In survey research, the design effect is a number that shows how well a sample of people may represent a larger group of people for a specific measure of interest (such as the mean). This is important when the sample comes from a sampling method that is different than just picking people using a simple random sample.
The design effect is a positive real number, represented by the symbol . If , then the sample was selected in a way that is just as good as if people were picked randomly. When , then inference from the data collected is not as accurate as it could have been if people were picked randomly.
When researchers use complicated methods to pick their sample, they use the design effect to check and adjust their results. It may also be used when planning a study in order to determine the sample size.
In survey methodology, the design effect (generally denoted as , , or ) is a measure of the expected impact of a sampling design on the variance of an estimator for some parameter of a population. It is calculated as the ratio of the variance of an estimator based on a sample from an (often) complex sampling design, to the variance of an alternative estimator based on a simple random sample (SRS) of the same number of elements. [1] : 258 The (be it estimated, or known a priori) can be used to evaluate the variance of an estimator in cases where the sample is not drawn using simple random sampling. It may also be useful in sample size calculations [2] and for quantifying the representativeness of samples collected with various sampling designs.
The design effect is a positive real number that indicates an inflation (), or deflation () in the variance of an estimator for some parameter, that is due to the study not using SRS (with , when the variances are identical). [3] : 53, 54 Intuitively we can get when we have some a-priori knowledge we can exploit during the sampling process (which is somewhat rare). And, in contrast, we often get when we need to compensate for some limitation in our ability to collect data (which is more common). Some sampling designs that could introduce generally greater than 1 include: cluster sampling (such as when there is correlation between observations), stratified sampling (with disproportionate allocation to the strata sizes), cluster randomized controlled trial, disproportional (unequal probability) sample (e.g. Poisson sampling), statistical adjustments of the data for non-coverage or non-response, and many others. Stratified sampling can yield that is smaller than 1 when using Proportionate allocation to strata sizes (when these are known a-priori, and correlated to the outcome of interest) or Optimum allocation (when the variance differs between strata and is known a-priori).[ citation needed ]
Many calculations (and estimators) have been proposed in the literature for how a known sampling design influences the variance of estimators of interest, either increasing or decreasing it. Generally, the design effect varies among different statistics of interests, such as the total or ratio mean. It also matters if the sampling design is correlated with the outcome of interest. For example, a possible sampling design might be such that each element in the sample may have a different probability to be selected. In such cases, the level of correlation between the probability of selection for an element and its measured outcome can have a direct influence on the subsequent design effect. Lastly, the design effect can be influenced by the distribution of the outcome itself. All of these factors should be considered when estimating and using design effect in practice. [4] : 13
The term "design effect" was coined by Leslie Kish in his 1965 book "Survey Sampling." [1] : 88, 258 In it, Kish proposed the general definition for the design effect, [lower-alpha 1] as well as formulas for the design effect of cluster sampling (with intraclass correlation); [1] : 162 and the famous design effect formula for unequal probability sampling. [1] : 427 These are often known as "Kish's design effect", and were later combined into a single formula.
In a 1995 paper, [5] : 73 Kish mentions that a similar concept, termed "Lexis ratio", was described at the end of the 19th century. The closely related Intraclass correlation was described by Fisher in 1950, while computations of ratios of variances were already published by Kish and others from the late 1940s to the 1950s. One of the precursors to Kish's definition was work done by Cornfield in 1951. [6] [4]
In his 1995 paper, Kish proposed that considering the design effect is necessary when averaging the same measured quantity from multiple surveys conducted over a period of time. [5] : 57–62 He also suggested that the design effect should be considered when extrapolating from the error of simple statistics (e.g. the mean) to more complex ones (e.g. regression coefficients). However, when analyzing data (e.g., using survey data to fit models), values are less useful nowadays due to the availability of specialized software for analyzing survey data. Prior to the development of software that computes standard errors for many types of designs and estimates, analysts would adjust standard errors produced by software that assumed all records in a dataset were i.i.d by multiplying them by a (see Deft definition below).[ citation needed ]
Symbol | Description |
---|---|
Variance of an estimator under a given sampling design | |
Variance of an estimator under simple random sampling without replacement (SRSWOR) | |
Variance of an estimator under simple random sampling with replacement (SRSWR) | |
, | Design effect, a measure of the impact of a sampling design on the variance of an estimator compared to simple random sampling without replacement (SRSWOR), |
, | Design effect factor, the square root of the ratio of variances under a given sampling design and SRS with replacement (SRSWR), |
Sample size | |
Population size | |
Effective sample size, the sample size under SRS needed to achieve the same variance as the given sampling design, | |
Weight for the -th unit | |
Sample size for stratum | |
Population size for stratum | |
Weight for stratum | |
Total number of strata | |
, | Average cluster size |
Total number of clusters | |
Sample size for cluster | |
Intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) for cluster sampling | |
, , | Measures of variation in weights using the coefficient of variation (CV) squared (relvariance) |
Estimated correlation between the outcome variable and the selection probabilities | |
Estimated intercept in the linear regression of the outcome variable on the selection probabilities | |
Estimated standard deviation of the outcome variable | |
, | Coefficient of variation for the weights and selection probabilities , respectively |
Sampling fraction, | |
Population variance of the outcome variable | |
, | Selection probability for the -th unit |
Inclusion probability for the -th unit |
The design effect, commonly denoted by (or , sometimes with additional subscripts), is the ratio of two theoretical variances for estimators of some parameter (): [1] [7]
So that:
In other words, measures the extent to which the variance has increased (or, in some cases, decreased) because the sample was drawn and adjusted to a specific sampling design (e.g., using weights or other measures) compared to if the sample was from a simple random sample (without replacement). Notice how the definition of is based on parameters of the population that are often unknown, and that are hard to estimate directly. Specifically, the definition involves the variances of estimators under two different sampling designs, even though only a single sampling design is used in practice.[ citation needed ]
For example, when estimating the population mean, the (for some sampling design p) is: [4] : 4 [3] : 54 [lower-alpha 2]
Where is the sample size, is the fraction of the sample from the population, is the (squared) finite population correction (FPC), is the unbiassed sample variance, and is some estimator of the variance of the mean under the sampling design. The issue with the above formula is that it is extremely rare to be able to directly estimate the variance of the estimated mean under two different sampling designs, since most studies rely on only a single sampling design.
There are many ways of calculation , depending on the parameter of interest (e.g. population total, population mean, quantiles, ratio of quantities etc.), the estimator used, and the sampling design (e.g. clustered sampling, stratified sampling, post-stratification, multi-stage sampling, etc.). [8] : 98 The process of estimating for specific designs will be described in the following section.
A related quantity to , proposed by Kish in 1995, is the Design Effect Factor, abbreviated as (or also ). [5] : 56 [4] It is defined as the square root of the variance ratios while also having the denominator use a simple random sample with replacement (SRSWR), instead of without replacement (SRSWOR):
In this later definition (proposed in 1995, vs 1965) Kish argued in favor of using over for several reasons. It was argued that SRS "without replacement" (with its positive effect on the variance) should be captured in the denominator part in the definition of the design effect, since it is part of the sampling design. Also, since often the use of the factor is in confidence intervals), it was claimed that using will be simpler than writing . It is also said that for many cases when the population is very large, is (almost) the square root of (), hence it is easier to use than exactly calculating the finite population correction (FPC).[ citation needed ] [lower-alpha 3]
Even so, in various cases a researcher might approximate the by calculating the variance in the numerator while assuming SRS with replacement (SRSWR) instead of SRS without replacement (SRSWOR), even if it is not precise. For example, consider a multistage design with primary sampling units (PSUs) selected systematically with probability proportional to some measure of size from a list sorted in a particular way (say, by number of households in each PSU). Also, let it be combined with an estimator that uses raking to match the totals for several demographic variables. In such a design, the joint selection probabilities for the PSUs, which are needed for a without replacement variance estimator, are 0 for some pairs of PSUs - implying that an exact design-based (i.e., repeated sampling) variance estimator does not exist. Another example is when a public use file issued by some government agency is used for analysis. In such a case the information on joint selection probabilities of first-stage units is almost never released. As a result, an analyst cannot estimate a with replacement variance for the numerator even if desired. The standard workaround is to compute a variance estimator as if the PSUs were selected with replacement. This is the default choice in software packages such as Stata, the R survey package, and the SAS survey procedures.[ citation needed ]
The effective sample size, defined by Kish in 1965, is calculated by dividing the original sample size by the design effect. [1] : 162, 259 [9] : 190, 192 Namely:
This quantity reflects what would be the sample size that is needed to achieve the current variance of the estimator (for some parameter) with the existing design, if the sample design (and its relevant parameter estimator) were based on a simple random sample. [10]
A related quantity is the effective sample size ratio, which can be calculated by simply taking the inverse of (i.e., ).
For example, let the design effect, for estimating the population mean based on some sampling design, be 2. If the sample size is 1,000, then the effective sample size will be 500. It means that the variance of the weighted mean based on 1,000 samples will be the same as that of a simple mean based on 500 samples obtained using a simple random sample.
Different sampling designs and statistical adjustments may have substantially different impact on the bias and variance of estimators (such as the mean).[ citation needed ]
An example of a design which can lead to estimation efficiency, compared to simple random sampling, is Stratified sampling. This efficiency is gained by leveraging information about the composition of the population. For example, if it is known that gender is correlated with the outcome of interest, and also that the male-female ratio for some population is (say) 50%-50%, then sampling exactly half of the sample from each gender will reduce the variance of the outcome's estimator. Similarly, if a particular sub-population is of special interest, deliberately over-sampling from that sub-population will decrease the variance for estimations made about it.[ citation needed ]
Improvement in variance efficiency might sometimes be sacrificed for convenience or cost. For example, in the cluster sampling case the units may have equal or unequal selection probabilities, irrespective of their intra-class correlation (and their negative effect of increasing the variance of the estimators). We might decide (for practical reasons) to collect responses from only 2 people of each household (i.e., a sampled cluster), which could lead to more complex post-sampling adjustment to deal with unequal selection probabilities. Also, such decisions could lead to less efficient estimators than just taking a fixed proportion of responses from a cluster.[ citation needed ]
When the sampling design isn’t set in advance and needs to be figured out from the data we have, this can lead to an increase of both the variance and bias of the weighted estimator. This might happen when making adjustments for issues like non-coverage, non-response, or an unexpected strata split of the population that wasn’t available during the initial sampling stage. In these cases, we might use statistical procedures such as post-stratification, raking, or inverse propensity score weighting (where the propensity scores are estimated), among other methods. Using these methods requires assumptions about the initial design model. For example, when we use post-stratification based on age and gender, it is assumed that these variables can explain a significant portion of the bias in the sample. The quality of these estimators is closely tied to the quality of the additional information and the missing at random assumptions used when making them. Either way, even when estimators (like propensity score models) do a good job capturing most of the sampling design, using the weights can make a small or a large difference, depending on the specific data-set.[ citation needed ]
Due to the large variety in sampling designs (with or without an effect on unequal selection probabilities), different formulas have been developed to capture the potential design effect, as well as to estimate the variance of estimators when accounting for the sampling designs. [11] Sometimes, these different design effects can be compounded together (as in the case of unequal selection probability and cluster sampling, more details in the following sections). Whether or not to use these formulas, or just assume SRS, depends on the expected amount of bias reduction vs. the increase in estimator variance (and in the overhead of methodological and technical complexity). [1] : 426
Formula Name | Equation | Description |
---|---|---|
Kish's design effect for unequal weights | Measures the loss in precision due to unequal weights, where is the weight for the -th unit. | |
Kish's design effect for cluster sampling | Measures the loss in precision due to cluster sampling, where is the average cluster size and is the intraclass correlation. | |
Kish's combined design effect | Measures the combined effect of unequal weights and cluster sampling, where and are the sample size and weight for the -th stratum, respectively. | |
Spencer's design effect for estimated total | Measures the design effect for estimating a total when there is a correlation between the outcome and the selection probabilities, where is the estimated correlation, is the relvariance of the weights, is the estimated intercept, and is the estimated standard deviation of the outcome. | |
Park and Lee's design effect for estimated ratio mean | Measures the design effect for estimating a ratio mean when there is a correlation between the outcome and the selection probabilities, where and are the coefficients of variation for the weights and selection probabilities, respectively. | |
Henry's design effect for calibration weighting | Extends Kish's design effect to include calibration weighting in single-stage samples | Proposes a model-assisted design effect measure for single-stage sampling with calibration weighting, considering the correlation between the outcome and the calibration variables. |
Lohr's design effect for regression slope | Provides design effect formulas for OLS and GLS regression slope estimators in cluster sampling | Presents design effect formulas for ordinary least squares (OLS) and generalized least squares (GLS) regression slope estimators in the context of cluster sampling, using a random coefficient model. |
Source | Description | Examples | Impact on Sampling Probabilities |
---|---|---|---|
Disproportional sampling | Deliberately over/under sampling specific sub-populations or clusters | - Optimum allocation in stratified sampling - Oversampling smaller groups for comparison - Cluster sampling with unequal cluster sizes | Leads to unequal selection probabilities by design |
Non-coverage | Failure to include all elements of the target population in the sampling frame | - Sampling based on incomplete lists (e.g., phone books) - Advertising to recruit survey participants | Affects sampling probabilities, but the impact is difficult to measure and adjust for |
Non-response | Failure to obtain measurements from sampled units that were intended to be measured | - Unit non-response (e.g., refusal, not-at-home) - Item non-response (e.g., sensitive questions) - Inability to respond (e.g., language barrier, illness) | Leads to unequal selection probabilities, as non-response rates may vary across subgroups |
Statistical adjustments | Post-hoc adjustments to the sample weights to account for known population characteristics or to mitigate non-coverage and non-response biases | - Post-stratification - Raking - Propensity score weighting - Calibration weighting | Introduces unequal weights to improve representativeness, but may increase variance |
There are various ways to sample units so that each unit would have the exact same probability of selection. Such methods are called equal probability sampling (EPSEM) methods. Some of the more basic methods include simple random sampling (SRS, with or without replacement) and systematic sampling for getting a fixed sample size. There is also Bernoulli sampling with a random sample size. More advanced techniques such as stratified sampling and cluster sampling can also be designed to be EPSEM. For example, in cluster sampling we can use a two stage sampling in which we sample each cluster (which may be of different sizes) with equal probability, and then sample from each cluster at the second stage using SRS with a fixed proportion (e.g. sample half of the cluster, the whole cluster, etc.). This method will yield EPSEM, but the specific number of elements we end up with is stochastic (i.e., non deterministic). [lower-alpha 4] [12] : 3–8 Another strategy for cluster sampling that leads to EPSEM is to sample clusters in a way that is proportional to their sizes, and then sample a fixed number of elements inside each cluster. [lower-alpha 5]
In their works, Kish and others highlight several known reasons that lead to unequal selection probabilities: [1] : 425 [9] : 185 [5] : 69 [13] : 50, 395 [14] : 306
Adjusting for unequal probability selection through "individual case weights" (e.g. inverse probability weighting), yields various types of estimators for quantities of interest. Estimators such as Horvitz–Thompson estimator yield unbiased estimators (if the selection probabilities are indeed known, or approximately known), for total and the mean of the population. Deville and Särndal (1992) coined the term "calibration estimator" for estimators using weights such that they satisfy some condition, such as having the sum of weights equal the population size. And more generally, that the weighted sum of weights is equal some quantity of an auxiliary variable: (e.g., that the sum of weighted ages of the respondents is equal to the population size in each age group). [20] [17] : 132 [21] : 1
The two primary ways to argue about the properties of calibration estimators are: [17] : 133–134 [22]
As we will see later, some proofs in the literature rely on the randomization-based framework, while others focus on the model-based perspective. When moving from the mean to the weighted mean, more complexity is added. For example, in the context of survey methodology, often the population size itself is considered an unknown quantity that is estimated. So in the calculation of the weighted mean is in fact based on a ratio estimator, with an estimator of the total at the numerator and an estimator of the population size in the denominator (making the variance calculation to be more complex). [23] [3] : 182
Weight Type | Description | Interpretation |
---|---|---|
Frequency weights | Each weight is an integer indicating the absolute frequency of an item in the sample | Specific value has an absolute meaning; weights represent the amount of information in the dataset |
Inverse-variance weights | Each element is assigned a weight that is the inverse of its known variance | When all elements have the same expectancy, using such weights for weighted averages has the least variance |
Normalized (convex) weights | Weights form a convex combination (sum to 1); can be normalized to sum to sample size (n) | Weights that sum to n have a relative interpretation: elements with weights > 1 are more "rare" than average and have larger influential on (say) the average, while weights < 1 are more "common" and less influential |
Inverse probability weights | Each element is given a weight proportional to the inverse of its selection probability | Weights represent how many items each element "represents" in the target population; sum of weights equals the size of the target population |
There are many types (and subtypes) of weights, with different ways to use and interpret them. With some weights their absolute value has some important meaning, while with other weights the important part is the relative values of the weights to each other. This section introduces some of the more common types of weights so that they can be referenced in follow-up sections.
There are also indirect ways of applying "weighted" adjustments. For example, the existing cases may be duplicated to impute missing observations (e.g. from non-response), with variance estimated using methods such as multiple imputation. An alternative approach is to remove (assign a weight of 0 to) some cases. For example, when wanting to reduce the influence of over-sampled groups that are less essential for some analysis. Both cases are similar in nature to inverse probability weighting but the application in practice gives more/less rows of data (making the input potentially simpler to use in some software implementation), instead of applying an extra column of weights. Nevertheless, the consequences of such implementations are similar to just using weights. So while in the case of removing observations the data can easily be handled by common software implementations, the case of adding rows requires special adjustments for the uncertainty estimations. Not doing so may lead to erroneous conclusions(i.e., there is no free lunch when using alternative representation of the underlying issues). [9] : 189, 190
The term "Haphazard weights", coined by Kish, is used to refer to weights that correspond to unequal selection probabilities, but ones that are not related to the expectancy or variance of the selected elements. [9] : 190, 191
When taking an unrestricted sample of elements, we can then randomly split these elements into disjoint strata, each of them containing some size of elements so that . All elements in each stratum has some (known) non-negative weight assigned to them (). The weight can be produced by the inverse of some unequal selection probability for elements in each stratum (i.e., inverse probability weighting following a procedure such as post-stratification). In this setting, Kish's design effect, for the increase in variance of the sample weighted mean due to this design (reflected in the weights), versus SRS of some outcome variable y (when there is no correlation between the weights and the outcome, i.e. haphazard weights) is: [1] : 427 [9] : 191(4.2)
By treating each item as coming from its own stratum , Kish (in 1992) simplified the above formula to the (well-known) following version: [9] : 191(4.3) [26] : 318 [4] : 8
This version of the formula is valid when one stratum had several observations taken from it (i.e., each having the same weight), or when there are just many strata were each one had one observation taken from it, but several of them had the same probability of selection. While the interpretation is slightly different, the calculation of the two scenarios comes out to be the same.
When using Kish's design effect for unequal weights, you may use the following simplified formula for "Kish's Effective Sample Size" [27] [1] : 162, 259
The above formula, by Kish, gives the increase in the variance of the weighted mean based on "haphazard" weights. This can also be written as the following formula where y are observations selected using unequal selection probabilities (with no within-cluster correlation, and no relationship to the expectancy or variance of the outcome measurement), [9] : 190, 191 and y' are the observations we would have had if we got them from a simple random sample:
It can be shown that the ratio of variances formula can be reduced to Kish's formula by using a model based perspective. [28] In it, Kish's formula will hold when all n observations () are (at least approximately) uncorrelated (), with the same variance () in the response variable of interest (y). It will also be required to assume the weights themselves are not a random variable but rather some known constants (e.g. the inverse of probability of selection, for some pre-determined and known sampling design).[ citation needed ]
The following is a simplified proof for when there are no clusters (i.e., no Intraclass correlation between element of the sample) and each stratum includes only one observation: [28]
Transitions:
The conditions on y are trivially held if the y observations are IID with the same expectation and variance. In such cases, , and we can estimate by using . [9] [29] If the y's are not all with the same expectations then we cannot use the estimated variance for calculation, since that estimation assumes that all s have the same expectation. Specifically, if there is a correlation between the weights and the outcome variable y, then it means that the expectation of y is not the same for all observations (but rather, dependent on the specific weight value for each observation). In such a case, while the design effect formula might still be correct (if the other conditions are met), it would require a different estimator for the variance of the weighted mean. For example, it might be better to use a weighted variance estimator.[ citation needed ]
If different s values have different variances, then while the weighted variance could capture the correct population-level variance, Kish's formula for the design effect may no longer be true.[ citation needed ]
A similar issue happens if there is some correlation structure in the samples (such as when using cluster sampling).[ citation needed ]
Notice that Kish's definition of the design effect is closely tied to the coefficient of variation (Kish also calls it relvariance or relvar for short [lower-alpha 8] ) of the weights (when using the uncorrected (population level) sample standard deviation for estimation). This has several notations in the literature: [9] : 191 [13] : 396
Where is the population variance of , and is the mean. When the weights are normalized to sample size (so that their sum is equal to n and their mean is equal to 1), then and the formula reduces to . While it is true we assume the weights are fixed, we can think of their variance as the variance of an empirical distribution defined by sampling (with equal probability) one weight from our set of weights (similar to how we would think about the correlation of x and y in a simple linear regression).[ citation needed ]
Kish's original definition compared the variance under some sampling design to the variance achieved through a simple random sample. Some literature provide the following alternative definition for Kish's design effect: "the ratio of the variance of the weighted survey mean under disproportionate stratified sampling to the variance under proportionate stratified sampling when all stratum unit variances are equal". [26] : 318 [13] : 396 Reflecting on this, Park and Lee (2006) stated that "The rationale behind [...][Kish's] derivation is that the loss in precision of [the weighted mean] due to haphazard unequal weighting can be approximated by the ratio of the variance under disproportionate stratified sampling to that under the proportionate stratified sampling". [4] : 8
Note that this alternative definition only approximated since if the denominator is based on "proportionate stratified sampling" (achieved via stratified sampling) then such a selection will yield a reduced variance as compared with simple random sample. This is since stratified sampling removes some of the variability in the specific number of elements per stratum, as occurs under SRS.[ citation needed ]
Relatedly, Cochran (1977) provides a formula for the proportional increase in variance due to deviation from optimum allocation (what, in Kish's formulas, would be called L). [3] : 116
Early papers used the term . [9] : 192 As more definitions of the design effect appeared, Kish's design effect for unequal selection probabilities was denoted (or ) or simply for short. [4] : 8 [13] : 396 [26] : 318 Kish's design effect is also known as the "Unequal Weighting Effect" (or just UWE), termed by Liu et al. in 2002. [30] : 2124
The estimator for the total is the "p-expanded with replacement" estimator (a.k.a.: pwr-estimator or Hansen and Hurwitz). It is based on a simple random sample (with replacement, denoted SIR) of n items () from a population of size N. [lower-alpha 9] Each item has a probability of (k from 1 to N) to be drawn in a single draw (, i.e. it is a multinomial distribution). The probability that a specific will appear in the sample is . The "p-expanded with replacement" value is with the following expectancy: . Hence , the pwr-estimator, is an unbiased estimator for the sum total of y. [3] : 51
In 2000, Bruce D. Spencer proposed a formula for estimating the design effect for the variance of estimating the total (not the mean) of some quantity (), when there is correlation between the selection probabilities of the elements and the outcome variable of interest. [31]
In this setup, a sample of size n is drawn (with replacement) from a population of size N. Each item is drawn with probability (where , i.e. multinomial distribution). The selection probabilities are used to define the Normalized (convex) weights: . Notice that for some random set of n items, the sum of weights will be equal to 1 only by expectation () with some variability of the sum around it (i.e., the sum of elements from a Poisson binomial distribution). The relationship between and is defined by the following (population) simple linear regression:
Where is the outcome of element i, which linearly depends on with the intercept and slope . The residual from the fitted line is . We can also define the population variances of the outcome and the residuals as and . The correlation between and is .[ citation needed ]
Spencer's (approximate) design effect for estimating the total of y is: [31] : 138 [32] : 4 [13] : 401
Where:
This assumes that the regression model fits well so that the probability of selection and the residuals are independent, since it leads to the residuals, and the square residuals, to be uncorrelated with the weights, i.e., that and also . [31] : 138
When the population size (N) is very large, the formula can be written as: [26] : 319
(since , where )
This approximation assumes that the linear relationship between P and y holds. And also that the correlation of the weights with the errors, and the errors squared, are both zero. I.e., and . [32] : 4
We notice that if , then (i.e., the average of y). In such a case, the formula reduces to
Only if the variance of y is much larger than its mean, then the right-most term is close to 0 (i.e., ), which reduces Spencer's design effect (for the estimated total) to be equal to Kish's design effect (for the ratio means): [32] : 5 . Otherwise, the two formulas will yield different results, which demonstrates the difference between the design effect of the total vs. the design effect of the mean.
In 2001, Park and Lee extended Spencer's formula to the case of the ratio-mean (i.e., estimating the mean by dividing the estimator of the total with the estimator of the population size). It is: [32] : 4
Where:
Park and Lee's formula is exactly equal to Kish's formula when . Both formulas relate to the design effect of the mean of y, while Spencer's relates to the estimation of the population total.
In general, the for the total () tends to be less efficient than the for the ratio mean () when is small. And in general, impacts the efficiency of both design effects. [4] : 8
For data collected using cluster sampling we assume the following structure:
When clusters are all of the same size , the design effect Deff, proposed by Kish in 1965 (and later re-visited by others), is given by: [1] : 162 [13] : 399 [4] : 9 [34] [35] [14] : 241
It is sometimes also denoted as . [30] : 2124
In various papers, when cluster sizes are not equal, the above formula is also used with as the average cluster size (which is also sometimes denoted as ). [36] [28] : 105 In such cases, Kish's formula (using the average cluster weight) serves as a conservative (upper bound) of the exact design effect. [28] : 106
Alternative formulas exists for unequal cluster sizes. [1] : 193 Followup work had discussed the sensitivity of using the average cluster size with various assumptions. [37]
In a 1987 paper, Kish proposed a combined design effect that incorporates both the effects due to weighting that accounts for unequal selection probabilities and cluster sampling: [36] : 16 [28] : 105 [38] : 4 [32] : 2
The above uses notations similar to what is used in this article (the original 1987 publication used different notation). [lower-alpha 10] A model based justification for this formula was provided by Gabler et al. [28]
In 2000, Liu and Aragon proposed a decomposition of unequal selection probabilities design effect for different strata in stratified sampling. [39] In 2002, Liu et al. extended that work to account for stratified samples, where within each stratum is a set of unequal selection probability weights. The cluster sampling is either global or per stratum. [30] Similar work was done also by Park et al. in 2003. [40]
The Chen-Rust extends the model-based justification of Kish’s 1987 formula for design effects proposed by Gabler, el. al., [28] applying it to two-stage designs with stratification at the first stage and to three-stage designs without stratification. [41] The modified formulae define the overall design effect using survey weights and population intracluster correlations. These formulae allow for insightful interpretations of design effects from various sources and can estimate intracluster correlations in completed surveys or predict design effects in future surveys.[ citation needed ]
Henry's [26] proposes an extended model-assisted weighting design-effect measure for single-stage sampling and calibration weight adjustments for a case where , where is a vector of covariates, the model errors are independent, and the estimator of the population total is the general regression estimator (GREG) of Särndal, Swensson, and Wretman (1992). [3] The new measure considers the combined effects of non-epsem sampling design, unequal weights from calibration adjustments, and the correlation between an analysis variable and the auxiliaries used in calibration.
Lohr's [42] is for ordinary least squares (OLS) and generalized least squares (GLS) estimators in the context of cluster sampling, using a random coefficient regression model. Lohr presents conditions under which the GLS estimator of the regression slope has a design effect less than 1, indicating higher efficiency. However, the design effect of the GLS estimator is highly sensitive to model specification. If an underlying random coefficient model is incorrectly specified as a random intercept model, the design effect can be seriously understated. In contrast, the OLS estimator of the regression slope and the design effect calculated from a design-based perspective are robust to misspecification of the variance structure, making them more reliable in situations where the model specification may not be accurate.[ citation needed ]
may be used when planning a future data collection, as well as a diagnostic tool: [14] : 85
Considering the design effect is unnecessary when [5] : 57–62 the source population is closely IID, or when the sample design of the data was drawn as a simple random sample. It is also less useful when the sample size is relatively small (at least partially, for practical reasons).
While Kish originally hoped to have the design effect be as agnostic as possible to the underlying distribution of the data, sampling probabilities, their correlations, and the statistics of interest, followup research has shown that these do influence the design effect. Hence, these properties should be carefully considered when deciding which calculation to use, and how to use it. [4] : 13 [32] : 6
The design effect is rarely applied when constructing confidence intervals. Ideally, one would be able to determine, for an estimator of a particular parameter, both the variance under Simple Random Sample (SRS) with replacement and the design effect (which accounts for all elements of the sampling design that change the variance). In such scenarios, the basic variance and the design effect could have been multiplied to compute the variance of the estimator for the specific design. [1] : 259 This computed value can then be employed to form confidence intervals. However, in real-world applications, it is uncommon to estimate both values simultaneously. As a result, other methods are favored. For instance, Taylor linearization is utilized to construct confidence intervals based on the variance of the weighted mean. More broadly, the bootstrap method, also known as replication weights, is applied for a range of weighted statistics.[ citation needed ]
Kish's design effect is implemented in various statistical software packages:
In statistics, an estimator is a rule for calculating an estimate of a given quantity based on observed data: thus the rule, the quantity of interest and its result are distinguished. For example, the sample mean is a commonly used estimator of the population mean.
In mathematics, the harmonic mean is one of several kinds of average, and in particular, one of the Pythagorean means. It is sometimes appropriate for situations when the average rate is desired.
The median of a set of numbers is the value separating the higher half from the lower half of a data sample, a population, or a probability distribution. For a data set, it may be thought of as the “middle" value. The basic feature of the median in describing data compared to the mean is that it is not skewed by a small proportion of extremely large or small values, and therefore provides a better representation of the center. Median income, for example, may be a better way to describe the center of the income distribution because increases in the largest incomes alone have no effect on the median. For this reason, the median is of central importance in robust statistics.
In statistics, the standard deviation is a measure of the amount of variation of the values of a variable about its mean. A low standard deviation indicates that the values tend to be close to the mean of the set, while a high standard deviation indicates that the values are spread out over a wider range. The standard deviation is commonly used in the determination of what constitutes an outlier and what does not.
In probability theory and statistics, variance is the expected value of the squared deviation from the mean of a random variable. The standard deviation (SD) is obtained as the square root of the variance. Variance is a measure of dispersion, meaning it is a measure of how far a set of numbers is spread out from their average value. It is the second central moment of a distribution, and the covariance of the random variable with itself, and it is often represented by , , , , or .
The weighted arithmetic mean is similar to an ordinary arithmetic mean, except that instead of each of the data points contributing equally to the final average, some data points contribute more than others. The notion of weighted mean plays a role in descriptive statistics and also occurs in a more general form in several other areas of mathematics.
In statistics, the mean squared error (MSE) or mean squared deviation (MSD) of an estimator measures the average of the squares of the errors—that is, the average squared difference between the estimated values and the actual value. MSE is a risk function, corresponding to the expected value of the squared error loss. The fact that MSE is almost always strictly positive is because of randomness or because the estimator does not account for information that could produce a more accurate estimate. In machine learning, specifically empirical risk minimization, MSE may refer to the empirical risk, as an estimate of the true MSE.
In probability theory and statistics, the beta distribution is a family of continuous probability distributions defined on the interval [0, 1] or in terms of two positive parameters, denoted by alpha (α) and beta (β), that appear as exponents of the variable and its complement to 1, respectively, and control the shape of the distribution.
In statistics, an effect size is a value measuring the strength of the relationship between two variables in a population, or a sample-based estimate of that quantity. It can refer to the value of a statistic calculated from a sample of data, the value of a parameter for a hypothetical population, or to the equation that operationalizes how statistics or parameters lead to the effect size value. Examples of effect sizes include the correlation between two variables, the regression coefficient in a regression, the mean difference, or the risk of a particular event happening. Effect sizes are a complement tool for statistical hypothesis testing, and play an important role in power analyses to assess the sample size required for new experiments. Effect size are fundamental in meta-analyses which aim to provide the combined effect size based on data from multiple studies. The cluster of data-analysis methods concerning effect sizes is referred to as estimation statistics.
Importance sampling is a Monte Carlo method for evaluating properties of a particular distribution, while only having samples generated from a different distribution than the distribution of interest. Its introduction in statistics is generally attributed to a paper by Teun Kloek and Herman K. van Dijk in 1978, but its precursors can be found in statistical physics as early as 1949. Importance sampling is also related to umbrella sampling in computational physics. Depending on the application, the term may refer to the process of sampling from this alternative distribution, the process of inference, or both.
Estimation theory is a branch of statistics that deals with estimating the values of parameters based on measured empirical data that has a random component. The parameters describe an underlying physical setting in such a way that their value affects the distribution of the measured data. An estimator attempts to approximate the unknown parameters using the measurements. In estimation theory, two approaches are generally considered:
Sample size determination or estimation is the act of choosing the number of observations or replicates to include in a statistical sample. The sample size is an important feature of any empirical study in which the goal is to make inferences about a population from a sample. In practice, the sample size used in a study is usually determined based on the cost, time, or convenience of collecting the data, and the need for it to offer sufficient statistical power. In complex studies, different sample sizes may be allocated, such as in stratified surveys or experimental designs with multiple treatment groups. In a census, data is sought for an entire population, hence the intended sample size is equal to the population. In experimental design, where a study may be divided into different treatment groups, there may be different sample sizes for each group.
Bootstrapping is a procedure for estimating the distribution of an estimator by resampling one's data or a model estimated from the data. Bootstrapping assigns measures of accuracy to sample estimates. This technique allows estimation of the sampling distribution of almost any statistic using random sampling methods.
In statistics, the bias of an estimator is the difference between this estimator's expected value and the true value of the parameter being estimated. An estimator or decision rule with zero bias is called unbiased. In statistics, "bias" is an objective property of an estimator. Bias is a distinct concept from consistency: consistent estimators converge in probability to the true value of the parameter, but may be biased or unbiased.
In statistics, the jackknife is a cross-validation technique and, therefore, a form of resampling. It is especially useful for bias and variance estimation. The jackknife pre-dates other common resampling methods such as the bootstrap. Given a sample of size , a jackknife estimator can be built by aggregating the parameter estimates from each subsample of size obtained by omitting one observation.
In statistics, efficiency is a measure of quality of an estimator, of an experimental design, or of a hypothesis testing procedure. Essentially, a more efficient estimator needs fewer input data or observations than a less efficient one to achieve the Cramér–Rao bound. An efficient estimator is characterized by having the smallest possible variance, indicating that there is a small deviance between the estimated value and the "true" value in the L2 norm sense.
In statistics, the Horvitz–Thompson estimator, named after Daniel G. Horvitz and Donovan J. Thompson, is a method for estimating the total and mean of a pseudo-population in a stratified sample by applying inverse probability weighting to account for the difference in the sampling distribution between the collected data and the a target population. The Horvitz–Thompson estimator is frequently applied in survey analyses and can be used to account for missing data, as well as many sources of unequal selection probabilities.
Inverse probability weighting is a statistical technique for estimating quantities related to a population other than the one from which the data was collected. Study designs with a disparate sampling population and population of target inference are common in application. There may be prohibitive factors barring researchers from directly sampling from the target population such as cost, time, or ethical concerns. A solution to this problem is to use an alternate design strategy, e.g. stratified sampling. Weighting, when correctly applied, can potentially improve the efficiency and reduce the bias of unweighted estimators.
Taylor's power law is an empirical law in ecology that relates the variance of the number of individuals of a species per unit area of habitat to the corresponding mean by a power law relationship. It is named after the ecologist who first proposed it in 1961, Lionel Roy Taylor (1924–2007). Taylor's original name for this relationship was the law of the mean. The name Taylor's law was coined by Southwood in 1966.
The ratio estimator is a statistical estimator for the ratio of means of two random variables. Ratio estimates are biased and corrections must be made when they are used in experimental or survey work. The ratio estimates are asymmetrical and symmetrical tests such as the t test should not be used to generate confidence intervals.
This article was submitted to WikiJournal of Science for external academic peer review in 2023 ( reviewer reports ). The updated content was reintegrated into the Wikipedia page under a CC-BY-SA-3.0 license ( 2024 ). The version of record as reviewed is: Tal Galili; et al. (5 May 2024). "Design effect". WikiJournal of Science. 7 (1): 4. doi: 10.15347/WJS/2024.004 . ISSN 2470-6345. Wikidata Q116768211.
{{cite book}}
: CS1 maint: DOI inactive as of May 2024 (link)R package version 2.2