Speech error

Last updated

A speech error, commonly referred to as a slip of the tongue [1] (Latin: lapsus linguae, or occasionally self-demonstratingly, lipsus languae) or misspeaking, is a deviation (conscious or unconscious) from the apparently intended form of an utterance. [2] They can be subdivided into spontaneously and inadvertently produced speech errors and intentionally produced word-plays or puns. Another distinction can be drawn between production and comprehension errors. Errors in speech production and perception are also called performance errors. [3] Some examples of speech error include sound exchange or sound anticipation errors. In sound exchange errors, the order of two individual morphemes is reversed, while in sound anticipation errors a sound from a later syllable replaces one from an earlier syllable. [4] Slips of the tongue are a normal and common occurrence. One study shows that most people can make up to as much as 22 slips of the tongue per day. [5]

Contents

Speech errors are common among children, who have yet to refine their speech, and can frequently continue into adulthood. When errors continue past the age of 9 they are referred to as "residual speech errors" or RSEs. [6] They sometimes lead to embarrassment and betrayal of the speaker's regional or ethnic origins. However, it is also common for them to enter the popular culture as a kind of linguistic "flavoring". Speech errors may be used intentionally for humorous effect, as with spoonerisms.

Within the field of psycholinguistics, speech errors fall under the category of language production. Types of speech errors include: exchange errors, perseveration, anticipation, shift, substitution, blends, additions, and deletions. The study of speech errors has contributed to the establishment/refinement of models of speech production since Victoria Fromkin's pioneering work on this topic. [7]

Psycholinguistic explanations

Speech errors are made on an occasional basis by all speakers. [1] They occur more often when speakers are nervous, tired, anxious or intoxicated. [1] During live broadcasts on TV or on the radio, for example, nonprofessional speakers and even hosts often make speech errors because they are under stress. [1] Some speakers seem to be more prone to speech errors than others. For example, there is a certain connection between stuttering and speech errors. [8] Charles F. Hockett explains that "whenever a speaker feels some anxiety about possible lapse, he will be led to focus attention more than normally on what he has just said and on what he is just about to say. These are ideal breeding grounds for stuttering." [8] Another example of a "chronic sufferer" is Reverend William Archibald Spooner, whose peculiar speech may be caused by a cerebral dysfunction, but there is much evidence that he invented his famous speech errors (spoonerisms). [1]

An explanation for the occurrence of speech errors comes from psychoanalysis, in the so-called Freudian slip . Sigmund Freud assumed that speech errors are the result of an intrapsychic conflict of concurrent intentions. [1] "Virtually all speech errors [are] caused by the intrusion of repressed ideas from the unconscious into one's conscious speech output", Freud explained. [1] In fact, his hypothesis explains only a minority of speech errors. [1]

Psycholinguistic classification

There are few speech errors that clearly fall into only one category. The majority of speech errors can be interpreted in different ways and thus fall into more than one category. [9] For this reason, percentage figures for the different kinds of speech errors may be of limited accuracy. [10] Moreover, the study of speech errors gave rise to different terminologies and different ways of classifying speech errors. Here is a collection of the main types:

Types of speech errors
TypeDefinitionExample
Addition"Additions add linguistic material." [1] Target: We
Error: We and I
Anticipation"A later segment takes the place of an earlier segment." [1] Target: reading list
Error: leading list
BlendsBlends are a subcategory of lexical selection errors. [10] More than one item is being considered during speech production. Consequently, the two intended items fuse together. [1] Target: person/people
Error: perple
DeletionDeletions or omissions leave some linguistic material out. [1] Target: unanimity of opinion
Error: unamity of opinion
ExchangeExchanges are double shifts. Two linguistic units change places. [1] Target: getting your nose remodeled
Error: getting your model renosed
Lexical selection errorThe speaker has "problems with selecting the correct word". [10] Target: tennis racquet
Error: tennis bat
Malapropism, classicalThe speaker has the wrong beliefs about the meaning of a word. Consequently, they produce the intended word, which is semantically inadequate. Therefore, this is a competence error rather than a performance error. Malapropisms are named after 'Mrs. Malaprop', a character from Richard B. Sheridan's eighteenth-century play The Rivals . [3] Target: The flood damage was so bad they had to evacuate the city.
Error: The flood damage was so bad they had to evaporate the city.
Metathesis "Switching of two sounds, each taking the place of the other." [3] Target: pus pocket
Error: pos pucket
Morpheme-exchange error [10] Morphemes change places.Target: He has already packed two trunks.
Error: He has already packs two trunked.
Morpheme strandingMorphemes remain in place but are attached to the wrong words. [11] Target: He has already packed two trunks.
Error: He has already trunked two packs.
Omissioncf. deletionsTarget: She can't tell me.
Error: She can tell me.
Perseveration"An earlier segment replaces a later item." [1] Target: black boxes
Error: black bloxes
Residual speech errors"Distortions of late-developing sounds such as /s/, /l/, and /r/." [6] Target: The box is red.

Error: The box is wed.

Shift"One speech segment disappears from its appropriate location and appears somewhere else." [1] Target: She decides to hit it.
Error: She decide to hits it.
Sound-exchange errorTwo sounds switch places. [10] Target: Night life [nait laif]
Error: Knife light [naif lait]
Spoonerism A spoonerism is a kind of metathesis. Switching of initial sounds of two separate words. [3] They are named after Reverend William Archibald Spooner, who probably invented most of his famous spoonerisms. [10] Target: I saw you light a fire.
Error: I saw you fight a liar.
SubstitutionOne segment is replaced by an intruder. The source of the intrusion is not in the sentence. [1] Target: Where is my tennis racquet?
Error: Where is my tennis bat?
Word-exchange errorA word-exchange error is a subcategory of lexical selection errors. [10] Two words are switched.Target: I must let the cat out of the house.
Error: I must let the house out of the cat.

Speech errors can affect different kinds of segments or linguistic units:

Segments
SegmentExample
Distinctive or phonetic featuresTarget:clear blue sky
Error:glear plue sky (voicing)
Phonemes or soundsTarget:ad hoc
Error:odd hack
Sequences of soundsTarget:spoon feeding
Error:foon speeding
MorphemesTarget: sure
Error:unsure
WordsTarget: I hereby deputize you.
Error: I hereby jeopardize you.
PhrasesTarget: The sun is shining./The sky is blue.
Error: The sky is shining.

Types

Examples

  • "particuly" (particularly) ← elision
  • "syntaxically" (syntactically) ← vocabulary

Scientific relevance

Speech production is a highly complex and extremely rapid process, and thus research into the involved mental mechanisms proves to be difficult. [10] Investigating the audible output of the speech production system is a way to understand these mental mechanisms. According to Gary S. Dell "the inner workings of a highly complex system are often revealed by the way in which the system breaks down". [10] Therefore, speech errors are of an explanatory value with regard to the nature of language and language production. [12]

Performance errors may provide the linguist with empirical evidence for linguistic theories and serve to test hypotheses about language and speech production models. [13] For that reason, the study of speech errors is significant for the construction of performance models and gives insight into language mechanisms. [13]

Evidence and insights

Anticipation
Target: Take my bike.
Error: Bake my bike.
Perseveration
Target: He pulled a tantrum.
Error: He pulled a pantrum.
Speech errors involve substitutions, shifts, additions and deletions of segments. "In order to move a sound, the speaker must think of it as a separate unit." [3] Obviously, one cannot account for speech errors without speaking of these discrete segments. They constitute the planning units of language production. [1] Among them are distinctive features, phonemes, morphemes, syllables, words and phrases. Victoria Fromkin points out that "many of the segments that change and move in speech errors are precisely those postulated by linguistic theories." Consequently, speech errors give evidence that these units are psychologically real.
"There is a complex set of rules which the language user follows when making use of these units." [3] Among them are for example phonetic constraints, which prescribe the possible sequences of sounds. [3] Moreover, the study of speech error confirmed the existence of rules that state how morphemes are to be pronounced or how they should be combined with other morphemes. [3] The following examples show that speech errors also observe these rules:
Target: He likes to have his team rested. [rest+id]
Error: He likes to have his rest teamed. [ti:m+d]
Target: Both kids are sick. [kid+z]
Error: Both sicks are kids. [sik+s]
Here the past tense morpheme resp. the plural morpheme is phonologically conditioned, although the lemmas are exchanged. This proves that first the lemmas are inserted and then phonological conditioning takes place.
Target: Don't yell so loud! / Don't shout so loud!
Error: Don't shell so loud!
"Shout" and "yell" are both appropriate words in this context. Due to the pressure to continue speaking, the speaker has to make a quick decision which word should be selected. [8] This pressure leads to the speaker's attempt to utter the two words simultaneously, which resulted in the creation of a blend. [8] According to Charles F. Hockett there are six possible blends of "shout" and "yell". [8] Why did the speaker choose "shell" and not one of the alternatives? The speaker obeyed unconscious linguistic rules because he selected the blend, which satisfied the linguistic demands of these rules the best. [8] Illegal non-words are for example instantaneously rejected.
In conclusion, the rules which tell language users how to produce speech must also be part of our mental organization of language. [3]
Target: My thesis is too long.
Error: My thesis is too short.
In case of substitution errors both segments mostly belong to the same category, which means for example that a noun is substituted for a noun. Lexical selection errors are based on semantic relations such as synonymy, antonymy or membership of the same lexical field. [2] For this reason the mental lexicon is structured in terms of semantic relationships. [3]
Target: George's wife
Error: George's life
Target: fashion square
Error: passion square
Some substitution errors which are based on phonological similarities supply evidence that the mental lexicon is also organized in terms of sound. [3]
Four generalizations about speech errors have been identified: [1]
  1. Interacting elements tend to come from a similar linguistic environment, which means that initial, middle, final segments interact with one another.
  2. Elements that interact with one another tend to be phonetically or semantically similar to one another. This means that consonants exchange with consonants and vowels with vowels.
  3. Slips are consistent with the phonological rules of the language.
  4. There are consistent stress patterns in speech errors. Predominantly, both interacting segments receive major or minor stress.

Information obtained from performance additions

An example of the information that can be obtained is the use of "um" or "uh" in a conversation. [15] These might be meaningful words that tell different things, one of which is to hold a place in the conversation so as not to be interrupted. There seems to be a hesitant stage and fluent stage that suggest speech has different levels of production. The pauses seem to occur between sentences, conjunctional points and before the first content word in a sentence. That suggests that a large part of speech production happens there.

Schachter et al. (1991) conducted an experiment to examine if the numbers of word choices affect pausing. They sat in on the lectures of 47 undergraduate professors from 10 different departments and calculated the number and times of filled pauses and unfilled pauses. They found significantly more pauses in the humanities departments as opposed to the natural sciences. [16] These findings suggest that the greater the number of word choices, the more frequent are the pauses, and hence the pauses serve to allow us time to choose our words.

Slips of the tongue are another form of "errors" that can help us understand the process of speech production better. Slips can occur at various levels: syntactic, phrasal, lexical-semantic, morphological, and phonological. They can take multiple forms, such as additions, substitutions, deletions, exchanges, anticipations, perseverations, shifts, and haplologies M.F. Garrett, (1975). [17] Slips are orderly because language production is orderly.

There are some biases shown through slips of the tongue. One kind is a lexical bias which shows that the slips people generate are more often actual words than random sound strings. Baars Motley and Mackay (1975) found that it was more common for people to turn two actual words to two other actual words than when they do not create real words. [14] This suggests that lexemes might overlap somewhat or be stored similarly.

A second kind is a semantic bias which shows a tendency for sound bias to create words that are semantically related to other words in the linguistic environment. Motley and Baars (1976) found that a word pair like "get one" will more likely slip to "wet gun" if the pair before it is "damp rifle". These results suggest that we are sensitive to how things are laid out semantically. [18]

Euphemistic misspeaking

Since the 1980s, the word misspeaking has been used increasingly in politics to imply that errors made by a speaker are accidental and should not be construed as a deliberate attempt to misrepresent the facts of a case. As such, its usage has attracted a degree of media coverage, particularly from critics who feel that the term is overly approbative in cases where either ignorance of the facts or intent to misrepresent should not be discarded as possibilities. [19] [20]

The word was used by a White House spokesman after George W. Bush seemed to say that his government was always "thinking about new ways to harm our country and our people" (a classic example of a Bushism), and more famously by then American presidential candidate Hillary Clinton who recalled landing in at the US military outpost of Tuzla "under sniper fire" (in fact, video footage demonstrates that there were no such problems on her arrival). [20] [21] Other users of the term include American politician Richard Blumenthal, who incorrectly stated on a number of occasions that he had served in Vietnam during the Vietnam War. [20]

See also

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Language</span> Structured system of communication

Language is a structured system of communication that consists of grammar and vocabulary. It is the primary means by which humans convey meaning, both in spoken and written forms, and may also be conveyed through sign languages. The vast majority of human languages have developed writing systems that allow for the recording and preservation of the sounds or signs of language. Human language is characterized by its cultural and historical diversity, with significant variations observed between cultures and across time. Human languages possess the properties of productivity and displacement, which enable the creation of an infinite number of sentences, and the ability to refer to objects, events, and ideas that are not immediately present in the discourse. The use of human language relies on social convention and is acquired through learning.

A lexicon is the vocabulary of a language or branch of knowledge. In linguistics, a lexicon is a language's inventory of lexemes. The word lexicon derives from Greek word λεξικόν, neuter of λεξικός meaning 'of or for words'.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Morphology (linguistics)</span> Study of words, their formation, and their relationships in a word

In linguistics, morphology is the study of words, how they are formed, and their relationship to other words in the same language. It analyzes the structure of words and parts of words such as stems, root words, prefixes, and suffixes. Morphology also looks at parts of speech, intonation and stress, and the ways context can change a word's pronunciation and meaning. Morphology differs from morphological typology, which is the classification of languages based on their use of words, and lexicology, which is the study of words and how they make up a language's vocabulary.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Phonetics</span> Study of the sounds of human language

Phonetics is a branch of linguistics that studies how humans produce and perceive sounds, or in the case of sign languages, the equivalent aspects of sign. Linguists who specialize in studying the physical properties of speech are phoneticians. The field of phonetics is traditionally divided into three sub-disciplines based on the research questions involved such as how humans plan and execute movements to produce speech, how various movements affect the properties of the resulting sound, or how humans convert sound waves to linguistic information. Traditionally, the minimal linguistic unit of phonetics is the phone—a speech sound in a language which differs from the phonological unit of phoneme; the phoneme is an abstract categorization of phones, and it is also defined as the smallest unit that discerns meaning between sounds in any given language.

Psycholinguistics or psychology of language is the study of the interrelation between linguistic factors and psychological aspects. The discipline is mainly concerned with the mechanisms by which language is processed and represented in the mind and brain; that is, the psychological and neurobiological factors that enable humans to acquire, use, comprehend, and produce language.

A root is the core of a word that is irreducible into more meaningful elements. In morphology, a root is a morphologically simple unit which can be left bare or to which a prefix or a suffix can attach. The root word is the primary lexical unit of a word, and of a word family, which carries aspects of semantic content and cannot be reduced into smaller constituents. Content words in nearly all languages contain, and may consist only of, root morphemes. However, sometimes the term "root" is also used to describe the word without its inflectional endings, but with its lexical endings in place. For example, chatters has the inflectional root or lemma chatter, but the lexical root chat. Inflectional roots are often called stems. A root, or a root morpheme, in the stricter sense, may be thought of as a monomorphemic stem.

In linguistics, prosody is the study of elements of speech that are not individual phonetic segments but which are properties of syllables and larger units of speech, including linguistic functions such as intonation, stress, and rhythm. Such elements are known as suprasegmentals.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Word</span> Basic element of language

A word is a basic element of language that carries meaning, can be used on its own, and is uninterruptible. Despite the fact that language speakers often have an intuitive grasp of what a word is, there is no consensus among linguists on its definition and numerous attempts to find specific criteria of the concept remain controversial. Different standards have been proposed, depending on the theoretical background and descriptive context; these do not converge on a single definition. Some specific definitions of the term "word" are employed to convey its different meanings at different levels of description, for example based on phonological, grammatical or orthographic basis. Others suggest that the concept is simply a convention used in everyday situations.

Victoria Alexandra Fromkin was an American linguist who taught at UCLA. She studied slips of the tongue, mishearing, and other speech errors, which she applied to phonology, the study of how the sounds of a language are organized in the mind.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Speech</span> Human vocal communication using spoken language

Speech is a human vocal communication using language. Each language uses phonetic combinations of vowel and consonant sounds that form the sound of its words, and using those words in their semantic character as words in the lexicon of a language according to the syntactic constraints that govern lexical words' function in a sentence. In speaking, speakers perform many different intentional speech acts, e.g., informing, declaring, asking, persuading, directing, and can use enunciation, intonation, degrees of loudness, tempo, and other non-representational or paralinguistic aspects of vocalization to convey meaning. In their speech, speakers also unintentionally communicate many aspects of their social position such as sex, age, place of origin, physical states, psychological states, physico-psychological states, education or experience, and the like.

Speech segmentation is the process of identifying the boundaries between words, syllables, or phonemes in spoken natural languages. The term applies both to the mental processes used by humans, and to artificial processes of natural language processing.

Catherine Phebe Browman was an American linguist and speech scientist. She received her Ph.D. in linguistics from the University of California, Los Angeles (UCLA) in 1978. Browman was a research scientist at Bell Laboratories in New Jersey (1967–1972). While at Bell Laboratories, she was known for her work on speech synthesis using demisyllables. She later worked as researcher at Haskins Laboratories in New Haven, Connecticut (1982–1998). She was best known for developing, with Louis Goldstein, of the theory of articulatory phonology, a gesture-based approach to phonological and phonetic structure. The theoretical approach is incorporated in a computational model that generates speech from a gesturally-specified lexicon. Browman was made an honorary member of the Association for Laboratory Phonology.

Language production is the production of spoken or written language. In psycholinguistics, it describes all of the stages between having a concept to express and translating that concept into linguistic forms. These stages have been described in two types of processing models: the lexical access models and the serial models. Through these models, psycholinguists can look into how speeches are produced in different ways, such as when the speaker is bilingual. Psycholinguists learn more about these models and different kinds of speech by using language production research methods that include collecting speech errors and elicited production tasks.

The term linguistic performance was used by Noam Chomsky in 1960 to describe "the actual use of language in concrete situations". It is used to describe both the production, sometimes called parole, as well as the comprehension of language. Performance is defined in opposition to "competence"; the latter describes the mental knowledge that a speaker or listener has of language.

Speech production is the process by which thoughts are translated into speech. This includes the selection of words, the organization of relevant grammatical forms, and then the articulation of the resulting sounds by the motor system using the vocal apparatus. Speech production can be spontaneous such as when a person creates the words of a conversation, reactive such as when they name a picture or read aloud a written word, or imitative, such as in speech repetition. Speech production is not the same as language production since language can also be produced manually by signs.

Linguistics is the scientific study of language. Linguistics is based on a theoretical as well as a descriptive study of language and is also interlinked with the applied fields of language studies and language learning, which entails the study of specific languages. Before the 20th century, linguistics evolved in conjunction with literary study and did not exclusively employ scientific methods.

In psychology, a lemma is an abstract conceptual form of a word that has been mentally selected prior to the early stages of speech production. This concept is used to explain how the process of generating speech occurs. In particular, lemmas are seen as the mental representations of words that are organised and retrieved from memory before they are eventually spoken.. A lemma represents a specific meaning but does not have any specific sounds that are attached to it.

The mental lexicon is defined as a mental dictionary that contains information regarding the word store of a language user, such as their meanings, pronunciations, and syntactic characteristics. The mental lexicon is used in linguistics and psycholinguistics to refer to individual speakers' lexical, or word, representations. However, there is some disagreement as to the utility of the mental lexicon as a scientific construct.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Bilingual memory</span>

Bilingualism is the regular use of two fluent languages, and bilinguals are those individuals who need and use two languages in their everyday lives. A person's bilingual memories are heavily dependent on the person's fluency, the age the second language was acquired, and high language proficiency to both languages. High proficiency provides mental flexibility across all domains of thought and forces them to adopt strategies that accelerate cognitive development. People who are bilingual integrate and organize the information of two languages, which creates advantages in terms of many cognitive abilities, such as intelligence, creativity, analogical reasoning, classification skills, problem solving, learning strategies, and thinking flexibility.

Prosodic bootstrapping in linguistics refers to the hypothesis that learners of a primary language (L1) use prosodic features such as pitch, tempo, rhythm, amplitude, and other auditory aspects from the speech signal as a cue to identify other properties of grammar, such as syntactic structure. Acoustically signaled prosodic units in the stream of speech may provide critical perceptual cues by which infants initially discover syntactic phrases in their language. Although these features by themselves are not enough to help infants learn the entire syntax of their native language, they provide various cues about different grammatical properties of the language, such as identifying the ordering of heads and complements in the language using stress prominence, indicating the location of phrase boundaries, and word boundaries. It is argued that prosody of a language plays an initial role in the acquisition of the first language helping children to uncover the syntax of the language, mainly due to the fact that children are sensitive to prosodic cues at a very young age.

References

  1. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 Carroll, David (1986). Psychology of language. Pacific Grove, CA, USA: Brooks/Cole Pub. Co. pp. 253–256. ISBN   978-0-534-05640-7. OCLC   12583436.
  2. 1 2 Bussmann, Hadumod. Routledge dictionary of language and linguistics. Routledge: London 1996, 449.
  3. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 Tserdanelis, Georgios; Wai Sum Wong (2004). Language files: materials for an introduction to language & linguistics . Columbus: Ohio State University Press. pp.  320–324. ISBN   978-0-8142-0970-7. OCLC   54503589.
  4. Dell, Gary S.; Reich, Peter A. (December 1981). "Stages in sentence production: An analysis of speech error data". Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 20 (6): 611–629. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(81)90202-4.
  5. "Slips of the Tongue". Psychology Today. Retrieved 16 May 2017.
  6. 1 2 Preston, Jonathan; Byun, Tara (November 2015). "Residual Speech Errors: Causes, Implications, Treatment". Seminars in Speech and Language. 36 (4): 215–216. doi:10.1055/s-0035-1562904. ISSN   0734-0478. PMID   26458196. S2CID   39709535.
  7. Fromkin, Victoria. "The Non-Anomalous Nature of Anomalous Utterances" (PDF). Stanford.
  8. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Hockett, Charles F. (1973). "Where the tongue slips, there slip I". In Victoria Fromkin (ed.). Speech errors as linguistic evidence. The Hague: Mouton. pp. 97–114. OCLC   1009093.
  9. Pfau, Roland. Grammar as processor: a distributed morphology account of spontaneous speech. John Benjamins Publishing Co.: Amsterdam 2009, 10.
  10. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 Eysenck, Michael W.; Keane, Mark A. (2005). Cognitive Psychology: A Student's Handbook. Psychology Press (UK). p. 402. ISBN   978-1-84169-359-0. OCLC   608153953.
  11. Anderson, John R. Kognitive Psychologie. Spektrum Akademischer Verlag: Heidelberg 1996 (2nd edition), 353.
  12. 1 2 Smith, Derek J. "Speech Errors, Speech Production Models, and Speech Pathology." Human Information Processing. Date of last revision: 12 December 2003. Date of access: 27 February 2010. "Speech-errors". Archived from the original on 5 December 2007. Retrieved 5 December 2007..
  13. 1 2 Fromkin, Victoria (1973). "Introduction". In Victoria Fromkin (ed.). Speech errors as linguistic evidence. The Hague: Mouton. p. 13. ISBN   978-90-279-2668-5. OCLC   1009093.
  14. 1 2 Baars, Bernard J.; Michael T. Motley; Donald G. MacKay (August 1975). "Output editing for lexical status in artificially elicited slips of the tongue". Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 14 (4): 382–391. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(75)80017-X.
  15. Clark HH, Fox Tree JE (May 2002). "Using uh and um in spontaneous speaking". Cognition. 84 (1): 73–111. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.5.7958 . doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00017-3. PMID   12062148. S2CID   37642332.
  16. Schachter, Stanley; Nicholas Christenfeld; Bernard Ravina; Frances Bilous (March 1991). "Speech Disfluency and the Structure of Knowledge". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 60 (3): 362–367. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.60.3.362.[ dead link ]
  17. Garrett, M. F. (1975). "The analysis of sentence production.". In Gordon H Bower (ed.). The Psychology of learning and motivation. Volume 9 : advances in research and theory. New York: Academic Press. pp.  133–177. ISBN   978-0-12-543309-9. OCLC   24672687.
  18. Motley, Michael T.; Bernard J. Baars (1976). "Semantic bias effects on the outcomes of verbal slips". Cognition. 43 (2): 177–187. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(76)90003-2. S2CID   53152698.[ dead link ]
  19. Hendrik Hertzberg (21 April 2008). "Mr. and Ms. Spoken". The New Yorker. Retrieved 14 August 2011.
  20. 1 2 3 Dominic Lawson (23 May 2010). "Don't lie – try misspeaking instead". The Sunday Times. Retrieved 28 August 2011.
  21. "Does 'misspeak' mean lying?". BBC News. 26 March 2008. Retrieved 28 August 2011.

Further reading