Uwa | |
---|---|
Tunebo | |
Uw Cuwa | |
Native to | Colombia, formerly in Venezuela |
Region | the largest groups live on the northern slopes of the Sierra Nevada del Cocuy, Boyacá Department |
Ethnicity | U'wa |
Native speakers | 3,550 (2000) [1] |
Chibchan
| |
Language codes | |
ISO 639-3 | Variously: tnd – Angosturas Tunebo/Bahiyakuwa tbn – Barro Negro Tunebo (Eastern Tunebo/Yithkaya) tuf – Central Tunebo (Cobaría/Kubaru'wa & Tegría/Tagrinuwa) tnb – Western Tunebo (Aguas Blancas/Rikuwa) |
Glottolog | tune1260 |
ELP | Tunebo |
The Uwa language, Uw Cuwa, commonly known as Tunebo, is a Chibchan language spoken by between 1,800 and 3,600 of the Uwa people of Colombia, out of a total population of about 7,000. [2]
There are half a dozen known varieties. Communication between modern varieties can be difficult, so they are considered distinct languages.
Adelaar (2004) lists the living
Umaña (2012) lists Cobaría, Tegría, Agua Blanca, Barro Negro.[ needs to be confirmed with footnote in original ]
Berich lists the dialects Cobaría; Agua Blanca (= Uncasía, Tamarana, Sta Marta); Rinconada, Tegría, Bócota, & Báchira
Cassani lists Sínsiga, Tegría, Unkasía (= Margua), Pedraza, Manare, Dobokubí (= Motilón)
Osborn (1989) lists
the latter all extinct
Fabre (2005) lists:
Additional names in Loukotka are Manare and Uncasica (presumably a spelling variant of Unkasía/Uncacía), as well as Morcote, of which nothing is known. Manare, at the source of the Casanare, is Eastern Tunebo.
Front | Back | |
---|---|---|
High | i | u |
Mid | e | o |
Low | a |
Labial | Alveolar | Palatal | Velar | Labio-velar | Glottal | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Nasal | m | n | ||||
Stop | b | t | k | kʷ | ʔ | |
Fricative | s | ʃ | h | |||
Vibrant | r | |||||
Oral semi-vowel | w | j | ||||
Nasal semi-vowel | w̃ |
Uwa is an ergative–absolutive language with an SOV word order.
All isolated verbs end in -a. Nouns can be divided into three groups: personal nouns, verbal nouns, and other. The plurality of a referent is not explicitly marked on a verb; however, it is possible to mark a group of human referents using the -in suffix. Some kinship terms use a different term instead of using the -in suffix (e.g., wacjá ‘son’; sasa ‘sons’). Verbal nouns are derived from verbs by appending -quib (refers to one actor, e.g., yew̃quib ‘he who carries’), -quin (refers to multiple actors, e.g., yew̃quin ‘those who carry’) or -quey (the action, e.g., raquey ‘the coming’, OR the patient of an action, e.g., rojoquey ‘that which is brought’). Other nouns cannot be affixed with the aforementioned suffixes.
There are four case suffixes: ergative, absolutive, genitive and vocative.
The subject of a transitive clause, i.e., the ergative case, is marked with the -at suffix:
Bónit-at
mouse-ERG
eb
corn
yá-ca-ro.
eat-PRES-DECL
A mouse is eating the corn.
The subject of an intransitive clause or the object of a transitive clause, i.e., the absolutive case, takes the null suffix -∅:
As
I
isura
down
be-n-ro.
go-INTEN-DECL.
I'm going down[stairs].
Ow-at
suitcase-ERG
as
my
cuá-ca-ro.
tire-PRES-DECL
The suitcase is killing me.
The owner of a referent is marked with the genitive case using the -ay suffix. It can replace the ergative marker -at in the 1st person singular pronoun of transitive sentences.
Eb
corn
quehrós
parrot
yay
eat
ay-ti
leave-NEG
quehw̃u-wa.
frighten-IMP
Is-ay-an
our-GEN-EMP
bar
already
ri-jac-cua-no.
plant-PASS-USIT-DECL.
Shoo away the parrots so that they don't eat the corn. Ours is already planted.
The vocative suffix -u is used to identify a referent being addressed:
Wanis-u
Wanisa-VOC
Wanisa!
The personal pronouns distinguish between the 1st, 2nd and 3rd person (which is further divided into proximal and distal), as well as between the singular and plural:
singular | plural | ||
---|---|---|---|
1st person | asa, as | isa, is | |
2nd person | baha, bah | baa | |
3rd person | proximal | uiya | uiyina |
distal | eya, ey | eyina, eyin |
It is possible to use personal pronouns as possessive pronouns by placing them before the relevant noun (for comparison—adjectives are placed after the noun). However, there exist distinct forms of possessives, which will be discussed later.
The demonstrative pronouns make a two-way distinction: ucha (proximal, ‘this’) and eya (distal, ‘that’).
Additionally, there exists an intensifier-reflexive pronoun that is analogous to the English ‘oneself’ or ‘alone’. The pronoun itself is subject to inflection:
singular | plural | |
---|---|---|
1st person | ajmar, amar | ijmár |
2nd person | behmar | bemar |
3rd person | ima | imar |
The possessive pronouns in Uwa, just like the personal pronouns, make a proximal-distal distinction in the 3rd person. These are:
singular | plural | ||
---|---|---|---|
1st person | ajáy | isay | |
2nd person | bahay | bay | |
3rd person | proximal | uiyay | uiyinay |
distal | eyay | eyinay |
The Uwa language uses a base-10 (decimal) number system.
In general, adjectives are placed after the noun, although there are instances where they can be placed before it. Nevertheless, the majority of the time, adjectives are utilized in the verbal form:
cúmac-ro
big-DECL
It is big.
Carson
pants
bacsoy
black
racat-ro.
want-DECL
I want black pants.
In noun phrases, the adjective tends to take the -a suffix. [3]
Verbs in Uwa language can be divided into the following categories: transitive, intransitive, bitransitive, impersonal, objective clause, auxiliary and copular.
Asa
I
rícara
day
wiqui-n-ro
return-INTEN-DECL
I will return by day.
Aj-át
I-ERG
bajít
machete
wá-ro-ra.
buy-DECL-CNTR
I bought a machete.
Babcar
Babcara
ac
DEST
cuncuari
calabash
bacáy
leave
bi-ca-ro.
go-PRES-DECL
I'm going to leave the calabash for Babcara.
As
I
bar
already
seo-jac-ro.
tire-PST-DECL
I'm tired [of it]. / I've had enough.
“As-ra
[I-CONT
bar
already
be-n-ro,”
go-INTEN-DECL]
wa-no-ra.
say-DECL-CONT
I said, “I'm leaving [now].”
The verb séhlw̃anro is most frequently preceded by the intentional form of the verb without the declarative suffix:
As
I
ucasi
ten
ac
DEST
be-n
go-INTEN
séhlw̃a-ro.
think-DECL
I plan on going for ten days.
Cuc
thirst
wini
give
reh-ca-ro.
be-PRES-DECL
[He] is thirsty.
Cue
sad
reh-ti
be-NEG
ja-w̃i.
AUX-IMP
Don't be sad.
There are a number of different affixes that can appended to the verb.
The intentional suffixes -in and -n indicate the intention to be fulfilled by the action of the verb. The action occurs in the future.
Bacat
four
ubach
house
tuw̃-in-ro.
clean-INTEN-DECL
In four days I'm going to do the house.
Negation can be marked three ways. Future, ability or obligation, and stative verbs are negated with the word bár. Inability or impossibility is marked with -ajar/-ajat in the main verb and with an interrogative word in the same clause, plus an -i suffix on the focused word of focus. The -ti suffix is used on the main verb:
Ahajira
still
bahnaqu-i
everything-EMP.NEG
sín-ti-ro.
learn-NEG-DECL
I have not yet learned everything.
Ability or obligation is indicated in the verb by the suffix -ata. It indicates that something can or must be done. It can also function as a way to express command without using the imperative.
The inability is indicated by appending the suffix -ajar:
Is
we
oya
clothing
bár
not
bin-at-ra
who-ERG-CONT
ay
good
éy-in-ra
3-COL-CONT
yéhw̃-ajar-cua-no.
marry-IMPOSS-USIT-DECL
We poor people cannot marry the good ones.
Four tenses can be distinguished: present, past, immediate past and immediate future.
Tense | Suffix |
---|---|
past | -jac/-jec/-joc |
immediate past | -ira/-iri |
present | -ca [lower-alpha 1] |
immediate future | -ayquira |
The suffixes -ca/-qui and -ya/-yi are used to mark questions in the present and past tenses, respectively.
Adverbs are positioned immediately following the verb:
In
fast
ra-w̃i.
come-IMP
Come quick.
Mam is a Mayan language spoken by about half a million Mam people in the Guatemalan departments of Quetzaltenango, Huehuetenango, San Marcos, and Retalhuleu, and the Mexican states of Campeche and Chiapas. Thousands more make up a Mam diaspora throughout the United States and Mexico, with notable populations living in Oakland, California and Washington, D.C. The most extensive Mam grammar is Nora C. England's A grammar of Mam, a Mayan language (1983), which is based on the San Ildefonso Ixtahuacán dialect of Huehuetenango Department.
Hurrian is an extinct Hurro-Urartian language spoken by the Hurrians (Khurrites), a people who entered northern Mesopotamia around 2300 BC and had mostly vanished by 1000 BC. Hurrian was the language of the Mitanni kingdom in northern Mesopotamia and was likely spoken at least initially in Hurrian settlements in modern-day Syria.
Greenlandic is an Eskimo–Aleut language with about 57,000 speakers, mostly Greenlandic Inuit in Greenland. It is closely related to the Inuit languages in Canada such as Inuktitut. It is the most widely spoken Eskimo–Aleut language. In June 2009, the government of Greenland, the Naalakkersuisut, made Greenlandic the sole official language of the autonomous territory, to strengthen it in the face of competition from the colonial language, Danish. The main variety is Kalaallisut, or West Greenlandic. The second variety is Tunumiit oraasiat, or East Greenlandic. The language of the Inughuit of Greenland, Inuktun or Polar Eskimo, is a recent arrival and a dialect of Inuktitut.
Karitiana, otherwise known as Caritiana or Yjxa, is a Tupian language spoken in the State of Rondônia, Brazil, by 210 out of 320 Karitiana people, or 400 according to Cláudio Karitiana, in the Karitiana reserve 95 kilometres south of Porto Velho. The language belongs to the Arikém language family from the Tupi stock. It is the only surviving language in the family after the other two members, Kabixiâna and Arikém, became extinct.
Kanoê or Kapishana is a nearly extinct language isolate of Rondônia, Brazil. The Kapishana people now speak Portuguese or other indigenous languages from intermarriage.
Cavineña is an indigenous language spoken on the Amazonian plains of northern Bolivia by over 1,000 Cavineño people. Although Cavineña is still spoken, it is an endangered language. Guillaume (2004) states that about 1200 people speak the language, out of a population of around 1700. Nearly all Cavineña are bilingual in Spanish.
Adang is a Papuan language spoken on the island of Alor in Indonesia. The language is agglutinative. The Hamap dialect is sometimes treated as a separate language; on the other hand, Kabola, which is sociolinguistically distinct, is sometimes included. Adang, Hamap, and Kabola are considered a dialect chain. Adang is endangered as fewer speakers raise their children in Adang, instead opting for Indonesian.
Pashto is an S-O-V language with split ergativity. Adjectives come before nouns. Nouns and adjectives are inflected for gender (masc./fem.), number (sing./plur.), and case. The verb system is very intricate with the following tenses: Present; simple past; past progressive; present perfect; and past perfect. In any of the past tenses, Pashto is an ergative language; i.e., transitive verbs in any of the past tenses agree with the object of the sentence. The dialects show some non-standard grammatical features, some of which are archaisms or descendants of old forms.
Uyghur is a Turkic language spoken mostly in the west of China.
Mamaindê, also known as Northern Nambikwara, is a Nambikwaran language spoken in the Mato Grosso state of Brazil, in the very north of the indigenous reserve, Terra Indígena Vale do Guaporé, between the Pardo and Cabixi Rivers. In the southern part of the reserve, speakers of Sabanê and Southern Nambikwara are found.
Yolmo (Hyolmo) or Helambu Sherpa, is a Tibeto-Burman language of the Hyolmo people of Nepal. Yolmo is spoken predominantly in the Helambu and Melamchi valleys in northern Nuwakot District and northwestern Sindhupalchowk District. Dialects are also spoken by smaller populations in Lamjung District and Ilam District and also in Ramecchap District. It is very similar to Kyirong Tibetan and less similar to Standard Tibetan and Sherpa. There are approximately 10,000 Yolmo speakers, although some dialects have larger populations than others.
Aramba (Arammba), also known as Serki or Serkisetavi, is a Papuan language of Papua New Guinea. It is spoken to the south of Western Province in the Trans Fly region. Aramba belongs to the Tonda Sub-Family, which is next to the Nambu Sub-Family region and the Suki language. Alternative names for the language include Upper Morehead, Rouku, Kamindjo and Tjokwasi.
Mẽbêngôkre, sometimes referred to as Kayapó is a Northern Jê language spoken by the Kayapó and the Xikrin people in the north of Mato Grosso and Pará in Brazil. There are around 8,600 native speakers since 2010 based on the 2015 Ethnologue 18th edition. Due to the number of speakers and the influence of Portuguese speakers, the language stands at a sixth level of endangerment; in which the materials for literacy and education in Mẽbêngôkre are very limited.
Kĩsêdjê is a Northern Jê language spoken in Mato Grosso, Brazil. It is closely related to Tapayúna; together, they form the Tapajós branch of Northern Jê.
Mavea is an Oceanic language spoken on Mavea Island in Vanuatu, off the eastern coast of Espiritu Santo. It belongs to the North–Central Vanuatu linkage of Southern Oceanic. The total population of the island is approximately 172, with only 34 fluent speakers of the Mavea language reported in 2008.
Longgu (Logu) is a Southeast Solomonic language of Guadalcanal, but originally from Malaita.
Neverver (Nevwervwer), also known as Lingarak, is an Oceanic language. Neverver is spoken in Malampa Province, in central Malekula, Vanuatu. The names of the villages on Malekula Island where Neverver is spoken are Lingarakh and Limap.
The grammar of the constructed Naʼvi language was created for the movie Avatar by Paul Frommer. It is a tripartite, primarily affixing agglutinative language.
Matis is a language spoken by the indigenous Matis people in the state of Amazonas in Brazil.
The Hachijō language shares much of its grammar with its sister language of Japanese—having both descended from varieties of Old Japanese—as well as with its more distant relatives in the Ryukyuan language family. However, Hachijō grammar includes a substantial number of distinguishing features from modern Standard Japanese, both innovative and archaic.