Grand Valley Dani language

Last updated
Grand Valley Dani
Native to Indonesia
Region Highland Papua
Ethnicity Dani and Lani [1]
Native speakers
(90,000 cited 1990–1996) [2]
Language codes
ISO 639-3 Variously:
dni   Lower
dnt   Mid
dna   Upper
hap    Hupla
Glottolog gran1246

Grand Valley Dani, or simply Dani, [3] is one of the most populous Papuan languages in Indonesian New Guinea (also known as Papua). The Dani people live in the Baliem Valley of the Western Highlands.

Contents

Dialects

Dialectical differentiation is great enough that Ethnologue assigns separate codes to three varieties:

Lower Grand Valley Dani contains subdialects Lower Grand Valley Hitigima (Dani-Kurima, Kurima), Upper Bele, Lower Bele, Lower Kimbin (Kibin), and Upper Pyramid. Hupla, traditionally considered a separate language, is closer to Lower Grand Valley than the varieties of Grand Valley Dani are to each other.

Phonology

Grand Valley Dani has established its own orthography during a conference between linguists of the Dutch New Guinea government and different missionary bodies in February 1961. This is the phonology of the Central Grand Valley Dani language: [4]

Consonants

Consonants
Bilabial Alveolar Palatal Velar Glottal
plain lab.
Nasal m n ŋ
⟨ng⟩
Plosive voiceless p
⟨b, p⟩
t
⟨d, t⟩
k
⟨g, k⟩

⟨gw, kw⟩
ʔ
⟨'⟩
aspirated
⟨p, ph⟩

⟨t, th⟩

⟨k, kh⟩
kʷʰ
⟨kw, kwh⟩
implosive ɓ
⟨bp⟩
ɗ
⟨dl⟩
Fricative s h
Lateral l
Semivowel j w

Unlike other orthographies of local languages in Indonesia (largely based on the standard orthography), the original Grand Valley Dani orthography (the current one might be not known) has j instead of y, in common with the Indonesian old spelling.

Vowels

Vowels
front central back
close i u
ɪ
⟨y⟩
ʊ
⟨v⟩
mid e o
low a

Grammar

Verbs

Verbs in Grand Valley Dani are highly inflected for many tenses. Infinitive is marked by the suffix -in, although verb stems in -s- change to -t- before consonants: wetasin "to roast", but wetathy "I roasted".

Finite tenses

Default personal markers [7]
number
SingularPlural
person
1st-y-o
2nd-en-ep
3rd-e-em

Although there are claimed "default" personal markers, the correspondences between tense suffixes and personal markers are often highly irregular. Nevertheless, inflections of verbs are still highly regular. Unless denoted in the table, verb forms are marked by personal markers.

List
TenseSuffix(es)
Near future-ikin in the singular, -ukun in the plural.
Never inflected by person, only by number.
Indefinite future-isikin in the singular, -isukun in the plural.
Never inflected by person, only by number.
Near past-h-.
Remote past-hikh- in the 3SG, -hukh- in the 3PL, and -hVk- elsewhere.
-V- is an echo vowel from the personal markers, e.g. -hyky, -heken, etc.
Perfect pastSuffixing the near past with -tik in the 1SG, -ttik in the 2SG, -sip in the 2PL and -sik elsewhere.
In the second person, the final consonants of original near past endings, when suffixed, have to be deleted (-hen (2SG) + -ttik-hettik, -hep (2PL) + -sip-hesip).
The ending for 3PL is irregular: -hasik instead of *-hemsik.
HabitualReplacing every instances of syllable-final -i- and -sik (but not -sip-sep) of the perfect past with -e- and -tek, respectively (-hettik-hettek).
Habitual perfectInfixing -si- into the main habitual ending (-hettek-hettesik).
The ending for 2PL is irregular: -hesep-hetesip instead of *-hesesip.
SingularPlural
FirstSecondThirdFirstSecondThird
FutureNear-ikin-ukun
Indefinite-isikin-isukun
PastNear-hy-hen-he-ho-hep-hem
Remote-hyky-heken-hikhe-huku-hikip-hukha
Perfect-hytik-hettik-hesik-hosik-hesip-hasik
HabitualMain-hytek-hettek-hetek-hotek-hesep-hatek
Perfect-hytesik-hettesik-hetesik-hotesik-hetesip-hatesik
Progressive-hylahy-hylaken-iako-hylako-hylakep-iakoei

Semantics

The Dani language differentiates only two basic colours, mili for cool/dark shades such as blue, green, and black, and mola for warm/light colours such as red, yellow, and white. This trait makes it an interesting field of research for language psychologists, such as Eleanor Rosch, investigating the Whorf hypothesis. [8] [9]

Related Research Articles

Bench is a Northern Omotic language of the "Gimojan" subgroup, spoken by about 174,000 people in the Bench Maji Zone of the Southern Nations, Nationalities, and Peoples Region, in southern Ethiopia, around the towns of Mizan Teferi and Shewa Gimira. In a 2006 dissertation, Christian Rapold described three varieties of Bench as "...mutually intelligible...varieties of one and the same language". Bench is the ancestral language of the Bench people.

Kalanga, or TjiKalanga, is a Bantu language spoken by the Kalanga people in Botswana and Zimbabwe. It has an extensive phoneme inventory, which includes palatalised, velarised, aspirated and breathy-voiced consonants, as well as whistled sibilants.

Yaqui, locally known as Yoeme or Yoem Noki, is a Native American language of the Uto-Aztecan family. It is spoken by about 20,000 Yaqui people in the Mexican state of Sonora and across the border in Arizona in the United States. It is partially intelligible with the Mayo language, also spoken in Sonora, and together they are called Cahitan languages.

Tshangla is a Sino-Tibetan language of the Bodish branch closely related to the Tibetic languages. Tshangla is primarily spoken in Eastern Bhutan and acts as a lingua franca in the region; it is also spoken in the adjoining Tawang tract in the Indian state of Arunachal Pradesh and the Pemako region of Tibet. Tshangla is the principal pre-Tibetan language of Bhutan.

Yakkha is a language spoken in parts of Nepal, Darjeeling district and Sikkim. The Yakkha-speaking villages are located to the East of the Arun river, in the southern part of the Sankhuwasabha district and in the northern part of the Dhankuta district of Nepal. About 14,000 people still speak the language, out of 17,003 ethnic Yakkha in Nepal. Genealogically, Yakkha belongs to the Eastern Kiranti languages and is in one subgroup with several Limbu languages, e.g. Belhare, Athpare, Chintang and Chulung. Ethnically however, the Yakkha people perceive themselves as distinct from the other Kiranti groups such as Limbu.

Big Nambas is an Oceanic language spoken by about 3,400 people in northwest Malekula, Vanuatu. Approximately nineteen villages in the Big Nambas region of the Malekula Interior use the language exclusively with no variation in dialect. It was studied in-depth over a period of about 10 years by missionary Greg. J. Fox, who published a grammar and dictionary in 1979. A Big Nambas translation of the Bible has been completed recently by Andrew Fox.

The Gujarati language is an Indo-Aryan language native to the Indian state of Gujarat. Much of its phonology is derived from Sanskrit.

This article is about the sound system of the Navajo language. The phonology of Navajo is intimately connected to its morphology. For example, the entire range of contrastive consonants is found only at the beginning of word stems. In stem-final position and in prefixes, the number of contrasts is drastically reduced. Similarly, vowel contrasts found outside of the stem are significantly neutralized. For details about the morphology of Navajo, see Navajo grammar.

The Dani or Baliem Valley languages are a family of clearly related Trans–New Guinea languages spoken by the Dani and related peoples in the Baliem Valley in the Highland Papua, Indonesia. Foley (2003) considers their Trans–New Guinea language group status to be established. They may be most closely related to the languages of Paniai Lakes, but this is not yet clear. Capell (1962) posited that their closest relatives were the Kwerba languages, which Ross (2005) rejects.

Tifal is an Ok language spoken in Papua New Guinea. Dialects are Tifal (Tifalmin), Urap (Urapmin) and Atbal (Atbalmin).

Jarawa is the most populous of the Bantu languages of northern Nigeria. It is a dialect cluster consisting of many varieties.

Western Dani, or Lani, is a Nuclear-Trans-New Guinea language. It is the Papuan language with the most speakers in Indonesian New Guinea. It is spoken by the Lani people in the province of Highland Papua.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Siona language</span> Tucanoan language spoken in South America

Siona is a Tucanoan language of Colombia and Ecuador. The language is essentially the same as Secoya, but speakers are ethnically distinct.

The Kwaio language, or Koio, is spoken in the centre of Malaita Island in the Solomon Islands. It is spoken by about 13,000 people.

Kwerba is a Papuan language of Indonesia. Alternate names are Airmati (Armati), Koassa, Mataweja, Naibedj, Segar Tor, Tekutameso.

Vute is a Mambiloid language of Cameroon and Gabon, with a thousand speakers in Nigeria. The orthography was standardized on March 9, 1979. Noted dialect clusters are eastern, central, and Doume.

Awa is a Kainantu language of Papua New Guinea.

Wano is a Papuan language spoken by the Wano people in Puncak and Puncak Jaya regencies of the Indonesian province of Central Papua.

Matigsalug is a Manobo language of Mindanao in the Philippines. It belongs to the Austronesian language family.

The Bantayanon language is the regional language of the Bantayan islands in the Philippines. It is a part of the Bisayan language family and is closely related to Waray and Hiligaynon. There are three dialects of Bantayanon, based in the three municipalities that comprise the island group: Binantayanun, Linawisanun, and Sinantapihanun, the most idiosyncratic of the three. There are also significant dialectal differences between the speech patterns of those that live in the town centers and those that live outside of the more rural areas of the islands.

References

  1. Tabuni, Gasper (2017). Kunume Wene-Nya Masyarakat Adat Balim: Studi Kasus Makna 'Kunume Wene' dalam Perilaku Orang Kombarabuni dalam Jangkauan Zaman (Thesis). Magister Studi Pembangunan Program Pascasarjana UKSW. Retrieved 2024-06-18.
  2. Lower at Ethnologue (18th ed., 2015) (subscription required)
    Mid at Ethnologue (18th ed., 2015) (subscription required)
    Upper at Ethnologue (18th ed., 2015) (subscription required)
    Hupla at Ethnologue (18th ed., 2015) (subscription required)
  3. Compare Lani
  4. van der Stap 1966 , p. 10–11
  5. Bromley 1961 , p. 35
  6. Bromley 1961 , p. 31–32
  7. van der Stap 1966 , p. 22–23
  8. Saunders, Barbara Ann Christine (1992). The Invention of Basic Colour Terms. R.U.U.-I.S.O.R. ISBN   9789051870879 via Google Books.
  9. Heider, Eleanor Rosch (1972). "Probabilities, Sampling, and Ethnographic Method: The Case of Dani Colour Names". Man. 7 (3): 448–466. doi:10.2307/2800917. JSTOR   2800917.

Further reading