   15901215. S2CID   12544814.
  36. Moen, P.; Dempster-Mcclain, D.; Williams, R. M. (1992). "Successful aging: A life-course perspective on women's multiple roles and health". American Journal of Sociology. 97 (6): 1612–1638. doi:10.1086/229941. S2CID   4828775.
  37. Oman, D.; Thoresen, C. E.; McMahon, K. (1999). "Volunteerism and mortality among the community-dwelling elderly". Journal of Health Psychology. 4 (3): 301–316. doi: 10.1177/135910539900400301 . PMID   22021599.
  38. Otake, K.; Shimai, S.; Tanaka-Matsumi, J.; Otsui, K.; Fredrickson, B. L. (2006). "Happy people become happier through kindness: A counting kindnesses intervention". Journal of Happiness Studies. 7 (3): 361–375. doi:10.1007/s10902-005-3650-z. PMC   1820947 . PMID   17356687.
  39. Underwood, B.; Froming, W. J.; Moore, B. S. (1977). "Mood, attention, and altruism: A search for mediating variables". Developmental Psychology. 13 (5): 541–542. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.13.5.541.
  40. Schwartz, C.; Meisenhelder, J.; Ma, Y.; Reed, G. (2003). "Altruistic Social Interest Behaviors Are Associated With Better Mental Health". Psychosomatic Medicine. 65 (5): 778–785. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.529.7780 . doi:10.1097/01.PSY.0000079378.39062.D4. PMID   14508020. S2CID   20644442.
  41. Pathological Altruism. Oxford University Press. 19 December 2011. ISBN   978-0-19-973857-1.
  42. 1 2 "Altruism, Morality, and Social Solidarity". American Sociological Association. Archived from the original on 3 May 2012.
  43. Thulin, E.W.; Bicchieri, C. (2016). "I'm so angry I could help you: Moral outrage as a driver of victim compensation". Social Philosophy & Policy. 32 (2): 146–160. doi:10.1017/S0265052516000145. S2CID   148548711.
  44. 1 2 "The Medicine of Altruism". Archived from the original on 3 October 2009.
  45. The phrase "core of our being" is Freudian; see Bettina Bock von Wülfingen (2013). "Freud's 'Core of our Being' Between Cytology and Psychoanalysis". Berichte zur Wissenschaftsgeschichte. 36 (3): 226–244. doi:10.1002/bewi.201301604. PMID   32545937.
  46. Leviticus 19 and Matthew 22
  47. Thomas Aquinas, Summa Theologica , II:II Quaestio 25, Article 4
  48. Aristotle, Nicomachean Ethics IX.4 1166a1
  49. 1 2 3 Scheler, Max (1961). Ressentiment.
  50. 'iythar Google Translate
  51. Schmiedel, Ulrich; Smith, Graeme (15 February 2018). Religion in the European Refugee Crisis. Springer. pp. 295–96. ISBN   978-3-319-67961-7.
  52. Gülen, M. Fethullah (2004). Key Concepts in the Practice of Sufism: Emerald Hills of the Heart. Rutherford, N.J.: Fountain. pp. 10–11. ISBN   978-1-932099-75-1.
  53. Neusner, Jacob Eds (2005). Altruism in World Religions. Washington, D.C.: Georgetown Univ. Press. pp. 79–80. ISBN   978-1-58901-065-9.
  54. Kook, Abraham Isaac; Bokser, Ben Zion (1978). Abraham Isaac Kook: The lights of penitence, The moral principles, Lights of holiness, essays, letters, and poems . Paulist Press. pp.  135–136. ISBN   978-0-8091-2159-5.
  55. Luzzatto, Moshe Ḥayyim (1997). The way of God. Feldheim Publishers. pp. 37–38. ISBN   978-0-87306-769-0.
  56. 1 2 Ashlag, Yehuda (2006). Building the Future Society. Thornhill, Canada: Laitman Kabbalah Publishers. ISBN   978-965-7065-34-1.
  57. Cole, W. Owen; Sambhi, Piara Singh (1990). A Popular Dictionary of Sikhism. Curzon Press. pp.  38–39, 84. Retrieved 2 December 2018.
  58. Cunningham, Joseph Davey (1918). "A History of the Sikhs". Oxford University Press. p. ix. Retrieved 30 November 2018.
  59. Ralhan, O. P. (1997). The great gurus of the Sikhs. New Delhi: Anmol Publications Pvt Ltd. p. 253. ISBN   978-81-7488-479-4.
  60. Sivananda, Swami. Phaladhikaranam, Topic 8, Sutras 38–41.
  61. Kraut, Richard (2020), "Altruism", in Zalta, Edward N. (ed.), The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Fall 2020 ed.), Metaphysics Research Lab, Stanford University, retrieved 16 January 2024
  62. MacAskill, William (31 January 2017). "Effective Altruism: Introduction". Essays in Philosophy. 18 (1): 2. doi: 10.7710/1526-0569.1580 . ISSN   1526-0569.
  63. Matthews, Dylan (24 April 2015). "You have $8 billion. You want to do as much good as possible. What do you do?". Vox . Retrieved 27 April 2015.
  64. Bennett, Nicole; Carter, Ashley; Resney, Romney; Woods, Wendy. "How Tech Entrepreneurs Are Disrupting Philanthropy". BCG Perspectives. Boston Consulting Group. Retrieved 11 March 2017.
  65. MacAskill, William (2015). Doing Good Better. Avery. ISBN   978-1-59240-910-5.
  66. Walters, Helen (19 September 2013). "The why and how of effective altruism: Peter Singer's talk visualized". TED Blog.
  67. "Cari Tuna and Dustin Moskovitz: Young Silicon Valley billionaires pioneer new approach to philanthropy". The Washington Post. 26 December 2014.
  68. Callahan, Favid (12 September 2013). "Meet Cari Tuna, the Woman Giving Away Dustin Moskovitz's Facebook Fortune". Inside Philanthropy. Retrieved 1 March 2018.
  69. Thompson, Derek (15 June 2015). "The Greatest Good". The Atlantic.
  70. "Peter Singer: "The Most Good You Can Do" | Talks at Google". YouTube.
  71. de Waal, Frans B.M. (1 January 2008). "Putting the Altruism Back into Altruism: The Evolution of Empathy". Annual Review of Psychology. 59 (1): 279–300. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093625. ISSN   0066-4308. PMID   17550343.
  72. Wilson, John P. (December 1976). "Motivation, modeling, and altruism: A Person × Situation analysis". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 34 (6): 1078–1086. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.34.6.1078. ISSN   1939-1315.
  73. Piliavin, Jane Allyn; Charng, Hong-Wen (1990). "Altruism: A Review of Recent Theory and Research". Annual Review of Sociology. 16: 27–65. doi:10.1146/annurev.so.16.080190.000331. ISSN   0360-0572. JSTOR   2083262.
  74. Krebs, Dennis L. (1991). "Altruism and Egoism: A False Dichotomy?". Psychological Inquiry. 2 (2): 137–139. doi:10.1207/s15327965pli0202_9. ISSN   1047-840X. JSTOR   1449250.
  75. 1 2 3
  76. 1 2 3 4 Rusch, Hannes (2022). "Heroic behavior: A review of the literature on high-stakes altruism in the wild". Current Opinion in Psychology. 43: 238–243. doi:10.1016/j.copsyc.2021.07.024. PMID   34454246.
  77. 1 2 Marsh, Abigail A. (2016). "Understanding amygdala responsiveness to fearful expressions through the lens of psychopathy and altruism". Journal of Neuroscience Research. 94 (6): 513–525. doi:10.1002/jnr.23668. ISSN   0360-4012. PMID   26366635.
  78. Marsh, Abigail A.; Kozak, Megan N.; Ambady, Nalini (2007). "Accurate identification of fear facial expressions predicts prosocial behavior". Emotion. 7 (2): 239–251. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.239. ISSN   1931-1516. PMC   2743452 . PMID   17516803.
  79. 1 2 Rand, David G.; Epstein, Ziv G. (15 October 2014). Boraud, Thomas (ed.). "Risking Your Life without a Second Thought: Intuitive Decision-Making and Extreme Altruism". PLOS ONE. 9 (10): e109687. Bibcode:2014PLoSO...9j9687R. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0109687 . ISSN   1932-6203. PMC   4198114 . PMID   25333876.
  80. Carlson, Ryan W.; Aknin, Lara B.; Liotti, Mario (1 July 2016). "When is giving an impulse? An ERP investigation of intuitive prosocial behavior". Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 11 (7): 1121–1129. doi:10.1093/scan/nsv077. ISSN   1749-5024. PMC   4927032 . PMID   26084530.
  81. Lopp, Leonie (12 June 2013). Regulations Regarding Living Organ Donation in Europe: Possibilities of Harmonisation. Springer Science & Business Media. ISBN   978-3-642-33799-4.
  82. Friedman Ross, Lainie; Glannon, Walter; Josephson, Michelle A.; Thistlethwaite, J. Richard Jr (15 August 2002). "Should all living donors be treated equally?". Transplantation. 74 (3): 418–21, discussion 421-2. doi:10.1097/00007890-200208150-00025. ISSN   0041-1337. PMID   12177627.
  83. Vekaria, Kruti M.; Brethel-Haurwitz, Kristin M.; Cardinale, Elise M.; Stoycos, Sarah A.; Marsh, Abigail A. (28 April 2017). "Social discounting and distance perceptions in costly altruism". Nature Human Behaviour. 1 (5). doi:10.1038/s41562-017-0100. ISSN   2397-3374.
  84. 1 2 Johnson, Ronald C. (September 1996). "Attributes of carnegie medalists performing acts of heroism and of the recipients of these acts". Ethology and Sociobiology. 17 (5): 355–362. doi:10.1016/S0162-3095(96)00059-3.
  85. 1 2 Lyons, Minna T. (1 September 2005). "Who are the Heroes? Characteristics of People Who Rescue Others". Journal of Cultural and Evolutionary Psychology. 3 (3): 245–254. doi:10.1556/JCEP.3.2005.3-4.2. ISSN   1589-7397.
  86. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Barclay, Sara Kafashan, Adam Sparks, Amanda Rotella, Pat (2016). "Why Heroism Exists: Evolutionary Perspectives on Extreme Helping" (PDF). In Allison, Scott T.; Goethals, George R.; Kramer, Roderick M. (eds.). Handbook of Heroism and Heroic Leadership. Routledge. doi:10.4324/9781315690100. ISBN   978-1-315-69010-0.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  87. Kelly, Susan; Dunbar, R. I. M. (June 2001). "Who dares, wins: Heroism versus altruism in women's mate choice". Human Nature. 12 (2): 89–105. doi:10.1007/s12110-001-1018-6. ISSN   1045-6767. PMID   26192164.
  88. FeldmanHall, Oriel; Dalgleish, Tim; Evans, Davy; Mobbs, Dean (January 2015). "Empathic concern drives costly altruism". NeuroImage. 105: 347–356. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.10.043. PMC   4275572 . PMID   25462694.
  89. Thompson, GJ; Hurd, PL; Crespi, BJ (2013). "Genes underlying altruism". Biol Lett. 9 (6): 20130395. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2013.0395. PMC   3871336 . PMID   24132092.
  90. 1 2 Klisanin, Dana (2011). "Is the Internet Giving Rise to New Forms of Altruism" (PDF). Media Psychology Review. 3 (1): 1–11. Archived from the original (PDF) on 9 October 2022.

Related Research Articles

In ethical philosophy, ethical egoism is the normative position that moral agents ought to act in their own self-interest. It differs from psychological egoism, which claims that people can only act in their self-interest. Ethical egoism also differs from rational egoism, which holds that it is rational to act in one's self-interest. Ethical egoism holds, therefore, that actions whose consequences will benefit the doer are ethical.

Psychological egoism is the view that humans are always motivated by self-interest and selfishness, even in what seem to be acts of altruism. It claims that, when people choose to help others, they do so ultimately because of the personal benefits that they themselves expect to obtain, directly or indirectly, from doing so.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Reciprocal altruism</span> Form of behaviour between organisms

In evolutionary biology, reciprocal altruism is a behaviour whereby an organism acts in a manner that temporarily reduces its fitness while increasing another organism's fitness, with the expectation that the other organism will act in a similar manner at a later time.

Sympathy is the perception of, understanding of, and reaction to the distress or need of another life form.

Reciprocity is a crucial aspect of how people interact and live in society but researchers who study these interactions have often overlooked its importance. Reciprocity, as a fundamental principle in social psychology, revolves around the concept that individuals tend to respond to the actions of others in a manner that mirrors the positive or negative nature of those actions. It involves a mutual exchange of behaviors and reactions, where individuals reciprocate the same type of behavior they have received from others. People's choices in how they behave are mostly based on what they can gain from others in return, while feelings of trust, liking, and togetherness are strongly influenced by the idea of giving and receiving equally

In biology, altruism refers to behaviour by an individual that increases the fitness of another individual while decreasing their own. Altruism in this sense is different from the philosophical concept of altruism, in which an action would only be called "altruistic" if it was done with the conscious intention of helping another. In the behavioural sense, there is no such requirement. As such, it is not evaluated in moral terms—it is the consequences of an action for reproductive fitness that determine whether the action is considered altruistic, not the intentions, if any, with which the action is performed.

C. Daniel Batson is an American social psychologist. He has two doctoral degrees, in theology and psychology. Batson obtained his doctorate under John Darley and taught at the University of Kansas. He retired in 2006 and now is an emeritus professor in the psychology department of the University of Tennessee. He is best known for his contributions to the social psychology of altruism, empathic concern, and psychology of religion.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Jean Decety</span> American neuroscientist

Jean Decety is an American–French neuroscientist specializing in developmental neuroscience, affective neuroscience, and social neuroscience. His research focuses on the psychological and neurobiological mechanisms underpinning social cognition, particularly social decision-making, empathy, moral reasoning, altruism, pro-social behavior, and more generally interpersonal relationships. He is Irving B. Harris Distinguished Service Professor at the University of Chicago.

Empathy-altruism is a form of altruism based on moral emotions or feelings for others.

The concept of the evolution of morality refers to the emergence of human moral behavior over the course of human evolution. Morality can be defined as a system of ideas about right and wrong conduct. In everyday life, morality is typically associated with human behavior rather than animal behavior. The emerging fields of evolutionary biology, and in particular evolutionary psychology, have argued that, despite the complexity of human social behaviors, the precursors of human morality can be traced to the behaviors of many other social animals. Sociobiological explanations of human behavior remain controversial. Social scientists have traditionally viewed morality as a construct, and thus as culturally relative, although others such as Sam Harris argue that there is an objective science of morality.

Prosocial behavior, or intent to benefit others, is a social behavior that "benefit[s] other people or society as a whole", "such as helping, sharing, donating, co-operating, and volunteering". Obeying the rules and conforming to socially accepted behaviors are also regarded as prosocial behaviors. These actions may be motivated by empathy and by concern about the welfare and rights of others, as well as for egoistic or practical concerns, such as one's social status or reputation, hope for direct or indirect reciprocity, or adherence to one's perceived system of fairness. It may also be motivated by altruism, though the existence of pure altruism is somewhat disputed, and some have argued that this falls into philosophical rather than psychological realm of debate. Evidence suggests that pro sociality is central to the well-being of social groups across a range of scales, including schools. Prosocial behavior in the classroom can have a significant impact on a student's motivation for learning and contributions to the classroom and larger community. In the workplace, prosocial behaviour can have a significant impact on team psychological safety, as well as positive indirect effects on employee's helping behaviors and task performance. Empathy is a strong motive in eliciting prosocial behavior, and has deep evolutionary roots.

The negative-state relief model states that human beings have an innate drive to reduce negative moods. They can be reduced by engaging in any mood-elevating behaviour, including helping behaviour, as it is paired with positive value such as smiles and thank you. Thus negative mood increases helpfulness because helping others can reduce one's own bad feelings.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Helping behavior</span> Voluntarily prosocial behaviour

Helping behavior refers to voluntary actions intended to help others, with reward regarded or disregarded. It is a type of prosocial behavior.

Homo reciprocans, or reciprocating human, is the concept in some economic theories of humans as cooperative actors who are motivated by improving their environment through positive reciprocity or negative reciprocity, even in situations without foreseeable benefit for themselves.

Warm-glow giving is an economic theory describing the emotional reward of giving to others. According to the original warm-glow model developed by James Andreoni, people experience a sense of joy and satisfaction for "doing their part" to help others. This satisfaction - or "warm glow" - represents the selfish pleasure derived from "doing good", regardless of the actual impact of one's generosity. Within the warm-glow framework, people may be "impurely altruistic", meaning they simultaneously maintain both altruistic and egoistic (selfish) motivations for giving. This may be partially due to the fact that "warm glow" sometimes gives people credit for the contributions they make, such as a plaque with their name or a system where they can make donations publicly so other people know the "good" they are doing for the community.

Social preferences describe the human tendency to not only care about one's own material payoff, but also the reference group's payoff or/and the intention that leads to the payoff. Social preferences are studied extensively in behavioral and experimental economics and social psychology. Types of social preferences include altruism, fairness, reciprocity, and inequity aversion. The field of economics originally assumed that humans were rational economic actors, and as it became apparent that this was not the case, the field began to change. The research of social preferences in economics started with lab experiments in 1980, where experimental economists found subjects' behavior deviated systematically from self-interest behavior in economic games such as ultimatum game and dictator game. These experimental findings then inspired various new economic models to characterize agent's altruism, fairness and reciprocity concern between 1990 and 2010. More recently, there are growing amounts of field experiments that study the shaping of social preference and its applications throughout society.

Elevation is an emotion elicited by witnessing actual or imagined virtuous acts of remarkable moral goodness. It is experienced as a distinct feeling of warmth and expansion that is accompanied by appreciation and affection for the individual whose exceptional conduct is being observed. Elevation motivates those who experience it to open up to, affiliate with, and assist others. Elevation makes an individual feel lifted up and optimistic about humanity.

Moral emotions are a variety of social emotions that are involved in forming and communicating moral judgments and decisions, and in motivating behavioral responses to one's own and others' moral behavior. As defined by Jonathan Haidt, moral emotions "are linked to the interests or welfare either of a society as a whole or at least of persons other than the judge or agent". A person may not always have clear words to articulate, yet simultaneously, that same person knows it to be true deep down inside.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Psychological barriers to effective altruism</span> Area of study

In the philosophy of effective altruism, an altruistic act such as charitable giving is considered more effective, or cost-effective, if it uses a set of resources to do more good per unit of resource than other options, with the goal of trying to do the most good. Following this definition of effectiveness, researchers in psychology and related fields have identified psychological barriers to effective altruism that can cause people to choose less effective options when they engage in altruistic activities such as charitable giving. These barriers can include evolutionary influences as well as motivational and epistemic obstacles.

Parochial altruism is a concept in social psychology, evolutionary biology, and anthropology that describes altruism towards an in-group, often accompanied by hostility towards an out-group. It is a combination of altruism, defined as behavior done for the benefit of others without direct effect on the self, and parochialism, which refers to having a limited viewpoint. Together, these concepts create parochial altruism, or altruism which is limited in scope to one's in-group. Parochial altruism is closely related to the concepts of in-group favoritism and out-group discrimination. Research has suggested that parochial altruism may have evolved in humans to promote high levels of in-group cooperation, which is advantageous for group survival. Parochial altruism is often evoked to explain social behaviors within and between groups, such as why people are cooperative within their social groups and why they may be aggressive towards other social groups.

References