Flight zone

Last updated
Flight initiation distance (FID) buffer from critical wildlife area. Flight Initiation Distance Buffers.jpg
Flight initiation distance (FID) buffer from critical wildlife area.

The flight zone of an animal is the area surrounding an animal that if encroached upon by a potential predator or threat, including humans, will cause alarm and escape behavior. The flight zone is determined by the animal's flight distance, sometimes called [3] flight initiation distance (FID) [4] which extends horizontally from the animal and sometimes vertically. It may also be termed[ citation needed ]escape distance, alert distance, flush distance, and escape flight distance.

Contents

Swiss zoologist Heini Hediger distinguished between flight distance (run boundary), critical distance (attack boundary), personal distance (distance separating members of non-contact species, as a pair of swans), and social distance (intraspecies communication distance).

Flight distance can be used as a measure of the willingness of an animal to take risks. Escape theory predicts that the probability of fleeing and flight distance increase as predation risk increases and decrease as escape cost increases. [5] Flight initiation distance is one measure of animals' fear responses to humans. [6]

In a study comparing 56 bird species with long flight distances, it was found these had declining populations in Europe. This indicates that standardized measures of flight distance can provide reliable information about the population consequences of risk-taking behaviour by individuals and the susceptibility of different species to increased levels of disturbance by humans. [5] A further study analyzing 75 flight initiation distance studies of 212 species found that larger species are more tolerant of humans. [6]

When the flight zone of a group of bulls was invaded by a mechanical trolley, the bulls moved away and maintained a constant distance between themselves and the trolley. [7] This indicates animals sometimes maintain a flight zone around inanimate objects.

The flight initiation distance is being used as a tool in wildlife management. [8] By studying flight zones, wildlife managers are able to reduce the impact of humans by creating buffer zones between human populations and animal habitats. [8]

The alert distance (AD) is the distance, by definition greater, within which the animal changes its behaviour in a manner enabling it to better observe the stimulus, as by raising the head in an alert posture, but does not necessarily flee unless the stimulus is also within the escape distance. [9] [10] [11] These measures are usually used to quantify the tolerance of wildlife to humans.

Factors influencing size

Animals faced with approaching predators must decide at which distance to initiate their flight, and they are expected to do so in a way that maximizes their fitness. As flight has both costs (including lost opportunity) and benefits, there will be in general an optimal flight initiation distance, defined as the first point in a predator's approach at which the benefit of flight exceeds the cost. The benefit of flight is equivalent to the cost of remaining in place or, in other words, to the risk of capture. The size of the flight zone can therefore vary according to circumstances. However, it has been shown in burrowing owls that individuals showed high repeatability in their FID. [12]

Wildlife management

Wildlife managers often use ED and FID to develop set-back distances to reduce human impacts on wildlife, [17] [18] [1] [4] both in wildlife refuges, and, e.g., in planning areas for outdoor recreation. [19]

These measures are also important in birding and nature photography.

The FID in multiple species differs from rural to urban areas. [20] A study by Møller et al. examined 811 FIDs from 37 species of birds and determined that the FID of birds in urban areas is reduced, compared to the FID of birds in rural areas. [20] Urbanization of birds has also been shown to correlate with changes in stress physiology and anti predator behaviour. [20] This may be due to a number of factors differing in rural vs urban areas, such as; difference in predator communities, length of exposure time to humans, relative abundance of humans, and the presence/abundance of food (bird-feeders in winter for example). [20] Wildlife managers must adjust buffer zones depending on urban/rural environments.

Some physical characteristics are very important to determine an animal's FID. [21] Eye size and brain size have a role in determining the FID. [21] FID in 107 species of birds was studied in relation to eye size and brain size and was shown that FID increases with larger eyes and decreases with larger brains. [21] Larger eyes mean that predators can be detected from further away and thus the FID would be larger compared to smaller eyes. [21] Larger brains decrease the FID compared to smaller brains, since they can better process the intent of predators and can delay their flight response for as long as possible. [21]

FID can be highly variable, but it can also be viewed as a species-specific trait. [8] A study conducted using eight species of shorebirds at six different sites in Australia was conducted to determine if FID was species specific. [8] It was demonstrated that while both the species and the site influenced the FID, there was no significant interaction between them. [8] This indicates that FID is species-specific, and while sites do influence the FID of a species, the average FID is a good reference for wildlife managers to use when creating buffer zones. [8]

While escape distance has been generally used as a measure of tolerance, other changes in animal behavior in presence of humans, such as increased vigilance time at the cost of decreased feeding time, may have significant overall impact on wildlife. Therefore, it is suggested that a more conservative measure, namely, the alert distance, should be used in determining minimum approaching distance. [19] The latter typically adds a certain buffer distance to the given tolerance measure. [4]

Animal handling

The flight zone is an important principle for herding, working, and mustering livestock. An animal can be stimulated to move simply by skirting its flight zone, and the animal will move in the desired direction according to the point of balance. The point of balance is usually located at the animal's shoulder according to their wide angled vision. An overstimulated animal will have a larger flight zone, for example an excited or scared animal. [22] A Cumulative Flight Zone is formed when animals move in a herd. In this situation the lead animal's and the following animals' Points of Balance, within the cumulative flight zone, must both be crossed to entice movement. [22]

The flight distance during handling is usually 1.5 to 7.6 m for beef cattle raised in a feeding operation and up to 30 m on mountain ranges. [23] Brahman cattle have a larger flight zone than most English breeds. [24] The flight zone can be thought of as the animal's personal space. The size of the flight zone is determined by the tameness of the animal; the more domesticated an animal, the smaller the zone. Fully tame animals have no flight zone. [25]

The flight zones in cattle vary depending on the situation they are experiencing. [26] Novel situations increase their flight zone, while accustomed stimuli will decrease their flight zone. [26] The flight zone is larger in the front than behind, due to the majority of their senses pointing forward. [26] As the animal becomes more relaxed in a situation or with a person its flight zone will reduce. [26] The cow's prior experiences with humans has also been shown to affect their flight zone. [26] Cows with positive handling experiences were shown to have smaller flight zones than those with negative handling experiences. [26]

Studies with sheep indicated that animals confined in a narrow alley had a smaller flight zone compared to animals confined in a wider alley. [27]

Handlers sometimes make the mistake of deeply invading the flight zone when animals are being driven down an alley or into an enclosed area such as a crowd pen. If the handler deeply penetrates the flight zone, the animals may turn back and run over them in an attempt to escape. [28] Confining a livestock animal in a crush (chute) or alley can make it feel more secure and thus reduce the size of the flight zone; however, it does not eliminate the flight zone. An animal in a livestock raceway or alley that feels threatened may panic and injure itself or other animals. [29] If handlers lean over fences around animals they penetrate the "zone of safety" and may cause the animals to rear. [30]

Husbandry

Animals have a tendency to move in the opposite direction when their handler walks deep into their flight zone. By crossing an animal's point of balance, within the flight zone, a handler can move the herd in a particular direction and control their speed of movement. For example, crossing the point of balance from front to back will move the animal forwards, while the opposite is also true. The handler's pace should always reflect the animal's speed when herding. Additionally, pressure should be alternated on the flight zone to reduce stress. [25] Constant pressure should never be applied.

If animals turn to face the handler he or she is considered to be no longer penetrating the flight zone. [25]

It is important that a handler does not pursue any struggling animals as this will cause undue stress. Instead the animal should be allowed to return to the group as animals naturally have herd instincts, and will follow the group's leader. A good herding practice applying this is the movement of animals through a race by maintaining a steady flow of animals, not herding in groups, this allows new animals to follow the leader calmly. [31] When yarded, animals should always have room to turn away from the handler to reduce stress. Minimal stress prevents injury to the animal and maintains good production, such as increased quality of meat and improved muscle and fat scores. [29]

Appropriate frequent penetration of the flight zone can train the animals to minimise their flight zone to the handler. [25]

Poor husbandry skills including over penetration of the flight zone results in the following behaviours: stress, panic, aggression, bolting, prey behaviour, charging, fainting, sickness and self-inflicted damage. Rough handling, such as constant flight zone pressure, can raise the heart rate of an animal. These factors are all reflective of the General Adaptation Syndrome.

General adaptation syndrome

The general adaptation syndrome (GAS) is a three-phase response to stress in animals.

Sample values

Sample escape distances (mostly mean) from humans:

Birds of Europe

Because many birds flee from humans, ornithologists and birdwatchers sometimes use digiscoping equipment, allowing them to take pictures from long distances. Camera and telescopic lens on a tripod -onithology-7July2007.jpg
Because many birds flee from humans, ornithologists and birdwatchers sometimes use digiscoping equipment, allowing them to take pictures from long distances.
SpeciesED [ m ]Ref.
Brent goose (130–1000) 319 [10]
Northern pintail (100–500) 294 [10]
Grey heron 255 [11]
Mallard (60–400) 236 [10]
Northern lapwing 162 [11]
Dunlin (15–450) 70 [10]
Eurasian coot 68 [11]
Whinchat 20–30 [34]
Eurasian blackbird 10 [34]
Blue tit 10 [34]

Factors affecting escape distances for birds

Fleeing blackbird Fleeing Blackbird.jpg
Fleeing blackbird

Escape distance may differ significantly depending on many circumstances. Body size is the best known general factor influencing interspecies differences. Very frequently large species are more timid than small species, because size affects how rapidly a bird can take off. [11] [10] Surprisingly, an analysis of hundreds of studies found that larger birds are more tolerant of humans, despite substantial research that shows larger animals are less tolerant of humans. [6]

Among the affecting factors:

Flight Zone in Lizards

It is shown that the Lizard Urosaurus ornatus where different colored males have differing FID values due to differences in hormonal levels.

See also

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Wildebeest</span> Genus of antelope

Wildebeest, also called gnu, are antelopes of the genus Connochaetes and native to Eastern and Southern Africa. They belong to the family Bovidae, which includes true antelopes, cattle, goats, sheep, and other even-toed horned ungulates. There are two species of wildebeest: the black wildebeest or white-tailed gnu, and the blue wildebeest or brindled gnu.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Black-billed magpie</span> Species of bird in the Americas

The black-billed magpie, also known as the American magpie, is a bird in the corvid family found in the western half of North America. It is black and white, with black areas on the wings and tail showing iridescent hints of blue or blue-green. It is one of only four North American songbirds whose tail makes up half or more of the total body length.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Animal husbandry</span> Management, selective breeding, and care of farm animals by humans

Animal husbandry is the branch of agriculture concerned with animals that are raised for meat, fibre, milk, or other products. It includes day-to-day care, selective breeding, and the raising of livestock. Husbandry has a long history, starting with the Neolithic Revolution when animals were first domesticated, from around 13,000 BC onwards, predating farming of the first crops. By the time of early civilisations such as ancient Egypt, cattle, sheep, goats, and pigs were being raised on farms.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Feral</span> Wild-living but normally domestic animal or plant

A feral animal or plant is one that lives in the wild but is descended from domesticated individuals. As with an introduced species, the introduction of feral animals or plants to non-native regions may disrupt ecosystems and has, in some cases, contributed to extinction of indigenous species. The removal of feral species is a major focus of island restoration.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Herding dog</span> Type of dog used for herding

A herding dog, also known as a stock dog, shepherd dog, sheep dog or working dog, is a type of dog that either has been trained in herding or belongs to breeds that are developed for herding.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">African buffalo</span> Bovine species

The African buffalo is a large sub-Saharan African bovine. There are five subspecies that are recognized as being valid. Syncerus caffer caffer, the Cape buffalo, is the nominotypical subspecies, and the largest one, found in Southern and East Africa. S. c. nanus is the smallest subspecies, common in forest areas of Central and West Africa, while S. c. brachyceros is in West Africa and S. c. aequinoctialis is in the savannas of East Africa. The adult African buffalo's horns are its characteristic feature: they have fused bases, forming a continuous bone shield across the top of the head referred to as a "boss".

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Herd</span> Similar as Group

A herd is a social group of certain animals of the same species, either wild or domestic. The form of collective animal behavior associated with this is called herding. These animals are known as gregarious animals.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Anti-predator adaptation</span> Defensive feature of prey for selective advantage

Anti-predator adaptations are mechanisms developed through evolution that assist prey organisms in their constant struggle against predators. Throughout the animal kingdom, adaptations have evolved for every stage of this struggle, namely by avoiding detection, warding off attack, fighting back, or escaping when caught.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Escape response</span>

Escape response, escape reaction, or escape behavior is a mechanism by which animals avoid potential predation. It consists of a rapid sequence of movements, or lack of movement, that position the animal in such a way that allows it to hide, freeze, or flee from the supposed predator. Often, an animal's escape response is representative of an instinctual defensive mechanism, though there is evidence that these escape responses may be learned or influenced by experience.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Horse behavior</span>

Horse behavior is best understood from the view that horses are prey animals with a well-developed fight-or-flight response. Their first reaction to a threat is often to flee, although sometimes they stand their ground and defend themselves or their offspring in cases where flight is untenable, such as when a foal would be threatened.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Island tameness</span> Natural tendency of behaviour as a result of a lack of predators and prolonged geographic isolation

Island tameness is the tendency of many populations and species of animals living on isolated islands to lose their wariness of potential predators, particularly of large animals. The term is partly synonymous with ecological naïveté, which also has a wider meaning referring to the loss of defensive behaviors and adaptations needed to deal with these "new" predators. Species retain such wariness of predators that exist in their environment; for example, a Hawaiian goose retains its wariness of hawks, but does not exhibit such behaviors with mammals or other predators not found on the Hawaiian Islands. The most famous example is that of the dodo, which owed its extinction in a large part to a lack of fear of humans, and many species of penguin.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Flock (birds)</span>

A flock is a gathering of individual birds to forage or travel collectively. Avian flocks are typically associated with migration. Flocking also offers foraging benefits and protection from predators, although flocking can have costs for individual members.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Mobbing (animal behavior)</span> Antipredator adaptation in which individuals of prey species mob a predator

Mobbing in animals is an antipredator adaptation in which individuals of prey species mob a predator by cooperatively attacking or harassing it, usually to protect their offspring. A simple definition of mobbing is an assemblage of individuals around a potentially dangerous predator. This is most frequently seen in birds, though it is also known to occur in many other animals such as the meerkat and some bovines. While mobbing has evolved independently in many species, it only tends to be present in those whose young are frequently preyed upon. This behavior may complement cryptic adaptations in the offspring themselves, such as camouflage and hiding. Mobbing calls may be used to summon nearby individuals to cooperate in the attack.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Wildlife of Zambia</span>

The wildlife of Zambia refers to the natural flora and fauna of Zambia. This article provides an overview, and outline of the main wildlife areas or regions, and compact lists of animals focusing on prevalence and distribution in the country rather than on taxonomy. More specialized articles on particular groups are linked from here.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Cattle chute</span>

A cattle chute or cattle race also called a run or alley, is a narrow corridor built for cattle that separates them from the rest of the herd and allows handlers and veterinarians to provide medical care or restrain the animal for other procedures. A conventional cattle chute consists of parallel panels or fences with a space between them that is slightly wider than one animal so they are unable to turn around. Cattle chutes gently restrain the animal using a squeeze mechanism. The chute is connected to an alley, forming the animals into a queue that only allows them to go forward. Cattle tubs or a BudBox can also be used to help with animal flow and maintain low-stress cattle handling principles. It is used for routine husbandry activities such as drafting (sorting) or loading animals via ramp or loading chute into a vehicle; placing them one at a time in a cattle crush (variations also called a squeeze chute or standing stock) for examination, marking or veterinary treatment. They are also used at packing plants to move animals into a crush designed for slaughter.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Livestock guardian dog</span> Dog type bred for the purpose of protecting livestock from predators

A livestock guardian dog (LGD) is a dog type bred for the purpose of protecting livestock from predators.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Human–wildlife conflict</span> Negative interactions between people and wild animals

Human–wildlife conflict (HWC) refers to the negative interactions between humans and wild animals, with undesirable consequences both for people and their resources on the one hand, and wildlife and their habitats on the other. HWC, caused by competition for natural resources between human and wildlife, influences human food security and the well-being of both humans and animals. In many regions, the number of these conflicts has increased in recent decades as a result of human population growth and the transformation of land use.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Collective animal behavior</span> Animal cognition

Collective animal behaviour is a form of social behavior involving the coordinated behavior of large groups of similar animals as well as emergent properties of these groups. This can include the costs and benefits of group membership, the transfer of information, decision-making process, locomotion and synchronization of the group. Studying the principles of collective animal behavior has relevance to human engineering problems through the philosophy of biomimetics. For instance, determining the rules by which an individual animal navigates relative to its neighbors in a group can lead to advances in the deployment and control of groups of swimming or flying micro-robots such as UAVs.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Cattle</span> Large, domesticated, cloven-hooved herbivores

Cattle or oxen are large, domesticated, bovid ungulates. They are prominent modern members of the subfamily Bovinae and the most widespread species of the genus Bos. Mature female cattle are referred to as cows and mature male cattle are referred to as bulls. Colloquially, young female cattle (heifers), young male cattle (bullocks), and castrated male cattle (steers) are also referred to as "cows".

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Allelomimetic behavior</span>

Allelomimetic behavior or allomimetic behavior is a range of activities in which the performance of a behavior increases the probability of that behavior being performed by other nearby animals. Allelomimetic behavior is sometimes called contagious behavior and has strong components of social facilitation, imitation, and group coordination. It is usually considered to occur between members of the same species. An alternate definition is that allelomimetic behavior is a more intense or more frequent response or the initiation of an already known response, when others around the individual are engaged in the same behavior. It is often referred to as synchronous behavior, mimetic behavior, imitative behavior, and social facilitation.

References

  1. 1 2 Bentrup G. (2008). "Conservation buffers: design guidelines for buffers, corridors, and greenways". Gen. Tech. Rep. Asheville, NC: USDA, Forest Service, Southern Research Station. SRS-109.
  2. Bentrup, G. (2008). "Flight Initiation Distance Buffers". USDA National Agroforestry Center. Archived from the original on 12 December 2012. Retrieved 6 September 2012.
  3. Grandin, Temple; Deesing, Mark (2014). Genetics and Behavior During Handling, Restraint, and Herding. Elsevier Inc. p. 121.
  4. 1 2 3 4 5 Bentrup, G. (2008). "Flight Initiation Distance Buffers". USDA National Agroforestry Center. Archived from the original on 12 December 2012. Retrieved 6 September 2012.
  5. 1 2 Moller, A.P. (2008). "Flight distance and population trends in European breeding birds". Behavioral Ecology. 19 (6): 1095–1102. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arn103 .
  6. 1 2 3 Stuart Wolpert (16 November 2015). "Why are some wild animals more tolerant to human interaction than others?". UCLA . Retrieved 30 December 2016.
  7. Kilgour, R., (1971). Animal handling in works, pertinent behaviour studies. 13th Meat Industry Research Conference, Hamilton, New Zealand. pp. 9–12
  8. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Blumstein, D. T.; Anthony, L. L.; Harcourt, R.; Ross, G. (2003). "Testing a key assumption of wildlife buffer zones: is flight initiation distance a species-specific trait?". Biological Conservation. 110 (1): 97–100. doi:10.1016/s0006-3207(02)00180-5.
  9. 1 2 Ruddock M., Whitfield D. P. (2007). "A Review of Disturbance Distances in Selected Bird Species, A report from Natural Research (Projects) Ltd to Scottish Natural Heritage" (PDF). Archived from the original (PDF) on February 28, 2013. Retrieved September 4, 2012.
  10. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 Laursen K.; Kahlert J.; Frikke; J. (2005). "Factors affecting escape distances of staging waterbirds" (PDF). Wildlife Biology. 11 (1): 13–19. doi:10.2981/0909-6396(2005)11[13:faedos]2.0.co;2. S2CID   86208796 . Retrieved 4 September 2012.
  11. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 Bregnballe T.; Aaen K.; Fox A. D. (2009). "Escape distances from human pedestrians by staging waterbirds in a Danish wetland" (PDF). Wildfowl (Special Issue 2): 115–130. Retrieved 4 September 2012.[ permanent dead link ]
  12. 1 2 Carrete, M.; Tella, J.L. (2009). "Individual consistency in flight initiation distances in burrowing owls: a new hypothesis on disturbance-induced habitat selection". Biology Letters. 6 (2): 167–170. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2009.0739 . PMC   2865052 . PMID   19864278.
  13. 1 2 Cooper, W.E. (2000). "Plesiomorphic escape decisions in cryptic horned lizards (Phrynosoma) having highly derived antipredatory defenses" . Retrieved 20 April 2013.
  14. Eason, P.K.; Sherman, P.T; Rankin, O.; Coleman, B. (2006). "Factors affecting flight initiation distance in American robin". The Journal of Wildlife Management. 70 (6): 1796–1800. doi:10.2193/0022-541x(2006)70[1796:fafidi]2.0.co;2. S2CID   85926285.
  15. Cooper, W.E. (2009). "Flight initiation distance decreases during social activity in lizards (Sceloporus virgatus)". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 63 (12): 1765–1771. doi:10.1007/s00265-009-0799-1. S2CID   24549272.
  16. Dill, L.M.; Houtman, R. (1989). "The influence of distance to refuge on flight initiation distance in the gray squirrel (Sciurus carolinensis)" (PDF). Canadian Journal of Zoology. 67: 233–235. doi:10.1139/z89-033.
  17. 1 2 Fernandez-Juricic E.; Jimenez M. D.; Lucas E. (2001). "Alert distance as an alternative measure of bird tolerance to human disturbance- implications for park design" (PDF). Environmental Conservation. 28 (3): 263–269. doi:10.1017/S0376892901000273. S2CID   44189097 . Retrieved 4 September 2012.
  18. 1 2 Blumstein D. T. (Oct 2003). "Flight-Initiation Distance in Birds Is Dependent on Intruder Starting Distance" (PDF). The Journal of Wildlife Management. Allen Press. 67 (4): 852–857. doi:10.2307/3802692. JSTOR   3802692 . Retrieved 4 September 2012.
  19. 1 2 Alert distance as an alternative measure of bird tolerance to human disturbance: implications for park design
  20. 1 2 3 4 Møller, A. P.; Tryjanowski, P; Díaz, M; Kwieciński, Z; Indykiewicz, P; Mitrus, C; Golawski, A; Polakowski, M (2015). "Urban habitats and feeders both contribute to flight initiation distance reduction in birds". Behavioral Ecology. 26 (3): 861–865. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arv024 .
  21. 1 2 3 4 5 Møller, A. P.; Erritzøe, J. (2013). "Predator-prey interactions, flight initiation distance and brain size". Journal of Evolutionary Biology. 26 (1): 23–42. doi:10.1111/jeb.12272. PMID   25990564. S2CID   41897304.
  22. 1 2 Grandin, Temple. "Behavioural Principles of Livestock Handling". Vision, Hearing, and Handling methods in Cattle and Pigs. American Registry of Professional Animal Scientists . Retrieved 7 October 2013.
  23. Grandin, T (1980). "Observations of cattle behavior applied to the design of cattle-handling facilities". Appl. Anim. Ethol. 6: 19–31. doi:10.1016/0304-3762(80)90091-7.
  24. Grandin, T (1978). "Observations of the spatial relationships between people and cattle during handling". Proc. Western Sect., Am. Soc. Anim. Sci. 29: 76–79.
  25. 1 2 3 4 Grandin, Temple. "Behavioural Principles of Livestock Handling". Vision, Hearing, and Handling methods in Cattle and Pigs. American Registry of Professional Animal Scientists. Retrieved 7 October 2013.
  26. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Moran, J; Doyle, R (2015). Cow talk: understanding dairy cow behaviour to improve their welfare on Asian farms. Clayton South, Vic: CSIRO Publishing. pp. 48–49.
  27. Hutson, G.D. (1982). "Flight distance in Merino sheep". Animal Production. 35 (2): 231–235. doi:10.1017/s0003356100027409.
  28. Grandin T. (1989). "Behavioral principles of livestock handling" . Retrieved 22 April 2013.
  29. 1 2 Chambers, P.G.; Grandin, T.; Heinz, G.; Srisuvan, T. (2001). "Guidelines for Humane Handling, Transport and Slaughter of Livestock". Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations – Regional Office for Asia and the Pacific. Retrieved 22 April 2013.
  30. Grandin, T. (1983). Handling and processing feedlot cattle. In: G.B. Thompson and C.C. O'Mary (Eds) The Feedlot, Lea & Febiger, Philadelphia. pp. 213–235
  31. Grandin, Temple. "Understanding Flight Zones and Point of Balance". ProWay . ProWay Livestock Equipment. Retrieved 30 September 2020.
  32. Loewy, Arthur. "Human Nervous System". Britannica. Retrieved 7 October 2013.
  33. 1 2 3 Unknown, Unknown. "General Adaptation Syndrome". Oracle Education Foundation. Archived from the original on 19 October 2013. Retrieved 7 October 2013.
  34. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Gotzman J.; Desselberger J. (1979). Z lornetką wśród ptaków [With binoculars among birds]. Nasze hobby [Our hobby] (in Polish) (1st ed.). Warsaw: Państwowe Wydawnictwo Rolnicze i Leśne (PWRiL).

Further reading