Onondaga language

Last updated
Onondaga
Onǫdaʼgegáʼ / Onoñdaʼgegáʼ
Pronunciation [onũdaʔɡeɡáʔ]
Native to Canada, United States
Region Six Nations Reserve, Ontario, and central New York state
Ethnicity1,600 Onondaga people (2007) [1]
Native speakers
40 (2007) [1]
Revival increasing numbers since 2010 [2]
Iroquoian
  • Northern
    • Lake Iroquoian
      • Five Nations
        • Onondaga
Language codes
ISO 639-3 ono
Glottolog onon1246
ELP Onondaga
This article contains IPA phonetic symbols. Without proper rendering support, you may see question marks, boxes, or other symbols instead of Unicode characters. For an introductory guide on IPA symbols, see Help:IPA.

The Onondaga language (Onoñdaʼgegáʼ nigaweñoʼdeñʼ, IPA: [onũdaʔɡeɡáʔniɡawẽnoʔdẽʔ] , literally "Onondaga is our language") is the language of the Onondaga First Nation, one of the original five constituent tribes of the League of the Iroquois (Haudenosaunee).

This language is spoken in the United States and Canada, primarily on the reservation in central New York State and near Brantford, Ontario.

Usage and revitalization

According to the UNESCO Atlas of the World's Languages in Danger, there are about 10 mother tongue Onondaga speakers in New York, and 40 native speakers on the Six Nations Reserve in Ontario, Canada. [3] The language has come to be endangered due to the pressure to assimilate to English as the language of power. Standardization also occurred in residential schools across Canada in the 1800s to 1900s. Young boys and girls at the Mohawk Institute Indian Residential School in Brantford, Ontario were punished for using their heritage language. [4]

The Onondaga Nation Language Center (called Neʼ Eñhadiweñnayeñdeʼnhaʼ, or "they will get to know the language") has been engaged in language revitalization efforts since 2010. Children learn the Onondaga language at Onondaga Nation School, and classes are also available for adults. In September 2015, it was announced that fifteen adults would enter a full-time language immersion class in Onondaga, after which they would become teachers of the language. [2] In Canada, Gawęnahwishe' Onǫda'gega' is a revitalization project that launched in 2017 with six new language learners. It is an adult immersion program that implements the Six Nations Language Commission's (SNLC) framework. [5] They are involved with translating the local radio station, putting on programs with local elementary and secondary schools, and outreach events for the community. [5]

Phonology

This table shows the (consonant) phonemes that are found in Onondaga.

Consonant phonemes
Alveolar Postalveolar
/ Palatal
Velar Glottal
Plosive tkʔ
Affricate
Fricative sh
Sonorant njw

The two plosives, /t/, /k/ are allophonically voiced to [d] and [ɡ] before vowels and resonants (the bottom row of the chart labelled 'sonorant') and are spelled d and g in this case. There is considerable palatalization and affrication in the language.

Front Central Back
Close i ũ
Mid e o
Open æ a

Onondaga has five oral vowels, /ieoæa/ (/æ/ is sometimes represented orthographically as ä), and two nasal vowels, /ẽ/ and /ũ/. The nasal vowels, following the Iroquoianist tradition, are spelled with ogoneks in the scholarly literature and in Ontario (ę and ǫ or ų). In New York, they are represented with a following ñ ( and ). Vowels can be both short and long. When vowel length derives from the now lost consonant *r, it is phonemic. Vowel length is written with a following colon, or raised dot (half colon) .

Morphology

Onondaga is a polysynthetic language, exhibiting a great deal of inflectional and derivational morphology on the verbal forms (including noun incorporation). Nominal forms have less morphology. Additionally, there are particles, which are monomorphemic.

Verbal morphology

Onondaga verbs can be divided into three main classes according to their aspectual properties (discussed below). These are the active verbs, motion verbs, and stative verbs. We must distinguish between tense and aspect. Tense refers to when the event takes place, either in the past, the present or the future. Aspect refers to the event itself, such as whether it is finished or ongoing or occurs repeatedly.

There are four aspects in Onondaga. The first is the habitual aspect (HAB). This aspect is used to refer to an event that takes place repeatedly or on an ongoing basis. The second is the punctual aspect (PUNC) (also known as perfective aspect). This aspect refers to an entire event in its completeness. When used in the past tense, the event is described as "over and done-with." It cannot describe an event that is interrupted or incomplete. The third aspect is the stative (STAT) (also known as imperfective) refers to an event that is ongoing or incomplete or, if it occurs in the past tense, that has some bearing on the present. Finally, there is the purposive aspect (PURP), which refers to imminent action and usually implies intent or volition on the part of the subject. Active verbs can appear with any of the first three aspects. Motion verbs can appear with any of all four aspects. Stative verbs can only appear with the stative aspect.

Verbal template

A typical Onondaga verb consists of several morphemes (components). The following chart outlines the order of the morphemes. Obligatory morphemes appear in boldface, and optional morphemes are in standard font. Note that some of the "optional" morphemes are obligatory with certain verb roots. The obligatory morphemes, however, must appear on each and every single verb.

pre-pronominal prefixespronominal prefixesreflexive or semireflexiveincorporated nounverb rootderivational suffixesaspect suffixexpanded aspect suffix

Each of the following sections outlines the shapes that these morphemes can take.

Pre-pronominal prefixes

The prepronominal prefixes express a variety of concepts and ideas. The first concept is modality, which expresses the degree of urgency, certainty or likelihood of the event. There are three modal prefixes in Onondaga. The modal prefixes only appear with the punctual aspect. They also appear if there is a modalizer suffix.

The first of these is commonly called the future modal prefix. This prefix expresses events that have not yet taken place at the time the speaker is talking.

ęhayę́꞉twaʔ

ę-

FUT-

ha-

3.SG.M.AG-

yętw-

plant-

PUNC

ę- ha- yętw- aʔ

FUT- 3.SG.M.AG- plant- PUNC

'He will plant it.'

The second is the factual modal prefix. This prefix indicates that the speaker knows the event happened for a fact. It typically has a past tense reading (since we are normally only sure about events that happened in the past).

waʔhayę́꞉twaʔ

waʔ-

FACT-

ha-

3.SG.M.AG-

yę꞉tw-

plant-

PUNC

waʔ- ha- yę꞉tw- aʔ

FACT- 3.SG.M.AG- plant- PUNC

'He planted it.'

The third is optative modal prefix. This prefix expresses the idea that the event should or ought to take place. It is also used to express untrue events or events that might have taken place but have not. Here are some examples.

ahayę́꞉twaʔ

a-

OPT-

ha-

3.SG.M.AG-

yę꞉tw-

plant-

PUNC

a- ha- yę꞉tw- aʔ

OPT- 3.SG.M.AG- plant- PUNC

'He might plant it.'

The following chart lists forms of the three modal prepronominal prefixes and indicates when to use which form.

MoodPre-pronominal PrefixWhen usedExample
factual waʔdefault
waʔhayę́꞉twaʔ

waʔ-ha-yę꞉tw-aʔ

FACT-he-plant-PUNC

waʔ-ha-yę꞉tw-aʔ

FACT-he-plant-PUNC

'He planted it.'

weʔany 2nd person or 1st person inclusive (except 2.SG.AG)
weʔdniyę́꞉twaʔ

weʔ-dni-yę꞉tw-aʔ

FACT-1.DU.INCL.AG-plant-PUNC

weʔ-dni-yę꞉tw-aʔ

FACT-1.DU.INCL.AG-plant-PUNC

'You and I planted it.'

ǫoptionally replaces sequence waʔwa
ǫgyę́꞉twaʔ

waʔ-wak-yę꞉tw-aʔ

FACT-1.SG.PAT-plant-PUNC

waʔ-wak-yę꞉tw-aʔ

FACT-1.SG.PAT-plant-PUNC

'It planted me.' (ex, a monster)

future ędoes not change
ęhayę́꞉twaʔ

ę-ha-yę꞉tw-aʔ

FUT-he-plant-PUNC

ę-ha-yę꞉tw-aʔ

FUT-he-plant-PUNC

'He will plant it.'

optative a / a꞉default (/a-/ and /a꞉-/ are in free variation)
ahayę́꞉twaʔ

a-ha-yę꞉tw-aʔ

OPT-he-plant-PUNC

a-ha-yę꞉tw-aʔ

OPT-he-plant-PUNC

'He might plant it.'

aeany 2nd person or 1st person inclusive (except 2.SG.AG)
aedniyę́꞉twaʔ

ae-dni-yę꞉tw-aʔ

OPT-1.DU.INCL.AG-plant-PUNC

ae-dni-yę꞉tw-aʔ

OPT-1.DU.INCL.AG-plant-PUNC

'You and I might plant it.'

optionally replaces sequence /awa/
aǫgyę́꞉twaʔ

a-wak-yę꞉tw-aʔ

OPT-1.SG.PAT-plant-PUNC

a-wak-yę꞉tw-aʔ

OPT-1.SG.PAT-plant-PUNC

'It might plant me.' (ex, a monster)

Non-modal pre-pronominal prefixes

In addition to the modal prefixes, there is also a set of prefixes that express a variety of concepts, some of which do not have a clearly distinct meaning, rather their meaning varies depending on context. The list of these includes repetitive, cislocative, dualic, translocative, partitive, coincident, contrastive, and negative.

Repetitive

The repetitive morpheme adds the meaning of doing something again or repeating some-thing. The basic form of the repetitive morpheme is /s-/. Here are some examples. Example (37) has the prepronominal prefix /sa-/, which is a combination of both repetitive and factual mood. Example (38) has the prepronominal prefix /ęs-/, which is a combination of repetitive and future. These contrast with example (39), which does not have the repetitive morpheme.

a.
sahayę́꞉twaʔ

sa–

REP.FACT-

ha–

3.SG.M-

yętw–

plant-

PUNC

sa– ha– yętw– aʔ

REP.FACT- 3.SG.M- plant- PUNC

'He planted it again.'

b.
ęshayę́꞉twaʔ

ęs–

REP.FUT-

ha–

3.SG.M-

yętw–

plant-

PUNC

ęs– ha– yętw– aʔ

REP.FUT- 3.SG.M- plant- PUNC

'He will plant it again.'

c.
waʔhayę́꞉twaʔ

waʔ–

FACT-

ha–

3.SG.M-

yętw–

plant-

PUNC

waʔ– ha– yętw– aʔ

FACT- 3.SG.M- plant- PUNC

'He planted it.'

Cislocative

The cislocative (CLOC) morpheme is used to indicate movement toward the speaker. It can also mean that a particular event is pinpointed back in time. In some cases, the meaning of the cislocative is unpredictable. Some of these are listed below. There are two forms of the cislocative.

/t-/ default
/di-/ used with any 2nd person or 1st person inclusive, except 2.SG.AG

Translocative

The translocative (TLOC) morpheme is used to indicate movement away from the speaker. The form of the translocative is /he-/

Dualic

The dualic (DUC) does not have a specific meaning. The form of the dualic is /de-/, but changes when it appears in combination with other prepronominal prefixes. Whenever it appears with a verb stem, it changes the meaning in unpredictable ways. Usually, however, there is some notion of there being two of something or of some reciprocal activity such as trading. Also, some verb roots must appear with the dualic prepronominal prefix. In examples (41) and (42), the dualic prefix is obligatory. In example (43), the dualic prefix adds the meaning of becoming two pieces.

a.
deyǫshę́thwas

de-

DUC-

yǫ-

3.SG.F-

ashęthw-as

cry-HAB

de- yǫ- ashęthw-as

DUC- 3.SG.F- cry-HAB

'She is crying'

b.
dehahahíyaʔks

de-

DUC-

ha-

3.SG.M-

ahah-

road-

iyaʔk-

cross-

s

HAB

de- ha- ahah- iyaʔk- s

DUC- 3.SG.M- road- cross- HAB

'He crosses a road.'

c.
dehá꞉yaʔks

de-

DUC-

ha-

3.SG.M-

yaʔk-

break-

s

HAB

de- ha- yaʔk- s

DUC- 3.SG.M- break- HAB

'He breaks it into two.'

d.
há꞉yaʔks

ha-

3.SG.M-

yaʔk-

break-

s

HAB

ha- yaʔk- s

3.SG.M- break- HAB

'He breaks it off.'

Pronominal prefixes

There are three series of pronominal prefixes in Onondaga. There is a transitive series, used with transitive verbs. Intransitive verbs use either the agent series or the patient series. The choice between the latter two is often complex, as we will see. The phonological shape of the pronominal prefix depends on the identity of the following sound. This gives rise to several series of pronominal prefixes, which are labelled according to the following segment. These include the c-series (for pronominal prefixes which precede a consonant), the a-series (for pronominal prefixes which precede /a/), the e-series, ę-series, o-series, ǫ-series, and i-series. Finally, the pronominal prefixes inflect for person, number and gender. We discuss each of these in turn.

Onondaga distinguishes three persons: first (I or we), second (you) and third (he, she, it or they). The first person can be either exclusive (EXCL), which excludes the listener, or inclusive (INCL), which includes the listener.

Here, when John says we, it does not include the person he's talking to. In other words, Mary doesn't get to go to the movies. This use of we is called the first person exclusive. English does not make a distinction between inclusive and exclusive we but Onondaga does. Consider the following two words. The pronominal prefix /dn-/ indicates a first person dual inclusive subject, and the pronominal prefix /agn-/ indicates a first person dual exclusive subject.

a.
weʔdnek

weʔ–

FACT-

dn–

1.DU.INCL-

ek–

eat-

Ø

PUNC

weʔ– dn– ek– Ø

FACT- 1.DU.INCL- eat- PUNC

'We two (you and I) ate it.'

b.
waʔagnek

waʔ–

FACT-

agn–

1.DU.EXCL-

ek–

eat-

Ø

PUNC

waʔ– agn– ek– Ø

FACT- 1.DU.EXCL- eat- PUNC

'We two (someone else and I) ate it.'

Three numbers are also distinguished in Onondaga: singular (for one entity, SG), dual (for two entities, DU), and plural (for three or more entities, PL). In the glosses, singular is marked with SG, dual is marked with DU, and plural is marked with PL.

a.
waʔsek

waʔ–

FACT-

s–

2.SG-

ek–

eat-

Ø

PUNC

waʔ– s– ek– Ø

FACT- 2.SG- eat- PUNC

'You (sing.) ate it.'

b.
weʔsnek

weʔ–

FACT-

sn–

2.DU-

ek–

eat-

Ø

PUNC

weʔ– sn– ek– Ø

FACT- 2.DU- eat- PUNC

'You two ate it.'

c.
weʔswek

weʔ–

FACT-

sw–

2.PL-

ek–

eat-

Ø

PUNC

weʔ– sw– ek– Ø

FACT- 2.PL- eat- PUNC

'You all ate it.'

Additionally, Onondaga distinguishes three genders, which are realized in the third person only. The first is masculine (M), which is used to refer to male humans and certain animals, either alone or in a group. The second is feminine (F). This is used to refer to female humans, certain animals, or some unknown person. It is sometimes called the feminine-indefinite. For groups of people that contain both men and women, the masculine is used. The third is neuter (N), which is used to refer to most animals and inanimate objects. In older texts, the neuter is used to refer to human females in certain circumstances, although this usage is no longer common. See Abrams (2006: 17) for more discussion. (Abbott, 1984 also discusses two feminine genders in Oneida.) Here are some examples.

a.
waʔek

waʔ-

FACT-

e-

3.SG.F-

k-

eat-

Ø

PUNC

waʔ- e- k- Ø

FACT- 3.SG.F- eat- PUNC

'She ate it.' OR 'Someone ate it.'

b.
waʔwek

waʔ-

FACT-

we-

3.SG.N-

k-

eat-

Ø

PUNC

waʔ- we- k- Ø

FACT- 3.SG.N- eat- PUNC

'Something/it ate it.'

c.
waʔhek

waʔ-

FACT-

he-

3.SG.M-

k-

eat-

Ø

PUNC

waʔ- he- k- Ø

FACT- 3.SG.M- eat- PUNC

'He ate it.'

Finally, we observe that there are two series of prefixes for intransitive verbs, the agent series (AG) and the patient series (PAT). As a general rule, verbs which involve active, purposeful movement or activity on the part of the subject are conjugated with the agent series. Verbs which involve involuntary action or states are conjugated with the patient series. There are so many exceptions to this generalization, however, that one has to simply learn for each intransitive verb whether it takes the agent or the patient series. There is an additional rule for the intransitive verbs that take the agent series. When these verbs appear with stative aspect, they use the patient series rather than the agent series. This rule does not have any exceptions.

There are six classes of conjugations which depend on the initial sound of the following morpheme (i.e., the first sound of the verb root or of the incorporated noun if there is one).

C-Stem

The following chart lists the pronominal prefixes for stems that begin with a consonant. Note that (y) or (w) in brackets disappears when preceded by a pre-pronominal prefix.

Intransitive, Agent-series
singulardualplural
1st personexclusivek-(y)agni-(y)agwa-
inclusivedni-dwa-
2nd persons-sni-swa-
3rd personmasculineha-hni-hadi-
feminine/
indefinite
(y)e-gni-gǫdi-
Intransitive, Patient-series
singulardualplural
1st person(w)ak-(y)ǫgni-(y)ǫgwa-
2nd personsa-sni-swa-
3rd personmasculineho-hodi-
feminine/
indefinite
((y)a)go-(y)odi-
Transitive Series
Patient (to right)
Agent (below)
1.SG1.DU1.PL2.SG2.DU2.PL3.NEUT3.SG.MASC3.FEM/INDEF.SG3.FEM.DU/PL3.MASC.DU/PL
1.SGgǫ-gni-gwa-he-khe-
1.DU.EXCLgni-gni-gwa-shagni-(y)akhni-
1.PL.EXCLgwa-gwa-gwa-shagwa-
1.DU.INCLshedni-(y)ethi-
1.PL.INCLshedwa-
2.SGsk-sgni-sgwa-hes-she-
2.DUsgni-sgni-sgwa-shesni-(y)etchi-
2.PLsgwa-sgwa-sgwa-sheswa-
3.SG.MASChak-shǫgni-shǫgwa-hya-shesni-sheswa-hǫwa-shago-
3.SG.FEM/INDEF(y)ǫk-(y)ǫkhi-(y)esa-(y)etchi-gǫwa-hǫwa-(y)ǫdat-gǫwadi-hǫwadi-
3.DU/PL.FEM((y)a)godi-
3.DU.PL.MASChǫk-shagodi-
A-Stem

The following chart lists the pronominal prefixes for stems that begin with a consonant. Note that (y) or (w) in brackets disappears when preceded by a pre-pronominal prefix.

Intransitive, Agent-series
singulardualplural
1st personexclusiveg-(y)agy-(y)agw-
inclusivedy-dw-
2nd person(h)s-jy-sw-
3rd personmasculinehǫhR-hy-hǫw-
feminine/
indefinite
yǫw-gy-gǫw-
Intransitive, Patient-series
singulardualplural
1st person(w)ag-(y)ǫgy-(y)ǫgw-
2nd persons-jy-sw-
3rd personmasculinehow-hon-
feminine/
indefinite
((y)a)gow-(y)on-
Transitive Series
Patient (to right)
Agent (below)
1.SG1.DU1.PL2.SG2.DU2.PL3.NEUT3.SG.MASC3.FEM/INDEF.SG3.FEM.DU/PL3.MASC.DU/PL
1.SGgǫy-gy-gw-hey-khey-
1.DU.EXCLgy-gy-gw-shagy-(y)akhiy-
1.PL.EXCLgw-gw-gw-shagw-
1.DU.INCLshedy-(y)ethy-
1.PL.INCLshedwa-
2.SGsg-sgy-sgw-hes-shey-
2.DUsgy-sgy-sgw-shejy-(y)etchiy-
2.PLsgw-sgw-sgw-shesw-
3.SG.MASChag-shǫgy-shǫgw-hy-(s)hejy-(s)hesw-hǫw-shagow-
3.SG.FEM/INDEF(y)ǫg-(y)ǫkhiy-(y)es-(y)etchiy-gǫw-hǫw-(y)ǫdad-gǫwan-hǫwadiy-
3.DU/PL.FEM((y)a)godiy-
3.DU.PL.MASChǫg-shagodi-
E-Stem

The following chart lists the pronominal prefixes for stems that begin with a consonant. Note that (y) or (w) in brackets disappears when preceded by a pre-pronominal prefix.

Intransitive, Agent-series
singulardualplural
1st personexclusiveg-(y)agn-(y)agw-
inclusivedn-dw-
2nd person(h)s-sn-sw-
3rd personmasculineh-hn-hęn-
feminine/
indefinite
yagǫ(y)-gn-gǫn-
Intransitive, Patient-series
singulardualplural
1st person(w)ag-(y)ǫgn-(y)ǫgw-
2nd persons-sn-sw-
3rd personmasculinehaw-hon-
feminine/
indefinite
((y)a)gaw-(y)on-
Transitive Series
Patient (to right)
Agent (below)
1.SG1.DU1.PL2.SG2.DU2.PL3.NEUT3.SG.MASC3.FEM/INDEF.SG3.FEM.DU/PL3.MASC.DU/PL
1.SGgǫy-gn-gw-hey-khey-
1.DU.EXCLgn-gn-gw-shagn-(y)akhiy-
1.PL.EXCLgw-gw-gw-shagw-
1.DU.INCLshedn-(y)ethy-
1.PL.INCLshedwa-
2.SGsg-sgn-sgw-hes-shey-
2.DUsgn-sgn-sgw-shejy-(y)etchiy-
2.PLsgw-sgw-sgw-shesw-
3.SG.MASChag-shǫgn-shǫgw-hy-(s)hesn-(s)hesw-hǫw-shagaw-
3.SG.FEM/INDEF(y)ǫg-(y)ǫkhiy-(y)es-(y)etchiy-gǫw-hǫw-(y)ǫdad-gǫwan-hǫwadiy-
3.DU/PL.FEM((y)a)godiy-
3.DU/PL.MASChǫg-shagodi-
Ę-Stem

The following chart lists the pronominal prefixes for stems that begin with /ę/. Note that (y) or (w) in brackets disappears when preceded by a pre-pronominal prefix.

Intransitive, Agent-series
singulardualplural
1st personexclusiveg-(y)agy-(y)agw-
inclusivedy-dw-
2nd person(h)s-jy-sw-
3rd personmasculinehǫhR-hy-hǫw-
feminine/
indefinite
yǫw/yag-gy-gǫn-
Intransitive, Patient-series
singulardualplural
1st person(w)ag-(y)ǫgy-(y)ǫgw-
2nd persons-jy-sw-
3rd personmasculinehow-hon-
feminine/
indefinite
((y)a)gow-(y)on-
Transitive Series
Patient (to right)
Agent (below)
1.SG1.DU1.PL2.SG2.DU2.PL3.NEUT3.SG.MASC3.FEM/INDEF.SG3.FEM.DU/PL3.MASC.DU/PL
1.SGgǫy-gy-gw-hey-khey-
1.DU.EXCLgy-gy-gw-shagy-(y)akhiy-
1.PL.EXCLgw-gw-gw-shagw-
1.DU.INCLshedy-(y)ethy-
1.PL.INCLshedwa-
2.SGsg-sgy-sgw-hes-shey-
2.DUsgy-sgy-sgw-shejy-(y)etchiy-
2.PLsgw-sgw-sgw-shesw-
3.SG.MASChag-shǫgy-shǫgw-hy-(s)hejy-(s)hesw-hǫw-shagow-
3.SG.FEM/INDEF(y)ǫg-(y)ǫkhiy-(y)es-(y)etchiy-gǫw-hǫw-(y)ǫdad-gǫwan-hǫwadiy-
3.DU/PL.FEM((y)a)godiy-
3.DU.PL.MASChǫg-shagodi-
O-Stem

The following chart lists the pronominal prefixes for stems that begin with a consonant. Note that (y) or (w) in brackets disappears when preceded by a pre-pronominal prefix.

Intransitive, Agent-series
singulardualplural
1st personexclusiveg-(y)agy-(y)agw-
inclusivedy-dw-
2nd person(h)s-jy-sw-
3rd personmasculinehǫhR-hy-hǫw-
feminine/
indefinite
yǫw-gy-gǫw-
Intransitive, Patient-series
singulardualplural
1st person(w)ag-(y)ǫgy-(y)ǫgw-
2nd persons-jy-sw-
3rd personmasculinehow-hon-
feminine/
indefinite
((y)a)gow-(y)on-
Transitive Series
Patient (to right)
Agent (below)
1.SG1.DU1.PL2.SG2.DU2.PL3.NEUT3.SG.MASC3.FEM/INDEF.SG3.FEM.DU/PL3.MASC.DU/PL
1.SGgǫy-gy-gw-hey-khey-
1.DU.EXCLgy-gy-gw-shagn-(y)akhiy-
1.PL.EXCLgw-gw-gw-shagy-
1.DU.INCLshedn-(y)ethiy-
1.PL.INCLshedy-
2.SGsg-sgn-sgy-hes-shey-
2.DUsgn-sgn-sgy-shesn-(y)etchiy-
2.PLsgy-sgy-sgy-shejy-
3.SG.MASChag-shǫgn-shǫgy-hyay-(s)hesn-(s)hejy-hǫy-shaga-
3.SG.FEM/INDEF(y)ǫg-(y)ǫkhiy-(y)es-(y)etchiy-gǫw-hǫw-(y)ǫdad-gǫwan-hǫwadiy-
3.DU/PL.FEM((y)a)godiy-
3.DU.PL.MASChǫg-shagodi-
Ǫ-Stem

The following chart lists the pronominal prefixes for stems that begin with a consonant. Note that (y) or (w) in brackets disappears when preceded by a pre-pronominal prefix.

Intransitive, Agent-series
singulardualplural
1st personexclusiveg-(y)agy-(y)agw-
inclusivedy-dw-
2nd person(h)s-jy-sw-
3rd personmasculinehǫhR-hy-hǫw-
feminine/
indefinite
yǫw-gy-gǫw-
Intransitive, Patient-series
singulardualplural
1st person(w)ag-(y)ǫgy-(y)ǫgw-
2nd persons-jy-sw-
3rd personmasculinehow-hon-
feminine/
indefinite
((y)a)gow-(y)on-
Transitive Series
Patient (to right)
Agent (below)
1.SG1.DU1.PL2.SG2.DU2.PL3.NEUT3.SG.MASC3.FEM/INDEF.SG3.FEM.DU/PL3.MASC.DU/PL
1.SGgǫy-gy-gw-hey-khey-
1.DU.EXCLgy-gy-gw-shagy-(y)akhiy-
1.PL.EXCLgw-gw-gw-shagw-
1.DU.INCLshedy-(y)ethy-
1.PL.INCLshedwa-
2.SGsg-sgy-sgw-hes-shey-
2.DUsgy-sgy-sgw-shejy-(y)etchiy-
2.PLsgw-sgw-sgw-shesw-
3.SG.MASChag-shǫgy-shǫgw-hy-(s)hejy-(s)hesw-hǫw-shagow-
3.SG.FEM/INDEF(y)ǫg-(y)ǫkhiy-(y)es-(y)etchiy-gǫw-hǫw-(y)ǫdad-gǫwan-hǫwadiy-
3.DU/PL.FEM((y)a)godiy-
3.DU.PL.MASChǫg-shagodi-
I-Stem

The following chart lists the pronominal prefixes for stems that begin with a consonant. Note that (y) or (w) in brackets disappears when preceded by a pre-pronominal prefix.

Intransitive, Agent-series
singulardualplural
1st personexclusiveg-(y)agy-(y)agw-
inclusivedy-dw-
2nd person(h)s-jy-sw-
3rd personmasculinehǫhR-hy-hǫw-
feminine/
indefinite
yǫw-gy-gǫw-
Intransitive, Patient-series
singulardualplural
1st person(w)ag-(y)ǫgy-(y)ǫgw-
2nd persons-jy-sw-
3rd personmasculinehow-hon-
feminine/
indefinite
((y)a)gow-(y)on-
Transitive Series
Patient (to right)
Agent (below)
1.SG1.DU1.PL2.SG2.DU2.PL3.NEUT3.SG.MASC3.FEM/INDEF.SG3.FEM.DU/PL3.MASC.DU/PL
1.SGgǫy-gy-gw-hey-khey-
1.DU.EXCLgy-gy-gw-shagy-(y)akhiy-
1.PL.EXCLgw-gw-gw-shagw-
1.DU.INCLshedy-(y)ethy-
1.PL.INCLshedwa-
2.SGsg-sgy-sgw-hes-shey-
2.DUsgy-sgy-sgw-shejy-(y)etchiy-
2.PLsgw-sgw-sgw-shesw-
3.SG.MASChag-shǫgy-shǫgw-hy-(s)hejy-(s)hesw-hǫw-shagow-
3.SG.FEM/INDEF(y)ǫg-(y)ǫkhiy-(y)es-(y)etchiy-gǫw-hǫw-(y)ǫdad-gǫwan-hǫwadiy-
3.DU/PL.FEM((y)a)godiy-
3.DU.PL.MASChǫg-shagodi-

Reflexive and semireflexive

This section discusses reflexive sentences such as "John saw himself," and "We like ourselves," and reciprocal sentences such as "We like each other." The basic form of the reflexive marker (REFL) is /atat-/ and it appears right after the pronominal prefix and before the incorporated noun, if any. The reflexive is typically found only on transitive verbs, but because there is only one participant in the event, we use the intransitive series of pronominal prefixes. Here's an example of a reflexive and a regular (non-reflexive) transitive for comparison.

a.
waʔgadadaehsę́thwaʔ

waʔ-

FACT-

k-

1.SG.AG-

atat-

REFL-

aehsęthw-

kick-

PUNC

waʔ- k- atat- aehsęthw- aʔ

FACT- 1.SG.AG- REFL- kick- PUNC

'I kicked myself.'

b.
waʔsgaehsę́thwaʔ

waʔ-

FACT-

sk-

2.SG.AG:1.SG.PAT-

aehsęthw-

kick-

PUNC

waʔ- sk- aehsęthw- aʔ

FACT- 2.SG.AG:1.SG.PAT- kick- PUNC

'You kicked me.'

In the non-reflexive transitive form there is a pronominal prefix, /sk-/ that indicates the subject ('you') and the object ('me'). In the reflexive form, there is only one participant in the act of kicking ('me'), so the intransitive form of the pronominal prefix is used, /k-/.

A reflexive action is something that you do to yourself. A reciprocal action is something people do to each other. Reciprocals must have at least two people involved. They are formed with the reflexive marker and the dualic prepronominal prefix discussed in section 3.4.1.1.2.4. Here is an example.

a.
waʔthyadadyǫ́dyahdęʔ

waʔ-

FACT-

t-

DUC-

hy-

3.DU.AG-

atat-

REFL-

yǫtya-

laugh-

ht-

CAUS-

ęʔ

PUNC

waʔ- t- hy- atat- yǫtya- ht- ęʔ

FACT- DUC- 3.DU.AG- REFL- laugh- CAUS- PUNC

'They two made each other laugh.'

The semireflexive (SRFL) appears in a variety of circumstances that are not easy to pin down. The usual form of the semireflexive is /at-/, but certain verb roots take different forms. Listed here are some of the more common situations in which the semireflexive is used.

First, when a person's own body part is the object of the action a semireflexive is normally used. Here is an example with and without a semireflexive. Again notice that the form with the semireflexive uses the intransitive pronominal prefix while the form without the semireflexive uses the transitive pronominal prefix.

a.
waʔgadnęntshagetsgwaʔ

waʔ-

FACT-

k-

1.SG.AG-

at-

SRFL-

nęntsh-

arm-

a-

JOIN-

getsgw-

raise-

PUNC

waʔ- k- at- nęntsh- a- getsgw- aʔ

FACT- 1.SG.AG- SRFL- arm- JOIN- raise- PUNC

'I raised my arm.' Unknown glossing abbreviation(s) (help);

b.
waʔkhenęntshagetsgwaʔ

waʔ-

FACT-

khe-

1.SG.AG:3.SG.F.PAT-

nęntsh-

arm-

a-

JOIN-

getsgw-

raise-

PUNC

waʔ- khe- nęntsh- a- getsgw- aʔ

FACT- 1.SG.AG:3.SG.F.PAT- arm- JOIN- raise- PUNC

'I raised her arm.' Unknown glossing abbreviation(s) (help);

Second, the semireflexive is used with verbs of grooming.

a.
John waʔhadyaʔdohaeʔ

John

John

waʔ-

FACT-

ha-

3.SG.M.AG-

at-

SRFL-

yaʔt-

body-

ohae-

wash-

ʔ

PUNC

John waʔ- ha- at- yaʔt- ohae- ʔ

John FACT- 3.SG.M.AG- SRFL- body- wash- PUNC

'John washed (himself).'

Finally, the semireflexive is used to describe events or actions that are internally caused or spontaneous.

a.
waʔwadehnhóhwak

waʔ-

FACT-

w-

3.SG.N.PAT-

at-

SRFL-

ehnhohw-

door-

aR-

apply-

k

PUNC

waʔ- w- at- ehnhohw- aR- k

FACT- 3.SG.N.PAT- SRFL- door- apply- PUNC

'The door closed.'

b.
odųhwejiyáʔgi꞉h

o-

3.SG.N.PAT-

at-

SRFL-

ųhwęjy-

earth-

yaʔk-

break-

ih

STAT

o- at- ųhwęjy- yaʔk- ih

3.SG.N.PAT- SRFL- earth- break- STAT

'The earth has caved in.'

c.
waʔdyųdetgwęʔdaʔnéꞏgaꞏʔ

waʔ-d-yų-adet-gwęʔd-ʔnegaꞏ-aʔ

FACT-change.of.state-she-self-belly-burst-at.one.point.in.time

waʔ-d-yų-adet-gwęʔd-ʔnegaꞏ-aʔ

FACT-change.of.state-she-self-belly-burst-at.one.point.in.time

"she blabbed, she revealed a secret" [literally, 'she burst her belly']

The constituent morphemes are separated by hyphens in the second line of the example. Each one is translated, as closely as possible, in the third line. It is important to understand that none of the component morphemes is a separate word, since they cannot be uttered, or understood, in isolation.

A second way in which linguists classify the morphology of languages is in terms of how the morphemes of a word combine. This distinction is between languages that are fusional and languages that are agglutinative. Fusion occurs in two ways: a single morpheme may have two or more functions (or meanings) in a given word or contiguous morphemes may affect each other's shape in such a way that it is difficult to segment the word into morphemes. A language is agglutinative if the morphemes composing a word each carries its own meaning and can be easily segmented from its neighbor. Onondaga is fusional (in the second sense of that term). Fusion is especially prevalent at the boundary between prefixes and the stem. Here certain phonological processes take place which change the shapes of one or both contiguous morphemes. For example:

a.
gędéꞏih

ga-idęꞏ-ih

N.AG-help.out-STAT

ga-idęꞏ-ih

N.AG-help.out-STAT

"it is helping."

Languages are also classified in terms of the preeminent morphological processes they manifest. In Onondaga the two major morphological processes are prefixing and suffixing. Prefixes and suffixes are bound morphemes, that is, they are morphemes that cannot occur in isolation. Onondaga verbs must, minimally, begin in a pronominal prefix and inflect for aspect. For example:

a.
hayę́thwas

ha-yę́thw-as

3.SG.M.AG-plant-HAB

ha-yę́thw-as

3.SG.M.AG-plant-HAB

"he plants"

Nouns must, minimally, begin in a nominal prefix and end in a noun suffix. For example:

a.
ganáʔjyaʔ

ga-naʔjy-aʔ

N.AG-pail-noun.suffix

ga-naʔjy-aʔ

N.AG-pail-noun.suffix

"pail"

Nominal Morphology

A basic noun is composed of the following three morphemes in this order: noun prefix, root, and noun suffix.

According to Woodbury (2003), nouns in Onondaga are categorized into two main classes. These are non-human and human. The inanimate nouns are further divided into natural and man-made. The prefix (NPRE) on the noun depends on these classes. For inanimate nouns, the prefix is either /o-/ for natural objects or /ka-/ for man-made objects. The noun forming suffix also depends on the noun class. It is /aʔ/ for non-human nouns and /-h/ for human nouns.

Noun incorporation

Noun incorporation is a process of compounding in which two stems, a noun and a verb stem, are combined into a new stem that is inflected with verbal morphology and that functions as a verb. Nouns occupying the semantic roles or thematic relations, of patient, theme, factitive theme, location, goal, path, or instrument are eligible for incorporation. Of these, it is nouns designating semantic patients and themes that are most frequently incorporated. Semantic agents, causers, and beneficiaries are not eligible for incorporation.

The first example shows that the nominal root -nęh- "corn" has been incorporated into the verbal complex forming a single word. The second example shows the noun /onęhaʔ/ ('corn') as a separate word and preceded by the particle neʔ, a particle which marks a following word or phrase as a nominal.

a.
waʔhanęhayę́thwaʔ

waʔ-ha-nęh-yęthw-aʔ

FACT-he-corn-plant-PUNC

waʔ-ha-nęh-yęthw-aʔ

FACT-he-corn-plant-PUNC

'he planted corn' [literally, 'he corn-planted'].

b.
waʔhayę́thwaʔ neʔ onę́haʔ

waʔ-ha-yęthw-aʔ

FACT-he-plant-PUNC

neʔ

nominal.particle

o-nęh-aʔ

it-corn-noun.suffix

waʔ-ha-yęthw-aʔ neʔ o-nęh-aʔ

FACT-he-plant-PUNC nominal.particle it-corn-noun.suffix

'he planted (the) corn'

Noun incorporation is a highly productive process in Onondaga. However, its productivity is an attribute of individual nouns and verbs. Every noun and every verb is lexically marked in terms of its incorporation characteristics.[ citation needed ] Some nouns incorporate frequently, that is, they can combine with many different verbs, others almost never. Among the verbs that can incorporate—and some do not incorporate at all—there is a continuum of productivity. At their most productive, verbs can incorporate one of any number of nouns, in fact, some verbs can only occur together with an incorporated noun. Verbs at their most unproductive can incorporate only a single noun. Between these extremes are additional types: verbs that can incorporate only a restricted set of nouns; verb and noun combinations that are highly idiomatic—these often denote conventionalized activities (e.g., English 'he information-gathered)—so that separating the noun, though interpretable, is perceived as inappropriate.

The use of noun incorporation is governed by various discourse factors. It is often used as a way of backgrounding information.[ citation needed ]

Word order

Word order is typically free in Onondaga (though see question formation below). It depends on various discourse factors.

Question formation

Wh-questions begin with the interrogative word:

a.
gaę nų́ tganųhsáꞏyęʔ

gaę

where

nų́

place

t-ga-nųhs-yę-ʔ

here-it-house-be.lying-STAT

gaę nų́ t-ga-nųhs-yę-ʔ

where place here-it-house-be.lying-STAT

"Where is the house?"

b.
wadę́ʔ nihsaꞏdyéꞏhaʔ

wadę́ʔ

what

ni-hs-adyéꞏ-haʔ

thus-you-do-HAB

wadę́ʔ ni-hs-adyéꞏ-haʔ

what thus-you-do-HAB

"What are you doing?"

Yes–no questions are formed by appending the question-particle to the questioned item:

a.
Sędáʔwih khę́h.

sa-idáʔw-ih

you-be.asleep-STAT

khę́h.

Q

sa-idáʔw-ih khę́h.

you-be.asleep-STAT Q

"Are you asleep?"

See also

Related Research Articles

Mam is a Mayan language spoken by about half a million Mam people in the Guatemalan departments of Quetzaltenango, Huehuetenango, San Marcos, and Retalhuleu, and the Mexican states of Campeche and Chiapas. Thousands more make up a Mam diaspora throughout the United States and Mexico, with notable populations living in Oakland, California and Washington, D.C. The most extensive Mam grammar is Nora C. England's A grammar of Mam, a Mayan language (1983), which is based on the San Ildefonso Ixtahuacán dialect of Huehuetenango Department.

Palauan is a Malayo-Polynesian language native to the Republic of Palau, where it is one of the two official languages, alongside English. It is widely used in day-to-day life in the country. Palauan is not closely related to other Malayo-Polynesian languages and its exact classification within the branch is unclear.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Greenlandic language</span> Inuit language spoken in Greenland

Greenlandic is an Eskimo–Aleut language with about 57,000 speakers, mostly Greenlandic Inuit in Greenland. It is closely related to the Inuit languages in Canada such as Inuktitut. It is the most widely spoken Eskimo–Aleut language. In June 2009, the government of Greenland, the Naalakkersuisut, made Greenlandic the sole official language of the autonomous territory, to strengthen it in the face of competition from the colonial language, Danish. The main variety is Kalaallisut, or West Greenlandic. The second variety is Tunumiit oraasiat, or East Greenlandic. The language of the Inughuit of Greenland, Inuktun or Polar Eskimo, is a recent arrival and a dialect of Inuktitut.

In linguistics, incorporation is a phenomenon by which a grammatical category, such as a verb, forms a compound with its direct object or adverbial modifier, while retaining its original syntactic function. The inclusion of a noun qualifies the verb, narrowing its scope rather than making reference to a specific entity.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Tuscarora language</span> Extinct language

Tuscarora, sometimes called Skarò˙rə̨ˀ, is the Iroquoian language of the Tuscarora people, spoken in southern Ontario, Canada, North Carolina and northwestern New York around Niagara Falls, in the United States before becoming extinct in late 2020. The historic homeland of the Tuscarora was in eastern North Carolina, in and around the Goldsboro, Kinston, and Smithfield areas.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Seneca language</span> Iroquoian language

Seneca is the language of the Seneca people, one of the Six Nations of the Hodinöhsö꞉niʼ ; it is an Iroquoian language, spoken at the time of contact in the western part of New York. While the name Seneca, attested as early as the seventeenth century, is of obscure origins, the endonym Onödowáʼga꞉ translates to "those of the big hill." About 10,000 Seneca live in the United States and Canada, primarily on reservations in western New York, with others living in Oklahoma and near Brantford, Ontario. As of 2022, an active language revitalization program is underway.

The Yimas language is spoken by the Yimas people, who populate the Sepik River Basin region of Papua New Guinea. It is spoken primarily in Yimas village, Karawari Rural LLG, East Sepik Province. It is a member of the Lower-Sepik language family. All 250-300 speakers of Yimas live in two villages along the lower reaches of the Arafundi River, which stems from a tributary of the Sepik River known as the Karawari River.

Pohnpeian is a Micronesian language spoken as the indigenous language of the island of Pohnpei in the Caroline Islands. Pohnpeian has approximately 30,000 (estimated) native speakers living in Pohnpei and its outlying atolls and islands with another 10,000-15,000 (estimated) living off island in parts of the US mainland, Hawaii and Guam. It is the second-most widely spoken native language of the Federated States of Micronesia the first being Chuukese.

Classical Kʼicheʼ was an ancestral form of today's Kʼicheʼ language, which was spoken in the highland regions of Guatemala around the time of the 16th-century Spanish conquest of Guatemala. Classical Kʼicheʼ has been preserved in a number of historical Mesoamerican documents, lineage histories, missionary texts, and dictionaries. Most famously, it is the language in which the renowned highland Maya mythological and historical narrative Popol Vuh is written. Another historical text of partly similar content is the Título de Totonicapán.

The Wuvulu-Aua language is an Austronesian language which is spoken on the Wuvulu and Aua Islands and in the Manus Province of Papua New Guinea.

Abui is a non-Austronesian language of the Alor Archipelago. It is spoken in the central part of Alor Island in Eastern Indonesia, East Nusa Tenggara (NTT) province by the Abui people. The native name in the Takalelang dialect is Abui tanga which literally translates as 'mountain language'.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Pashto grammar</span> Grammar of the Pashto language

Pashto is an S-O-V language with split ergativity. Adjectives come before nouns. Nouns and adjectives are inflected for gender (masc./fem.), number (sing./plur.), and case. The verb system is very intricate with the following tenses: Present; simple past; past progressive; present perfect; and past perfect. In any of the past tenses, Pashto is an ergative language; i.e., transitive verbs in any of the past tenses agree with the object of the sentence. The dialects show some non-standard grammatical features, some of which are archaisms or descendants of old forms.

Mehek is a Tama language spoken by about 6300 people in a somewhat mountainous area along the southern base of the Torricelli Mountains in northwestern Papua New Guinea. Mehek is spoken in six villages of Sandaun Province: Nuku, Yiminum, Mansuku, Yifkindu, Wilwil, and Kafle. Mehek is most closely related to Pahi, with 51% lexical similarity, and spoken approximately 20 kilometers to the southwest. Mehek is a fairly typical Papuan language, being verb-final, having a relatively simple phonology, and agglutinative morphology. There is very little published information about Mehek. The literacy rate in Tok Pisin, spoken by nearly everyone, is 50-75%. Mehek is not written, so there is no literacy in Mehek. Tok Pisin is primarily used in the schools, with 50% children attending. There is also a sign language used by the large number of deaf people in the Mehek community.

Ramarama, also known as Karo, is a Tupian language of Brazil.

Paresi is an Arawakan language spoken in Brazil. There are approximately 2000 Paresi people, and around 1800 speak the language. The Paresi live in the state of Mato Grosso, more specifically in nine indigenous territories: Rio Formoso, Utiariti, Estação Parecis, Estivadinho, Pareci, Juininha, Figueira, Ponte de Pedra, and Uirapuru. In terms of endangerment, it is not in immediate danger. It is used in many everyday domains, but there is a lack of transmission to younger generations, as well as an evident language shift to Portuguese. This is a result of Portuguese being used in education and healthcare, as well as the integration of Brazilian culture among the Paresi people, creating changes in their language and cultural practices.

Mekéns (Mekem), or Amniapé, is a nearly extinct Tupian language of the state of Rondônia, in the Amazon region of Brazil.

Tamashek or Tamasheq is a variety of Tuareg, a Berber macro-language widely spoken by nomadic tribes across North Africa in Algeria, Mali, Niger, and Burkina Faso. Tamasheq is one of the three main varieties of Tuareg, the others being Tamajaq and Tamahaq.

Neverver (Nevwervwer), also known as Lingarak, is an Oceanic language. Neverver is spoken in Malampa Province, in central Malekula, Vanuatu. The names of the villages on Malekula Island where Neverver is spoken are Lingarakh and Limap.

Matis is a language spoken by the indigenous Matis people in the state of Amazonas in Brazil.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Tsʼixa language</span> Khoe language of Botswana

Tsʼixa is a critically endangered African language that belongs to the Kalahari Khoe branch of the Khoe-Kwadi language family. The Tsʼixa speech community consists of approximately 200 speakers who live in Botswana on the eastern edge of the Okavango Delta, in the small village of Mababe. They are a foraging society that consists of the ethnically diverse groups commonly subsumed under the names "San", "Bushmen" or "Basarwa". The most common term of self-reference within the community is Xuukhoe or 'people left behind', a rather broad ethnonym roughly equaling San, which is also used by Khwe-speakers in Botswana. Although the affiliation of Tsʼixa within the Khalari Khoe branch, as well as the genetic classification of the Khoisan languages in general, is still unclear, the Khoisan language scholar Tom Güldemann posits in a 2014 article the following genealogical relationships within Khoe-Kwadi, and argues for the status of Tsʼixa as a language in its own right. The language tree to the right presents a possible classification of Tsʼixa within Khoe-Kwadi:

References

  1. 1 2 Onondaga at Ethnologue (25th ed., 2022) Closed Access logo transparent.svg
  2. 1 2 Moses, Sarah (September 13, 2015). "Onondaga Nation's urgent mission: Saving our language". Syracuse.com. Retrieved 2015-10-03.
  3. "UNESCO Atlas of the World's Languages in danger". www.unesco.org. Retrieved 2020-12-08.
  4. "Six Nations of the Grand River | The Canadian Encyclopedia". www.thecanadianencyclopedia.ca. Retrieved 2020-12-08.
  5. 1 2 "Gawęnahwishe' Onǫda'gega'". Gawęnahwishe' Onǫda'gega'. Retrieved 2020-12-08.