Cultural-historical activity theory

Last updated

Cultural-historical activity theory (CHAT) is a theoretical framework [1] to conceptualize and analyse the relationship between cognition (what people think and feel) and activity (what people do). [2] [3] [4] The theory was founded by L. S. Vygotsky [5] and Aleksei N. Leontiev, who were part of the cultural-historical school of Russian psychology. [6] The Soviet philosopher of psychology, S.L. Rubinshtein, developed his own variant of activity as a philosophical and psychological theory, independent from Vygotsky's work. [7] Political restrictions in Stalinist Russia had suppressed the cultural-historical psychology – also known as the Vygotsky School – in the mid-thirties. This meant that the core "activity" concept remained confined to the field of psychology. Vygotsky's insight into the dynamics of consciousness was that it is essentially subjective and shaped by the history of each individual's social and cultural experiences. [8] Since the 1990s, CHAT has attracted a growing interest among academics worldwide. [3] Elsewhere CHAT has been described as "a cross-disciplinary framework for studying how humans transform natural and social reality, including themselves, as an ongoing culturally and historically situated, materially and socially mediated process". CHAT explicitly incorporates the mediation of activities by society, which means that it can be used to link concerns normally independently examined by sociologists of education and (social) psychologists. [4] [9] Core ideas are: 1) humans act collectively, learn by doing, and communicate in and via actions; 2) humans make, employ, and adapt tools to learn and communicate; and 3) community is central to the process of making and interpreting meaning – and thus to all forms of learning, communicating, and acting. [10] [3]

Contents

The term CHAT was coined by Michael Cole [11] and popularized by Yrjö Engeström  [ fi ] [12] to promote the unity of what, by the 1990s, had become a variety of currents harking back to Vygotsky's work. [13] [14] Prominent among those currents are Cultural-historical psychology, in use since the 1930s, and Activity theory in use since the 1960s.

Historical overview

Origins: revolutionary Russia

CHAT traces its lineage to dialectical materialism, classical German philosophy, [15] [16] and the work of Lev Vygotsky, Aleksei N. Leontiev and Aleksandr Luria, known as "the founding troika" [17] of the cultural-historical approach to Social Psychology. In particular Goethe's romantic science ideas which were later taken up by Hegel. The conceptual meaning of "activity" is rooted in the German word Tätigkeit. Hegel is considered the first philosopher to point out that the development of humans' knowledge is not spiritually given, but developed in history from living and working in natural environments. In a radical departure from the behaviorism and reflexology that dominated much of psychology in the early 1920s, they formulated, in the spirit of Karl Marx's Theses on Feuerbach, the concept of activity, i.e., "artifact-mediated and object-oriented action". [8] By bringing together the notion of history and culture in the understanding of human activity, they were able to transcend the Cartesian dualism between subject and object, internal and external, between people and society, between individual inner consciousness and the outer world of society. At the beginning of and into the mid-20th century, psychology was dominated by schools of thought that ignored real-life processes in psychological functioning (e.g. Gestalt psychology, Behaviorism and Cognitivism (psychology)). Lev Vygotsky, who developed the foundation of cultural-historical psychology based on the concept of mediation, published six books on psychology topics during a working life which spanned only ten years. He died of tuberculosis in 1934 at the age of 37. A.N. Leont'ev worked with Lev Vygotsky and Alexandr Luria from 1924 to 1930, collaborating on the development of a Marxist psychology. Leontiev left Vygotsky's group in Moscow in 1931, to take up a position in Kharkov. There he was joined by local psychologists, including Pyotr Galperin and Pyotr Zinchenko. [18] He continued to work with Vygotsky for some time but, eventually, there was a split, although they continued to communicate with one another on scientific matters. [19] Leontiev returned to Moscow in 1934. Contrary to popular belief, Vygotsky's work was never banned in Stalinist Soviet Russia. [20] In 1950 A.N. Leontiev became the Head of the Psychology Department at the Faculty of Philosophy of the Lomonosov Moscow State University (MGU). This department became an independent Faculty in 1966. He remained there until his death in 1979. Leontiev's formulation of activity theory, post 1962, had become the new "official" basis for Soviet psychology. [21] In the two decades between a thaw in the suppression of scientific enquiry in Russia and the death of the Vygotsky's continuers, contact was made with the West.

Developments in the West

Michael Cole, a psychology post-graduate exchange student, arrived in Moscow in 1962 for a one-year stint of research under Alexandr Luria. He was one for the first Westerners to present Luria's and Vygotsky's ideas to an Anglo-Saxon public. [22] This, and a steady flow of books translated from Russian ensured the gradual establishment of a Cultural Psychology base in the west. [4] The earliest books translated into English were Lev Vygotsky's "Thought and Language" (1962), Luria's "Cognitive Development" (1976), Leontiev's Activity, Consciousness, and Personality (1978) and Wertsch's "The Concept of Activity in Soviet Psychology" (1981). Principal among the groups promoting CHAT-related research was Yjrö Engeström's Helsinki-based CRADLE. [23] In 1982, Yrjö Engeström organized an Activity Conference to concentrate on teaching and learning issues. This was followed by the Aarhus (Dk) Conference in 1983 and the Utrecht (Nl) conference in 1984. In October 1986, West Berlin's College of Arts hosted the first ISCAR International Congress on Activity Theory. [24] The second ISCRAT congress took place in 1990. In 1992, ISCRAT became a formal legal organization with its own by-laws in Amsterdam. Other ISCRAT conferences: Rome (1993), Moscow (1995), Aarhus (1998) and Amsterdam (2002), when ISCRAT and the Conference for Socio-Cultural Research merged into ISCAR. From here on, ISCAR organizes an international Congress every three years: Sevilla (Es) 2005; San Diego (USA) 2008; Rome (It) 2011; Sydney (Au) 2014; Quebec, Canada (2017).

In recent years, the implications of activity theory in organizational development have been the focus of researchers at the Centre for Activity Theory and Developmental Work Research (CATDWR), now known as CRADLE, at the University of Helsinki, as well as Mike Cole at the Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition (LCHC) at the University of California San Diego. [25]

Three generations of activity theory

Diverse philosophical and psychological sources inform activity theory. [12] In subsequent years, a simplified picture emerged, namely the idea that there are three principal 'stages' or 'generations' of activity theory, or "cultural-historical activity theory (CHAT). [26] [27] [28] [7] 'Generations' do not imply a 'better-worse' value judgment. Each generation illustrates a different aspect. Whilst the first generation built on Vygotsky's notion of mediated action from the individual's perspective, the second generation built on Leont'ev's notion of activity system, with emphasis on the collective. The third generation, which appeared in the mid-nineties, builds on the idea of multiple interacting activity systems focused on a partially shared object, with boundary-crossings between them. [29] [30] [27] An activity system is a collective in which one or more human actors engage in activity to cyclically transform an object (a raw material or problem) to repeatedly achieve a desired result. [31]

First generation – Vygotsky

The first generation emerges from Vygotsky's theory of cultural mediation, which was a response to behaviorism's explanation of consciousness, or the development of the human mind, by reducing the human "mind" to atomic components or structures associated with "stimulus – response" (S-R) processes. Vygotsky argued that the relationship between a human subject and an object is never direct but must be sought in society and culture because they evolve historically, rather than evolving in the human brain or individual mind unto itself. Vygotsky saw the past and present as fused within the individual, that the "present is seen in the light of history." [8] His cultural-historical psychology attempted to account for the social origins of language and thinking. To Vygotsky, consciousness emerges from human activity mediated by artifacts (tools) and signs. [8] These artifacts, which can be physical tools such as hammers, ovens, or computers; cultural artifacts, including language; or theoretical artifacts, like algebra or feminist theory, are created and/or transformed in the course of activity, which, in the first generation framework, happens at the individual level. [8]

First Generation CHAT Vygotsky Basic Mediated Action.jpg
First Generation CHAT

Semiotic mediation [32] is embodied in Vygotsky's triangular model [8] which features the subject (S), object (O), and mediating artifact. [33] Vygotsky's triangular representation of mediated action attempts to explain human consciousness development in a manner that did not rely on dualistic stimulus–response (S-R) associations. In mediated action the subject, object, and artifact stand in dialectical relationship whereby each affects the other and the activity as a whole. [34] [13] [8] Vygotsky argued that the use of signs leads to a specific structure of human behavior, which allows the creation of new forms of culturally-based psychological processes – hence the importance of a cultural-historical context. Individuals could no longer be understood without their cultural environment, nor society without the agency of the individuals who use and produce these artifacts. The objects became cultural entities, and action that was oriented towards the objects became key to understanding the human psyche. In the Vygotskyan framework, the unit of analysis is the individual. First-generation activity theory has been used to understand individual behavior by examining the ways in which a person's objectivized actions are culturally mediated. Mediation is a key theoretical idea behind activity: People don't simply use tools and symbol systems; instead, everyday lived experiences are significantly mediated and intermediated by use of tools and symbols systems. Therefore, activity theory helps frame our understanding of such mediation. There is a strong focus on material and symbolic mediation, as well as internalization of external (social, societal, and cultural) forms of mediation. In Vygotskyan psychology, internalization is a theoretical concept that explains how individuals process what they learned through mediated action in the development of individual consciousness. [16] Another important aspect of first generation CHAT is the concept of the zone of proximal development (ZPD) [8] or "the distance between the actual developmental level as determined by independent problem solving and the level of potential development as determined through problem solving under adult guidance or in collaboration with more capable peers". [8] ZPD is the theoretical range of what a performer can do with competent peers and assistance, as compared with what can be accomplished on one's own. [35]

Second generation – Leontiev

While Vygotsky formulated practical human activity as the general explanatory category in human psychology, he did not fully clarify its nature. A.N. Leontiev developed the second generation of activity theory, which is a collective model. In Engeström's [12] depiction of second-generation activity, the unit of analysis includes collective motivated activity toward an object, making room for understanding how collective action by social groups mediates activity. Leontiev theorized that activity resulted from the confluence of a human subject, the object of their activity as "the target or content of a thought or action" [36] and the tools (including symbol systems) that mediate the object(ive). He saw activity as tripartite in structure, being composed of unconscious operations on/with tools, conscious but finite actions which are goal-directed, and higher level activities which are object-oriented and driven by motives. Hence, second generation activity theory included community, rules, division of labor and the importance of analyzing their interactions with each other. Rules may be explicit or implicit. Division of labor refers to the explicit and implicit organization of the community involved in the activity. Engeström [12] described Vygotskian psychology as emphasizing the way semiotic and cultural systems mediate human action, whereas Leontiev's second-generation CHAT focused on the mediational effects of the systemic organization of human activity.

Second Generation CHAT Second Generation CHAT.jpg
Second Generation CHAT

In conceptualizing activity as only existing in relation to rules, community and division of labor, Engeström expanded the unit of analysis for studying human behavior from individual activity to a collective activity system. While the unit of analysis, for Vygotsky, is "individual activity" and, for Leontiev, the "collective activity system", for Jean Lave and others working around situated cognition the unit of analysis is "practice", "community of practice", and "participation". Other scholars analyze "the relationships between the individual's psychological development and the development of social systems". [37] [38] The activity system includes the social, psychological, cultural and institutional perspectives in the analysis. In this conceptualization, context or activity systems are inherently related to what Engeström argues are the deep-seated material practices and socioeconomic structures of a given culture. These societal dimensions had not been taken sufficiently into account by Vygotsky's, earlier triadic model. [39] [40] [41] In Leontiev's understanding, thought and cognition were understood as a part of social life – as a part of the means of production and systems of social relations on one hand, and the intentions of individuals in certain social conditions on the other. [10] In the second generation diagram, activity is positioned in the middle, mediation at the top, adding rules, community and division of labor at the bottom. The minimum components of an activity system are: the subject; the object; outcome; mediating instruments/tools/artifacts; rules and signs; community and division of labor.

In his example of the 'primeval collective hunt', [42] Leontiev clarifies the difference between an individual action ("the beater frightening game") and a collective activity ("the hunt"). While individuals' actions (frightening game) are different from the overall goal of the activity (hunt), they share the same motive (obtaining food). Operations, on the other hand, are driven by the conditions and tools at hand, i.e. the objective circumstances under which the hunt is taking place. To understand the separate actions of the individuals, one needs to understand the broader motive behind the activity as a whole. This accounts for the three hierarchical levels of human functioning: [12] [1] [4] [39] [43] object-related motives drive the collective activity (top); goals drive individual/group action(s) (middle); conditions and tools drive automated operations (lower level). [42] [17] [26] [35]

Third generation – Engeström et al.

After Vygotsky's foundational work on individuals' higher psychological functions [8] and Leontiev's extension of these insights to collective activity systems, [16] questions of diversity and dialogue between different traditions or perspectives became increasingly serious challenges. The work of Michael Cole and Yrjö Engeström in the 1970s and 1980s brought activity theory to a much wider audience of scholars in Scandinavia and North America. [3] Once the lives and biographies of all the participants and the history of the wider community are taken into account, multiple activity systems needed to be considered, positing, according to Engeström, the need for a "third generation" to "develop conceptual tools to understand dialogue, multiple perspectives, and networks of interacting activity Systems". [33] This larger canvas of active individuals (and researchers) embedded in organizational, political, and discursive practices constitutes a tangible advantage of second- and third-generation CHAT over its earlier Vygotskian ancestor, which focused on mediated action in relative isolation. [4] Third-generation activity theory is the application of Activity Systems Analysis (ASA) [28] in developmental research where investigators take a participatory and interventionist role in the participants' activities and change their experiences.

Third-generation CHAT Third Generation CHAT.jpg
Third-generation CHAT

Engeström's basic activity triangle [12] (which adds rules/norms, intersubjective community relations, and division of labor, as well as multiple activity systems sharing an object) has become the principal third-generation model for analysing individuals and groups. [17] Engeström summarizes the current state of CHAT with five principles:

  1. The activity system as primary unit of analysis: the basic third-generation model includes minimally two interacting activity systems.
  2. Multi-voicedness: an activity system is always a community of multiple points of views, traditions and interests.
  3. Historicity: activity systems take shape and get transformed over long stretches of time. Potentials and problems can only be understood against the background of their own histories.
  4. The central role of contradictions as sources of change and development. [33]
  5. Activity systems' possibility for expansive transformation (cycles of qualitative transformation): when object and motive are reconceptualized a radically wider horizon opens up. [33]

Learning technologists have used third-generation CHAT as a guiding theoretical framework to understand how technologies are adopted, adapted, and configured through use in complex social situations. [44] Engeström has acknowledged that the third-generation model was limited to analysing 'reasonably well-bounded' systems and that in view of new, often web-based participatory practices a Fourth generation was needed.

Informing research and practice

Leontiev and social development

From the 1960s onwards, starting in the global South, and independently from the mainstream European developmental line, [45] Leontiev's core Objective Activity concept [46] has been used in a Social Development context. In the Organization Workshop's Large Group Capacitation-method, [47] objective/ized activity acts as the core causal principle which postulates that, in order to change the mind-set of (large groups of) individuals, we need to start with changes to their activity – and/or to the object that "suggests" their activity. [48] In Leontievian vein, the Organization Workshop is about semiotically mediated activities through which (large groups of) participants learn how to manage themselves and the organizations they create to perform tasks that require complex division of labor. [48]

CHAT-inspired research and practice since the 1980s

Over the last two decades, CHAT has offered a theoretical lens informing research and practice, in that it posits that learning takes place through collective activities that are purposefully conducted around a common object. Starting from the premise that learning is a social and cultural process that draws on historical achievements, its systems thinking-based perspectives allow insights into the real world. [49] [50] [33]

Change Laboratory (CL)

Change Laboratory (CL) is a CHAT-based method for formative intervention in activity systems and for research on their developmental potential as well as processes of expansive learning, collaborative concept-formation, and transformation of practices, elaborated in the mid-nineties [51] [52] by the Finnish Developmental Work Research (DWR) group, which became CRADLE in 2008. [33] The CL method relies on collaboration between practitioners of the activity being analyzed and transformed, and academic researchers or interventionists supporting and facilitating collective developmental processes. [33] Engeström developed a theory of expansive learning, which "begins with individual subjects questioning accepted practices, and it gradually expands into a collective movement or institution. The theory enables a "longitudinal and rich analysis of inter-organizational learning by using observational as well as interventionist designs in studies of work and organization". [53] [12] From this, the foundation of an interventionist research approach at DWR [33] was elaborated in the 1980s, and developed further in the 1990s as an intervention method now known as Change Laboratory. [54] CL interventions are used both to study the conditions of change and to help those working in organizations to develop their work, drawing on participant observation, interviews, and the recording and videotaping of meetings and work practices. Initially, with the help of an external interventionist, the first stimulus that is beyond the actors' present capabilities, is produced in the Change Laboratory by collecting first-hand empirical data on problematic aspects of the activity. This data may comprise difficult client cases, descriptions of recurrent disturbances and ruptures in the process of producing the outcome. Steps in the CL process: Step 1 Questioning; Step 2 Analysis; Step 3 Modeling; Step 4 Examining; Step 5 Implementing; Step 6 Reflecting; Step 7 Consolidating. These seven action steps for increased understanding are described by Engeström as expansive learning, or phases of an outwardly expanding spiral, [2] while multiple kinds of actions can take place at any time. [55] The phases of the model simply allow for the identification and analysis of the dominant action type during a particular period of time. These learning actions are provoked by contradictions. [16] Contradictions are not simply conflicts or problems, but are "historically accumulating structural tensions within and between activity systems". [33] CL is used by a team or work unit or by collaborating partners across the organizational boundaries, initially with the help of an interventionist-researcher. [50] [54] The CL method has been used in agricultural contexts, [56] educational and media settings, [57] health care [58] and learning support. [2] [52]

Activity systems analysis (ASA)

Activity systems analysis is a CHAT-based method that uses Activity Theory concepts such as mediated action, goal-directed activity and dialectical relationship between the individual and environment for understanding human activity in real-world situations with data collection, analysis, and presentation methods that address the complexities of human activity in natural settings aimed to advance both theory and practice. [59] [60] [12] It is based on Vygotsky's concept of mediated action and captures human activity in a triangle model that includes the subject, tool, object, rule, community, and division of labor. [12] Subjects are participants in an activity, motivated toward a purpose or attainment of the object. The object can be the goal of an activity, the subject's motives for participating in an activity, and the material products that subjects gain through an activity. Tools are socially shared cognitive and material resources that subjects can use to attain the object. Informal or formal rules regulate the subject's participation while engaging in an activity. The community is the group or organization to which subjects belong. The division of labor is the shared participation responsibilities in the activity determined by the community. Finally, the outcome is the consequences that the subject faces due to actions driven by the object. These outcomes can encourage or hinder the subject's participation in future activities. In Part 2 of her video "Using Activity Theory to understand human behaviour", van der Riet 2010 shows how activity theory is applied to the problem of behavior change and HIV and AIDs (in South Africa). The video focuses on sexual activity as the activity of the system and illustrates how an activity system analysis, through a historical and current account of the activity, provides a way of understanding the lack of behavior change in response to HIV and AIDS. The book Activity Systems Analysis Methods [61] describes seven ASA case studies which fall "into four distinct work clusters. These clusters include works that help (a) understand developmental work research (DWR), (b) describe real-world learning situations, (c) design human-computer interaction systems, and (d) plan solutions to complicated work-based problems". Other uses of ASA include summarizing organizational change; [60] identifying guidelines for designing constructivist learning environments; [62] identifying contradictions and tensions that shape developments in educational settings; [14] [28] demonstrating historical developments in organizational learning, [63] and evaluating K–12 school and university partnership relations. [64]

Human–computer interaction (HCI)

When human-computer interaction (HCI) first appeared as a separate field of study in the early 1980s, HCI adopted the information processing paradigm of computer science as the model for human cognition, predicated on prevalent cognitive psychology criteria, which did not account for individuals' interests, needs and frustrations involved, nor that the technology depends on the social and dynamic contexts in which it takes place. [3] [1] Adopting a CHAT theoretical perspective carries implications for understanding how people use interactive technologies: for example, a computer is typically an object of activity rather than a mediating artefact means that people interact with the world through computers, rather than with computer 'objects'. [3] [65] [66] Since the 1980s, a number of diverse methodologies outlining techniques for human–computer interaction design have emerged. Most design methodologies stem from a model for how users, designers, and technical systems interact. [3] [1]

Systemic-structural activity theory (SSAT)

SSAT builds on the general theory of activity to provide an effective basis for both experimental and analytic methods of studying human performance, using developed units of analysis. [67] SSAT approaches cognition both as a process and as a structured system of actions or other functional information-processing units, developing a taxonomy of human activity through the use of structurally organized units of analysis. The systemic-structural approach to activity design and analysis involves identifying the available means of work, tools and objects; their relationship with possible strategies of work activity; existing constraints on activity performance; social norms and rules; possible stages of object transformation; and changes in the structure of activity during skills acquisition.This method is demonstrated by applying it to the study of a human–computer interaction task. [68]

Future

Evolving field of study

CHAT offers a philosophical and cross-disciplinary perspective for analyzing human practices as development processes in which both individual and social levels are interlinked, as well as interactions and boundary-crossings [30] between activity systems. [1] [69] Crossing boundaries involves "encountering difference, entering into unfamiliar territory, requiring cognitive retooling". [70] More recently, the focus of studies of organizational learning has increasingly shifted away from learning within single organizations or organizational units, towards learning in multi‐organizational or inter‐organizational networks, as well as to the exploration of interactions in their social contexts, multiple contexts and cultures, and the dynamics and development of particular activities. This shift has generated such concepts as "networks of learning", [16] "networked learning", [53] coworking, [71] and knotworking. [72] [31] [73] Industry has seen growth in nonemployer firms (NEFs) due to changes in long-term employment trends and developments in mobile technology [74] which have led to more work from remote locations, more distance collaboration, and more work organized around temporary projects. [75] Developments such as these and new forms of social production or commons-based peer production like open source software development and cultural production in peer-to-peer (P2P) networks have become a key focus in Engeström's work. [73] [76] Social production processes are simultaneous, multi-directional and often reciprocal. The density and complexity of these processes blur distinctions between process and structure. The object of the activity is unstable, resists control and standardization, and requires rapid integration of expertise from various locations and traditions. [71]

"Fourth generation"

The rapid rise of new forms of activities characterised by web-based social and participatory practices phenomena such as distributed workforce and the dominance of knowledge work, prompts a rethink of the third-generation model, bringing a need for a fourth generation activity system model. [71] [75] [2] [71] Fourth-Generation (4GAT) analysis should allow better examination of how activity networks interact, interpenetrate, and contradict each other. People "working alone together" may illuminate other examples of distributed, interorganizational, collaborative knowledge work. [75] In fourth generation CHAT, the object(ive) will typically comprise multiple perspectives and contexts and be inherently transient; collaborations between actors are likely to be temporary, with multiple boundary crossings between interrelated activities. [30] Fourth-generation activity theorists have specifically developed activity theory to better accommodate Castells's [74] (and others') insights into how work organization has shifted in the network society. Hence, they will focus less on the workings of individual activity systems (often represented by triangles) and more on the interactions across activity systems functioning in networks. [75] [71]

See also

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Developmental psychology</span> Scientific study of psychological changes in humans over the course of their lives

Developmental psychology is the scientific study of how and why humans grow, change, and adapt across the course of their lives. Originally concerned with infants and children, the field has expanded to include adolescence, adult development, aging, and the entire lifespan. Developmental psychologists aim to explain how thinking, feeling, and behaviors change throughout life. This field examines change across three major dimensions, which are physical development, cognitive development, and social emotional development. Within these three dimensions are a broad range of topics including motor skills, executive functions, moral understanding, language acquisition, social change, personality, emotional development, self-concept, and identity formation.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Lev Vygotsky</span> Soviet psychologist (1896–1934)

Lev Semyonovich Vygotsky was a Russian and Soviet psychologist, best known for his work on psychological development in children and creating the framework known as cultural-historical activity theory. After his early death, his books and research were banned in the Soviet Union until Joseph Stalin's death in 1953, with a first collection of major texts published in 1956.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Alexander Luria</span> Russian neuropsychologist (1902-1977)

Alexander Romanovich Luria was a Soviet neuropsychologist, often credited as a father of modern neuropsychology. He developed an extensive and original battery of neuropsychological tests during his clinical work with brain-injured victims of World War II, which are still used in various forms. He made an in-depth analysis of the functioning of various brain regions and integrative processes of the brain in general. Luria's magnum opus, Higher Cortical Functions in Man (1962), is a much-used psychological textbook which has been translated into many languages and which he supplemented with The Working Brain in 1973.

Aleksei Nikolayevich Leontiev, was a Soviet Russian developmental psychologist and philosopher and a founder of activity theory.

Activity theory is an umbrella term for a line of eclectic social-sciences theories and research with its roots in the Soviet psychological activity theory pioneered by Sergei Rubinstein in the 1930s. It was later advocated for and popularized by Alexei Leont'ev. Some of the traces of the theory in its inception can also be found in a few works of Lev Vygotsky. These scholars sought to understand human activities as systemic and socially situated phenomena and to go beyond paradigms of reflexology and classical conditioning, psychoanalysis and behaviorism. It became one of the major psychological approaches in the former USSR, being widely used in both theoretical and applied psychology, and in education, professional training, ergonomics, social psychology and work psychology.

The psychology of learning refers to theories and research on how individuals learn. There are many theories of learning. Some take on a more behaviorist approach which focuses on inputs and reinforcements. Other approaches, such as neuroscience and social cognition, focus more on how the brain's organization and structure influence learning. Some psychological approaches, such as social constructivism, focus more on one's interaction with the environment and with others. Other theories, such as those related to motivation, like the growth mindset, focus more on individuals' perceptions of ability.

Distributed cognition is an approach to cognitive science research that was developed by cognitive anthropologist Edwin Hutchins during the 1990s.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Constructivism (philosophy of education)</span> Philosophical viewpoint about the nature of knowledge; theory of knowledge

Constructivism in education is a theory that suggests that learners do not passively acquire knowledge through direct instruction. Instead, they construct their understanding through experiences and social interaction, integrating new information with their existing knowledge. This theory originates from Swiss developmental psychologist Jean Piaget's theory of cognitive development.

Cognitive development is a field of study in neuroscience and psychology focusing on a child's development in terms of information processing, conceptual resources, perceptual skill, language learning, and other aspects of the developed adult brain and cognitive psychology. Qualitative differences between how a child processes their waking experience and how an adult processes their waking experience are acknowledged. Cognitive development is defined as the emergence of the ability to consciously cognize, understand, and articulate their understanding in adult terms. Cognitive development is how a person perceives, thinks, and gains understanding of their world through the relations of genetic and learning factors. There are four stages to cognitive information development. They are, reasoning, intelligence, language, and memory. These stages start when the baby is about 18 months old, they play with toys, listen to their parents speak, they watch TV, anything that catches their attention helps build their cognitive development.

Group cognition is a social, largely linguistic phenomenon whereby a group of people produce a sequence of utterances that performs a cognitive act. That is, if a similar sequence was uttered or thought by an individual it would be considered an act of cognition or thinking. The group can be a small group, such as 3–5 people talking together or working together online. The group can also be a larger collective, such as a classroom of students or a global community contributing asynchronously to an extended discourse on a problem or topic or to a knowledge repository like Wikipedia. The theory of group cognition is a postcognitivism philosophy, which considers a larger unit of analysis than an individual mind as a producer of cognitive activities such as creative problem solving.

The Kharkov school of psychology is a tradition of developmental psychological research conducted in the paradigm of Lev Vygotsky's "sociocultural theory of mind" and Leontiev's psychological activity theory.

Cultural-historical psychology is a branch of psychological theory and practice associated with Lev Vygotsky and Alexander Luria and their Circle, who initiated it in the mid-1920s–1930s. The phrase "cultural-historical psychology" never occurs in the writings of Vygotsky, and was subsequently ascribed to him by his critics and followers alike, yet it is under this title that this intellectual movement is now widely known. The main goal of Vygotsky-Luria project was the establishment of a "new psychology" that would account for the inseparable unity of mind, brain and culture in their development in concrete socio-historical settings and throughout the history of humankind as socio-biological species. In its most radical forms, the theory that Vygotsky and Luria were attempting to build was expressed in terms of a "science of Superman", and was closely linked with the pronouncement for the need in a new psychological theory of consciousness and its relationship to the development of higher psychological functions. All this theoretical and experimental empirical work was attempted by the members of the Vygotsky Circle.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">László Garai</span>

László Garai was a scholar of psychology: studies theoretical psychology, social psychology and economic psychology.

Anna Stetsenko is a developmental psychologist known for her important contributions to cultural-historical activity theory, building on the work of Lev Vygotsky, Leontiev and Alexander Luria. Her research centers human development, education, and social theory.

Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition (LCHC) is a social science laboratory located at the University of California, San Diego (UCSD) since 1978. Scholars at LCHC pursue research focused on understanding the complex relationship between cognition and culture in individual and social development. Such research requires collaboration among scholars from a variety of research disciplines, including cognitive science, education, linguistics, psychology, anthropology, and sociology. LCHC also functions as a research and training institution, arranging for pre-doctoral, doctoral, and post-doctoral training, as well as research exchanges with scholars throughout the world. In addition, LCHC sponsors a journal, Mind, Culture and Activity: An International Journal (MCA), and an open internet discussion group, XMCA.

The Vygotsky Circle was an influential informal network of psychologists, educationalists, medical specialists, physiologists, and neuroscientists, associated with Lev Vygotsky (1896–1934) and Alexander Luria (1902–1977), active in 1920-early 1940s in the Soviet Union. The work of the Circle contributed to the foundation of the integrative science of mind, brain, and behavior in their cultural and bio-social development also known under somewhat vague and imprecise name of cultural-historical psychology.

In the framework of the Cultural-Historical Activity Theory (CHAT) the leading activity is the activity, or cooperative human action, which plays the most essential role in child development during a given developmental period. Although many activities may play a role in a child's development at any given time, the leading activity is theorized to be the type of social interaction that is most beneficial in terms of producing major developmental accomplishments, and preparing the child for the next period of development. Through engaging in leading activities, a child develops a wide range of capabilities, including emotional connection with others, motivation to engage in more complex social activities, the creation of new cognitive abilities, and the restructuring of old ones.

Large-group capacitation is an adult education and social psychology concept associated with the Brazilian sociologist Clodomir Santos de Morais, and grounded in the "activity" of the individual and the social psychology of the large group. When applied to the context of the Organization Workshop (OW), which, historically, has been used mainly for the purpose of job creation and income generation, it is known as Metodología da Capacitação Massiva (MCM) in Portuguese, Método de Capacitación Masiva (MCM) in Spanish and as Large-Group Capacitation Method (LGCM) in English.

Distributed leadership is a conceptual and analytical approach to understanding how the work of leadership takes place among the people and in context of a complex organization. Though developed and primarily used in education research, it has since been applied to other domains, including business and even tourism. Rather than focus on characteristics of the individual leader or features of the situation, distributed leadership foregrounds how actors engage in tasks that are "stretched" or distributed across the organization. With theoretical foundations in activity theory and distributed cognition, understanding leadership from a distributed perspective means seeing leadership activities as a situated and social process at the intersection of leaders, followers, and the situation.

Carol Diane Lee is an American professor, educational researcher, school director and author. Now retired, Lee was the Edwina S. Tarry Professor of Education and Social Policy, Professor of Learning Sciences, and Professor of African-American Studies at Northwestern University. Her scholarly interests focus on the influences of culture and literacy on education, particularly among students in the African-American community. She chairs the Board of Director of the Betty Shabbazz International Charter School, an institution she helped found.

References

  1. 1 2 3 4 5 Nardi, Bonnie A. (1996). Context and consciousness: activity theory and human-computer interaction. Cambridge (Mass.) London: MIT press. p. 7. ISBN   978-0-2621-4058-4.
  2. 1 2 3 4 Daniels, Harry, ed. (2010). Activity theory in practice: promoting learning across boundaries and agencies. London ; New York: Routledge. ISBN   978-0-4154-7724-6. OCLC   244057445.
  3. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Kaptelinin, Victor; Nardi, Bonnie A. (2006). Acting with technology: activity theory and interaction design. Acting with technology. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press. ISBN   978-0-2621-1298-7. OCLC   64098581.
  4. 1 2 3 4 5 Roth, Wolff-Michael; Lee, Yew-Jin (June 2007). ""Vygotsky's Neglected Legacy": Cultural-Historical Activity Theory". Review of Educational Research. 77 (2): 186–232. doi:10.3102/0034654306298273. ISSN   0034-6543.
  5. Yasnitsky, Anton (2018). Vygotsky: an intellectual biography. Abingdon, Oxon ; New York, NY: Routledge. ISBN   978-1-1388-0673-3.
  6. Engeström, Yrjö; Miettinen, Reijo; Punamäki-Gitai, Raija-Leena, eds. (2007). Perspectives on activity theory. Learning in doing social, cognitive and computational perspectives (Repr ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge Univ. Press. ISBN   978-0-5214-3730-1.
  7. 1 2 Bakhurst, David (20 May 2009). "Reflections on activity theory". Educational Review. 61 (2): 197–210. doi:10.1080/00131910902846916. ISSN   0013-1911., V. Lektorsky in Engeström, Miettinen & Punamäki-Gitai 1999 , p. 66 ;Brushlinskii, A. V. 2004 Archived 1 September 2014 at archive.today
  8. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in Society: Development of Higher Psychological Processes. Harvard University Press.
  9. Roth, Radford & Lacroix 2012.
  10. 1 2 Leontyev, Aleksei Nikolaevich (1978). Activity and Consciousness. Prentice-Hall.
  11. Cole, Michael (1996). Cultural psychology: a once and future discipline. Cambridge, Mass: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press. ISBN   978-0-6741-7951-6.
  12. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 Engeström, Yrjö (1987). Learning by expanding: an activity-theoretical approach to developmental research. Helsinki: Orienta-Konsultit Oy. ISBN   978-9-5195-9332-6.
  13. 1 2 Stetsenko, Anna (17 November 2009). "Activity as Object-Related: Resolving the Dichotomy of Individual and Collective Planes of Activity". Mind, Culture, and Activity. 12 (1): 70–88. doi:10.1207/s15327884mca1201_6. ISSN   1074-9039 via Taylor & Francis.
  14. 1 2 Yamagata-Lynch, Lisa C. (12 October 2007). "Confronting Analytical Dilemmas for Understanding Complex Human Interactions in Design-Based Research From a Cultural—Historical Activity Theory (CHAT) Framework". Journal of the Learning Sciences. 16 (4): 451–484. doi:10.1080/10508400701524777. ISSN   1050-8406.
  15. Blunden, Andy. 2012, "The Origins of Cultural Historical Activity Theory" Archived 27 January 2014 at the Wayback Machine  ; Blunden, Andy. Genealogy of CHAT (graph).
  16. 1 2 3 4 5 Engeström, Yrjö; Miettinen, Reijo; Punamäki-Gitai, Raija-Leena, eds. (1999). Perspectives on Activity Theory. Learning in doing. Cambridge University Press. ISBN   978-0-5214-3127-9.
  17. 1 2 3 Blunden, Andy (2012). An Interdisciplinary Theory of Activity. Haymarket Books.
  18. Yasnitsky, Anton; Ferrari, Michel (2008). "Rethinking the Early History of Post-Vygotskian Psychology: The Case of the Kharkov School". History of Psychology. 11 (2): 101–121.
  19. Veer, René van der; Valsiner, Jaan (1991). Understanding Vygotsky: a quest for synthesis. Oxford, UK ; Cambridge, USA: Blackwell. ISBN   978-0-6311-6528-6.
  20. Fraser, J.; Yasnitsky, A. (2015). "Deconstructing Vygotsky's Victimization Narrative: A Re-Examination of the "Stalinist Suppression" of Vygotskian Theory" (PDF). History of the Human Sciences. 28: 128–153. doi:10.1177/0952695114560200. S2CID   4934828.
  21. Wertsch, James V., ed. (1981). The Concept of activity in Soviet psychology. Armonk, N.Y: M.E. Sharpe. ISBN   978-0-8733-2158-7.
  22. Lurii︠a︡, A. R.; Cole, Michael; Cole, Sheila (1979). The making of mind: a personal account of Soviet psychology. Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press. ISBN   978-0-6745-4326-3.
  23. Other CHAT-based centers of research and learning: Oxford Center for Sociocultural and Activity Theory Research Archived 5 February 2015 at the Wayback Machine (OSAT), International Society o Cultural and Activity Research(ISCAR), LCSD: Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition (LCHC), Kansai University (Japan) CHAT: Center for Activity Theory Archived 28 July 2017 at the Wayback Machine . BERA British Educational Research Association [ permanent dead link ] (CHAT Special Interest Group).
  24. History of ISCRAT Archived 12 January 2005 at the Wayback Machine (International Standing Conference for Research, on Activity Theory)
  25. (Selective) list of other CHAT-related research groups and centers: ATUL Archived 14 February 2017 at the Wayback Machine (Activity Theory Usability Laboratory at the University of Wollongong Australia – "Organisations and communities of the Knowledge Age"). ; CHAT Archived 28 July 2017 at the Wayback Machine (Center for Human Activity Theory, Kansai University, Japan – "Links with Helsinki, Bath and California"). ; OSAT Archived 5 February 2015 at the Wayback Machine (Oxford Centre for Sociocultural and Activity Theory Research at Oxford University UK. – "Learning across the age range".) ; LIW (Learning in and for Interagency Working at the University of Bath, UK – "Effective multiagency working".)
  26. 1 2 Nussbaumer, Doris (15 September 2011). "An overview of cultural historical activity theory (CHAT) use in classroom research 2000 to 2009". Educational Review. 64 (1): 37–55. doi:10.1080/00131911.2011.553947. ISSN   0013-1911.
  27. 1 2 Engeström, Yrjö (1 April 2009). "Wildfire Activities: New Patterns of Mobility and Learning". International Journal of Mobile and Blended Learning (in Ndonga). 1 (2): 1–18. doi:10.4018/jmbl.2009040101. ISSN   1941-8647.
  28. 1 2 3 Yamagata-Lynch, Lisa C. (2010). Activity systems analysis methods: understanding complex learning environments. New York ; London: Springer. ISBN   978-1-4419-6320-8. OCLC   642290978.
  29. Yamagata-Lynch, Lisa C.; Haudenschild, Michael T. (2009). "Using activity systems analysis to identify inner contradictions in teacher professional development". Teaching and Teacher Education. 25 (3): 507–517. doi:10.1016/j.tate.2008.09.014.; Engeström, 2008, "The Future of Activity Theory" p. 6.
  30. 1 2 3 Akkerman, Sanne F.; Bakker, Arthur (1 June 2011). "Boundary Crossing and Boundary Objects". Review of Educational Research. 81 (2): 132–169. doi:10.3102/0034654311404435. ISSN   0034-6543.
  31. 1 2 Spinuzzi, Clay (2012). "Working Alone Together: Coworking as Emergent Collaborative Activity". Journal of Business and Technical Communication. 26 (4): 399–441. doi:10.1177/1050651912444070. ISSN   1050-6519.
  32. Wells, G (2007). "Semiotic Mediation, Dialogue and Construction of Knowledge" (PDF). Human Development. 50 (5): 244–274. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.506.7763 . doi:10.1159/000106414. S2CID   15984672.
  33. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 Engeström, Yrjö (2010). "Expansive Learning at Work: Toward an activity theoretical reconceptualization". Journal of Education and Work. 14 (1): 133–156. doi:10.1080/13639080020028747. ISSN   1363-9080.
  34. Hasan, Helen M. (1 January 2007). "A cultural-historical activity theory approach to users, usability and usefulness". Faculty of Commerce: 140–149.
  35. 1 2 Jonassen, David H.; Land, Susan, eds. (2012). Theoretical foundations of learning environments (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Routledge. ISBN   978-0-4158-9421-0.
  36. Kaptelinin, Victor (17 November 2009). "The Object of Activity: Making Sense of the Sense-Maker". Mind, Culture, and Activity. 12 (1): 4–18. doi:10.1207/s15327884mca1201_2. ISSN   1074-9039.
  37. Engeström, Yrjö (2007), Daniels, Harry; Wertsch, James V.; Cole, Michael (eds.), "Putting Vygotsky to Work: The Change Laboratory as an Application of Double Stimulation", The Cambridge Companion to Vygotsky, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 363–382, doi:10.1017/ccol0521831040.015, ISBN   978-0-5215-3787-2 , retrieved 11 October 2024
  38. Lave, Jean; Wenger, Etienne (1991). Situated learning: legitimate peripheral participation. Learning in doing. Cambridge [England] ; New York: Cambridge University Press. ISBN   978-0-5214-1308-4.
  39. 1 2 Hardman, Joanne (2007). "Making sense of the meaning maker: tracking the object of activity in a computer-based mathematics lesson using activity theory". International Journal of Education and Development using Information and Communication Technology. 3 (4): 110–130.
  40. Engeström, Yrjö (March 1999). "Expansive Visibilization of Work: An Activity-Theoretical Perspective". Computer Supported Cooperative Work (CSCW). 8 (1–2): 63–93. doi:10.1023/A:1008648532192. ISSN   0925-9724.
  41. Wertsch 1981.
  42. 1 2 Leont'ev, A. N. (2009). The Development of Mind: Selected Works of Alekseĭ Nikolaevich Leontʹev. Marxists Internet Archive.
  43. Bozalek, Vivienne, ed. (2015). Activity theory, authentic learning and emerging technologies: towards a transformative higher education pedagogy. Routledge research in higher education. London ; New York: Routledge, Taylor & Francis Group. ISBN   978-1-1387-7859-7.
  44. Jonassen, David H.; Land, Susan M., eds. (2012). Theoretical foundations of learning environments (2nd ed.). New York: Routledge. ISBN   978-0-4158-9421-0. OCLC   742512527.
  45. Cole, Michael; Engeström, Yrjö; Vasquez, Olga A., eds. (1997). Mind, culture, and activity: seminal papers from the Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition. Cambridge ; New York: Cambridge University Press. ISBN   978-0-5215-5238-7.
  46. From the original Предметная деятельность(Russian) (predmetnaja-dejatelnost) and Gegenständliche Tätigkeit(German), commonly translated as "Actividad Objetivada"(Spanish) – see: Leontiev 1984 , p. 66, and as Objective Activity in English: e.g. Chapter 3 of Leontiev 1978 , p. 50ss, Wertsch 1981 , p. 46ss and Miettinen 2005 Abstract:'Leontiev's concept of practice or Objective Activity'. (For "Objectivis/zed", see e.g.: Leontiev 1978 , p. 116: "Activity's secondary objectivised existence" and Leontiev 1978 , p. 404: "the mental image objectivised in its product"). "Objectivized Activity" is perceived to be closer to both the letter and the spirit of dynamic, interactive, dialectical weighting of Leontiev's original predmetnaya dyaetel'nost.
  47. "Laboratories on Objectivized Activity" in: Labra & Labra 2012; Labra 1992: "Actividad Objetivada"(Spanish) p. 53; "de Morais' antecedent thought on Objectivised Activity" in Andersson 2004 , p. 221 ss; Carmen & Sobrado 2000 , p. 118 & note No. 2.
  48. 1 2 Andersson 2013
  49. Mutizwa Mukute, M.& Lotz-Sisitka, H. 2012 "Working With Cultural-Historical Activity Theory and Critical Realism to Investigate and Expand Farmer Learning in Southern Africa". Rhodes University. SA.
  50. 1 2 Foot, Kirsten A. (2014). "Cultural-historical activity theory: Exploring a theory to inform practice and research". Journal of Human Behavior in the Social Environment. 24 (3): 329–347 via Taylor & Francis.
  51. Sannino, 2008 "From Talk to Action: Experiencing Interlocution in Developmental Interventions" Sannino sees in Yves Clot's Vygotsky-inspired Clinic of Activity (Clinique de l'Activité) a potentially complementary – (to CL, that is) – intervention method. (See also Clot, Yves, 2009 Clinic of Activity: The Dialogue as Instrument in Sannino, Daniels & Gutiérrez 2009).
  52. 1 2 Santally, Mohammad Issack; Cooshna-Naik, Dorothy; Conruyt, Noel (May 2014). "A Model for the Transformation of the Mauritian Classroom Based on the Living Lab Concept". IST-Africa Conference Proceedings, IEEE.
  53. 1 2 Engeström, Yrjö; Kerosuo, Hannele (14 August 2007). Engestrom, Yrjö (ed.). "From workplace learning to inter‐organizational learning and back: the contribution of activity theory". Journal of Workplace Learning. 19 (6): 336–342. doi:10.1108/13665620710777084. ISSN   1366-5626.
  54. 1 2 Engeström, Yrjö; Virkkunen, Jaakko; Helle, Merja; Pihlaja, Juha; Poikela, Ritva (2005) [1996]. "Change Laboratory as a Tool for Transforming Work" (PDF). Developmental Work Research: Expanding Activity Theory in Practice.
  55. Engeström, Yrjö; Sannino, Annalisa (2010). "Studies of expansive learning: Foundations, findings and future challenges". Educational Research Review. 5 (1): 1–24. doi:10.1016/j.edurev.2009.12.002.
  56. e.g. Mukute, M., 2009 Archived 28 January 2015 at the Wayback Machine
  57. Teräs, Marianne (2007). Intercultural learning and hybridity in the culture laboratory (Thesis). Helsinki: University of Helsinki, Department of Education. ISBN   978-9-5210-3652-1.
  58. "Kerosuo, H., 2006". Archived from the original on 28 January 2015. Retrieved 26 January 2015.
  59. Cole, M., & Engeström, Y. (1993). "A cultural–historical approach to distributed cognition" in Salomon, G (Ed), 1993]
  60. 1 2 Engeström, Yrjö (25 June 1993), Chaiklin, Seth; Lave, Jean (eds.), "Developmental studies of work as a testbench of activity theory: The case of primary care medical practice", Understanding Practice (1 ed.), Cambridge University Press, pp. 64–103, doi:10.1017/cbo9780511625510.004, ISBN   978-0-5215-5851-8 , retrieved 11 October 2024
  61. Yamagata-Lynch, Lisa C. (2010). Activity systems analysis methods: understanding complex learning environments. New York Heidelberg: Springer. ISBN   978-1-4419-6320-8.
  62. Jonassen, David H.; Rohrer-Murphy, Lucia (1999). "Activity theory as a framework for designing constructivist learning environments". Educational Technology Research and Development. 47 (1): 61–79. doi:10.1007/BF02299477. S2CID   62209169.
  63. Yamagata-Lynch, Lisa C. (2003). "Using Activity Theory as an Analytic Lens for Examining Technology Professional Development in Schools". Mind, Culture, and Activity. 10 (2): 100–119. doi:10.1207/S1532-7884MCA1002_2. S2CID   143340720.
  64. Yamagata-Lynch, Lisa C.; Smaldino, Sharon (2007). "Using activity theory to evaluate and improve K-12 school and university partnerships". Evaluation and Program Planning. 30 (4): 364–380. doi:10.1016/j.evalprogplan.2007.08.003. PMID   17881055. – For ASA and (computer-based) mathematics education see e.g. Hardman 2007
  65. Bødker, S "A Human Activity approach to User Interfaces" in: Human-Computer Interaction, 1989, Volume 4, pp.171–195
  66. Kaptelinin, Victor (1 January 2014). "Activity Theory". Interaction Design Foundation - IxDF. Retrieved 11 October 2024.
  67. Bedny, G.; Karwowski, W. (2003). "A Systemic-Structural Activity Approach to the Design of Human–Computer Interaction Tasks". International Journal of Human-Computer Interaction. 16 (2): 235–260. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.151.8384 . doi:10.1207/s15327590ijhc1602_06. S2CID   16212346.
  68. Bedny, Karwowski & Bedny 2014; Bedny, I. S.; Karwowski, W.; Bedny, G. Z. (2010). "A Method of Human Reliability Assessment Based on Systemic-Structural Activity Theory". International Journal of Human-Computer Interaction. 26 (4): 377–402. doi:10.1080/10447310903575507. S2CID   32705523.; Bedny & Karwowski 2006;
  69. Engeström, Yrjö (1999). Lernen durch Expansion [Learning by Expanding: Ten Years After]. Internationale Studien zur Tätigkeitstheorie (1. Aufl ed.). Marburg: BdWi-Verl. ISBN   978-3-9246-8475-4.
  70. Tuomi-Gröhn, Terttu; Engeström, Yrjö, eds. (2003). Between school and work: new perspectives on transfer and boundary-crossing. Advances in learning and instruction series (1st ed.). Pergamon. ISBN   978-0-0804-4296-9.
  71. 1 2 3 4 5 Sannino, Annalisa; Daniels, Harry; Gutiérrez, Kris D., eds. (2009). Learning and expanding with activity theory (PDF). New York: Cambridge University Press. ISBN   978-0-5217-6075-1. OCLC   247439755.
  72. Spinuzzi, Clay (2011). "Losing by Expanding: Corralling the Runaway Object". Journal of Business and Technical Communication. 25 (4): 449–486. doi:10.1177/1050651911411040. ISSN   1050-6519.
  73. 1 2 Engeström, Yrjö (1 January 2004). "New forms of learning in co‐configuration work". Journal of Workplace Learning. 16 (1/2): 11–21. doi:10.1108/13665620410521477. ISSN   1366-5626.
  74. 1 2 Castells, Manuel (2001). The Internet galaxy: reflections on the Internet, business, and society. Oxford ; New York: Oxford University Press. ISBN   978-0-1992-4153-8.
  75. 1 2 3 4 Spinuzzi, Clay (4 March 2014). "How Nonemployer Firms Stage-Manage Ad Hoc Collaboration: An Activity Theory Analysis". Technical Communication Quarterly. 23 (2): 88–114. doi:10.1080/10572252.2013.797334. hdl: 2152/28329 . ISSN   1057-2252 via Taylor & Francis.
  76. Hughes, Jason; Jewson, Nick; Unwin, Lorna, eds. (2007). Communities of practice: critical perspectives. London ; New York, NY: Routledge. ISBN   978-0-4153-6473-7.

Works cited

Further reading