This article has multiple issues. Please help improve it or discuss these issues on the talk page . (Learn how and when to remove these messages)
|
Proto-Finnic | |
---|---|
Proto-Balto-Finnic | |
Reconstruction of | Finnic languages |
Reconstructed ancestor |
Proto-Finnic or Proto-Baltic-Finnic is the common ancestor of the Finnic languages, which include the national languages Finnish and Estonian. Proto-Finnic is not attested in any texts, but has been reconstructed by linguists. Proto-Finnic is itself descended ultimately from Proto-Uralic.
Three stages of Proto-Finnic are distinguished in literature.
Views on when and where Proto-Finnic was spoken have varied over the years. Many of the older sources do not recognize Middle Proto-Finnic, recognizing only Early and Late Proto-Finnic:
Year | Author(s) | Early Proto-Finnic | Middle Proto-Finnic | Late Proto-Finnic | Notes | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1965 | Décsy [1] | c. 400 BC–1 AD | 1–1000 AD | |||||
1977 | Sammallahti [2] | c. ...–1000 BC | c. 1000–200 BC | [a] | ||||
1981 | Korhonen [3] | c. 1500–1000 BC | c. 1000 BC–1 AD | [b] | ||||
2012–2014 | Kallio | c. 1800–500 BC [4] | c. 500 BC–200 AD [4] | split by the end of first millennium AD [5] | ||||
2018 | Lang [6] | c. 1200–800..500 BC | c. 800..500 BC–1 AD | c. 1 AD–800 AD | [d] |
a. Sammallahti places the divergence of South Estonian from Late Proto-Finnic (myöhäiskantasuomi, which he uses to refer to the stage before that point) at around 600–500 BC.
b. Korhonen recognizes a distinction between Middle and Late Proto-Finnic but does not specify the date of transition.
c. Lang places the transition from Early to Middle Proto-Finnic somewhere between 800–500 BC.
Proto-Finnic is thought to have been spoken around the Gulf of Finland, but theories on its earlier location have varied; traditionally it has been considered that Proto-Finnic arrived first on the eastern coast of the Gulf of Finland, but it has also been suggested that Middle Proto-Finnic was spoken in an area in modern-day Estonia and northeastern parts of Latvia. [7]
The sounds of Proto-Finnic can be reconstructed through the comparative method.
Reconstructed Proto-Finnic is traditionally transcribed using the Uralic Phonetic Alphabet. The following UPA and related conventions are adopted in this article for transcribing Proto-Finnic forms:
The Proto-Finnic consonant inventory had relatively few phonemic fricatives, much like that of the modern Finnic languages. Voicing was not phonemically contrastive, but the language did possess voiced allophones of certain voiceless consonants.
The table below lists the consonantal phonemes of Late Proto-Finnic. [17] [18] Phones written in parentheses represent allophones and are not independent phonemes. When a consonant is notated in this article with a symbol distinct from the corresponding IPA symbol, the former is given first, followed by the latter.
Labial | Dental/ Alveolar | Palatal | Velar | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Nasals | m | n | ([ŋ]) | ||
Plosives | Voiceless | p | t | k | |
Voiced | (b) | (d) | (ɡ) | ||
Affricate | c /t͡s/ | ||||
Fricatives | Voiceless | s | h /x/ | ||
Voiced | (β) | (ð) | (ɣ) | ||
Trill | r | ||||
Approximant | v/w/ | j | |||
Lateral | l |
Proto-Finnic possessed two phonemic levels of consonant duration, short and long (geminate). The contrast itself had been inherited from Proto-Uralic, but was considerably expanded: all consonants except *r, *h, *j and *w could be short or long. The three plosives and the affricate *c/ts/ also possessed a half-long duration ([pˑ], [tˑ], [kˑ] and [tsˑ]), but these appear to have been in complementary (allophonic) distribution with fully long consonants, and therefore are not thought to have been phonemic. They appeared in predictable positions as a result of consonant gradation, like the voiced fricatives.
Consonant gradation was a process of lenition that affected the obstruents. Short plosives became voiced fricatives, while long plosives became half-long:
Strong grade | Weak grade |
---|---|
p | b ([β],[b]) |
t | d ([ð],[d]) |
k | g ([ɣ],[ɡ]) |
s | h[x] |
pp | p'[pˑ] |
tt | t'[tˑ] |
kk | k'[kˑ] |
cc[tt͡s] | c'[t͡sˑ] |
Voiced plosives occurred after nasals (mb nd ŋg), voiced fricatives in all other weak grade environments.
It is unclear if single *c gradated, and if so, into what. No Finnic language has consonant gradation for former *c, both grades result in the same outcome (mostly s).
Gradation occurred in two different environments, and can therefore be split into two types:
It is unclear whether consonant gradation was a Finnic innovation, or a retention of an old Uralic feature that was lost in most other Uralic branches. It is likely that it was inherited from an earlier stage that was also the ancestor of the Sami languages, which have gradation that is very similar to that found in the Finnic languages. However, it was still productive after certain sound changes specific to Finnic, such as the apocope of final *-i, so it was probably present as a phonetic "post-processing" rule (a surface filter) over a long period of time. It is no longer fully productive in any Finnic language, but most languages still retain large amounts of words preserving the earlier alternations.
The Proto-Finnic vowel inventory is reconstructed to a great similarity to that of modern Finnish, although the distribution of the sounds was different. The following table lists the monophthong vowels reconstructable for Proto-Finnic. [17] [19]
Front neutral | Front | Back | |
---|---|---|---|
Close | i, ii /i/, /iː/ | ü, üü /y/, /yː/ | u, uu /u/, /uː/ |
Mid | e, ee /e/, /eː/ | (ö), öö (/ø/), /øː/ | o, oo, (ë) /o/, /oː/, ([ɤ]) |
Open | ä, ää /æ/, /æː/ | a, aa /ɑ/, /ɑː/ |
All vowels could occur both short and long. In Proto-Uralic, rounded vowels /uyo/ (*u, *ü, *o) did not occur in non-initial syllables, but because of sound changes, they emerged in Proto-Finnic.
The short unrounded mid back vowel *ë was not an independent vowel, but appeared as the counterpart of the front vowel *e in the system of harmony. It merged with *e in Northern Finnic. See below under vowel harmony for more details.
The status of short *ö is unclear. It was not present in ancestral Proto-Uralic, and many instances of ö found in modern Finnic languages have only developed after Proto-Finnic, due to various sound changes. For example, Finnish has öy from *eü: löytä- 'to find', köysi 'rope' < Proto-Finnic *leütä-, *keüci, while Estonian has unrounded the diphthong instead, giving leida- and köis. Short ö was also generally added to the system by researchers for reasons of symmetry, to complete the system of vowel harmony (see below). This happened in Finnish näkö 'sight' < Proto-Finnic *näko, but not in Votic näko.
The existence of öö is clear, as this sound had regularly evolved from other combinations of sounds, in words of Uralic origin (e.g. *söö- 'to eat' ← Proto-Uralic *sewi-).
Proto-Finnic also possessed diphthongs, which were formed by combinations of a short vowel with the vowels /i/, /y/ and /u/, or equivalently with the semivowels /j/ and /w/.
Front + *i | Front + *ü | Front + *u | Back + *i | Back + *u | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Close | *üi /yi/ | *iü /iy/ | *iu /iu/ | *ui /ui/ | |
Mid to close | *ei, *öi /ei/, /øi/ | *eü /ey/ | *eu (*ëu) /eu~ɤu/ | *oi /oi/ | *ou /ou/ |
Open to close | *äi /æi/ | *äü /æy/ | *ai /ɑi/ | *au /ɑu/ |
No length contrast occurred in diphthongs. A long vowel followed by a close vowel as a suffix was shortened: e.g. the imperfect forms of *saa- "to receive", *söö- "to eat" were *sai, *söi. This process is the only reconstructible source of *öi, *üi.
Proto-Finnic is reconstructed with a system of vowel harmony that is very similar to the one found in modern Finnish. Vowels in non-initial syllables had either a front or a back vowel, depending on the quality of the vowel of the first syllable. If the first syllable contained a front vowel, non-initial syllables would contain front vowels as well, while back vowels in the first syllable would be matched with back vowels in the other syllables. Thus, all inflectional and derivational suffixes came in two forms, a front-harmonic and a back-harmonic form.
In non-initial syllables, the vowels e and i were originally a single reduced schwa-like vowel in Proto-Uralic, but had become differentiated in height over time. i arose word-finally, while e appeared medially. These vowels were front vowels at the time, and had back-vowel counterparts ë/ɤ/ and ï/ɯ/. In Proto-Finnic, ï had merged with i (/ɯ/ > /i/), so that i then became neutral to vowel harmony, and now occurred in both front-vowel and back-vowel words, even if /i/ is a front vowel. The vowels e/e/ and ë/ɤ/ appear to have remained distinct in Proto-Finnic, and remained so in North and South Estonian (as e and õ) and Votic. [20] In the other Finnic languages, /ɤ/ and /e/ merged into e/e/.
Stress was not contrastive. Words were stressed in a trochaic pattern, with primary stress on the first syllable of a word, and secondary stress on every following odd-numbered syllable.
The occurrence of two-vowel sequences was much more restricted in non-initial syllables than in initial syllables. Long vowels were absent, and some diphthongs only occurred as a result of the late contraction of disyllabic *Vji to diphthongal *Vi but were otherwise absent. Some modern Finnic languages have redeveloped long vowels and additional diphthongs in non-initial syllables as a result of the loss of certain consonants (generally d, g and h).
Root words included at least two moras, and generally followed the structure CVCV, CVCCV, CVVCV. Rarer root types included monosyllabic roots, CVV, with either a long vowel (e.g. *maa "land, earth"; *puu "tree, wood") or a diphthong (e.g. *täi "louse", *käü-däk "to walk"); roots with three syllables: CVCVCV (e.g. *petägä "pine"; *vasara "hammer") or CVCCVCV (e.g. *kattila "kettle"); and roots with a long vowel in a closed syllable: CVVCCV (e.g. *mëëkka "sword"). A syllable could begin and end with at most one consonant. Any consonant phoneme could begin or end a syllable, but word-finally only the alveolar consonants (*l, *n, *t, *r, *s and perhaps *c) and the velars *k and *h occurred. Final *-k and *-h were often lost in the later Finnic languages, but occasionally left traces of their former presence.
Word-internal consonant clusters were limited to two elements originally. However, the widespread syncope of -e- (detailed above) could cause a cluster to come into contact with a third consonant. When such innovative large clusters appeared, the result was the deletion of one or more elements in the cluster, usually the first. Likewise, the apocope of -i after two or more syllables could create word-final clusters, which were also simplified. This led to alternations that are still seen, though unproductive, in e.g. Finnish:
Note in the examples of *tuhatta and *kolmatta that Proto-Finnic did not initially tolerate clusters of a sonorant plus a geminate consonant (i.e. clusters like *-ntt-). Through loanwords, analogy and further syncope, these have only later become permissible in the Finnic languages.
Traditionally a single three-consonant cluster *-str- has been reconstructed for a small group of words showing *-tr- in Southern Finnic and in Eastern Finnish, *-sr- in Karelian and Veps, and /-hr-/ in Western Finnish. This has recently been suggested to be reinterpreted as a two-consonant cluster *-cr- with an affricate as the initial member. [21]
All inflectional and derivational endings containing *a or *u also had front-vowel variants with ä and ü, which matched the vowels in the word stem following the rules of vowel harmony. *o did not follow this rule, as noted above.
Endings which closed the final syllable of a word triggered radical gradation on that syllable. An ending could also open a previously closed syllable, which would undo the gradation. Suffixal gradation affected the endings themselves. For example, partitive *-ta would appear as *-da when added to a two-syllable word ending in a vowel (e.g. *kala, *kalada "fish"), but as *-ta after a third syllable or a consonant (*veci, *vettä "water").
Proto-Finnic nouns declined in at least 13 cases. Adjectives did not originally decline, but adjective-noun agreement was innovated in Proto-Finnic. The plural of the nominative and accusative was marked with the ending *-t, while the plural of the other cases used *-i-. The genitive and accusative singular were originally distinct (genitive *-n, accusative *-m), but had fallen together when final *-m became *-n through regular sound change. Some pronouns had a different accusative ending, which distinguished them.
The following cases were present: [17] [22]
Case | Singular ending | Plural ending | Meaning/use |
---|---|---|---|
Nominative | ∅ | *-t | Subject, object of imperative |
Accusative | *-n (also -t) | *-t | Complete (telic) object |
Genitive | *-n | *-ten (-den) *-iden | Possession, relation |
Partitive | *-ta (-da) | *-ita (-ida) | Partial object, indefinite amount |
Locative cases | |||
Inessive | *-ssa | *-issa | Being inside |
Elative | *-sta | *-ista | Motion out of |
Illative | *-sen (-hen) | *-ihen | Motion into |
Adessive | *-lla | *-illa | Being on/at |
Ablative | *-lta | *-ilta | Motion off/from |
Allative | *-len / *-lek | *-ilen / *-ilek | Motion onto/towards |
Other cases | |||
Essive | *-na | *-ina | Being, acting as |
Translative | *-ksi | *-iksi | Becoming, turning into |
Abessive | *-tta(k) | *-itta(k) | Without, lacking |
Comitative | *-nek | *-inek | With, in company of |
Instructive | *-n | *-in | With, by means of |
The genitive plural was formed in two different ways:
Both types are still found in Finnish, although unevenly distributed. In the western type, the regular loss of -d- after an unstressed (even-numbered) syllable has created forms such as -ain (< *-a-den), which are now archaic, or dialectal.
The reconstructed locative cases of Proto-Finnic can be classified according to a three-way contrast (to, in and from a state) in each of the two series of locative cases: inner–s- ("inside") and outer–l- ("outside", "upon", and other grammaticalized functions to denote "possessor", "instrument", etc.), to give the following system of six locative cases.
System | Entering | Residing | Exiting |
---|---|---|---|
Inner | sVn "into" (illative) | ssA (<*s+nA) "in" (inessive) | stA "from (in)" (elative) |
Outer | *len, *lek? "onto" (allative) | llA (<*l+nA) "on" (adessive) | ltA "from on" (ablative) |
In the above, A stands for either a or ä depending on vowel harmony, and V for an epenthetic vowel. Note that -nA is reconstructible as the Proto-Finno-Ugric locative marker (the ancestor of the Finnic essive case), -tA the separative ("from") (ancestor of the partitive case), and –n, –s or –k the lative ("to").
This system is probably a Finnic innovation, although the –s and –l cases have corresponding forms in some other Uralic languages (Sami and Volgaic; and Permian, respectively). [17]
Adjectives formed comparatives using the suffix *-mpa. [17] This suffix survives in all Finnic languages, although in several the nominative has been replaced with -mpi for unclear reasons.
Only the northernmost Finnic languages have a distinct superlative suffix, like Finnish -in ~ -impa-. The suffix was possibly originally a consonantal stem *-im(e)-, which was modified to resemble the comparative more closely in Finnish. Its consonantal nature is apparent in an older, now-obsolete essive case form of the superlative in Finnish, which ended in -inna (< *-im-na < *-ime-na with syncope).
Proto-Finnic had a series of possessive suffixes for nominals, which acted partly as genitives. These have been lost from productive use in all southern languages (traces remain in e.g. folk poetry). The system given below may therefore represent Proto-Northern Finnic rather than Proto-Finnic proper.
Possessor | Single possessed | Plural possessed |
---|---|---|
First person singular | *-mi | *-nni |
Second person singular | *-ci ~ *-ti | *-nci |
Third person singular | *-hA ~ *-sA *-hVn, *-sVn | *-nsA *-nsVn |
First person plural | *-mVn *-mVk | *-nnVn (*-mmVn?) *-nnVk |
Second person plural | *-tVn *-tVk | *-ndVn *-ndVk |
Third person plural | *-hVk ~ *-sVk | *-nsVk |
The original vowels in the plural possessor endings are not settled: evidence exists for both *A (that is, *a ~ *ä) and *e. Laakso (2001) recognizes variation only for 1PP and 2PP (giving *A for 3PP). Hakulinen (1979), giving an Early Proto-Finnic paradigm, does not include vowel-final variants for 3PS.
Possessive suffixes were ordered after case endings, and typically attach to the oblique vowel stem: e.g. *sormi : *sorme-mi 'my finger'.
The number-of-possessed contrast is by the 20th century lost everywhere except in the Southeastern Tavastian dialect of Finnish, around the municipalities of Iitti and Orimattila, and even there only in the nominative in the first and second person singular. [23] The original 3PS / 3PP contrast is lost everywhere except Ingrian. In most cases, both ending variants however still remain in use, with different endings generalized in different varieties. Standard Finnish adopts 1PP -mme (derived from the singular possessed series, with analogical mm based on the verbal inflection), but 1PS -ni, 2PP -nne (derived from the plural possessed series, with regular *nd > nn); and adopts 3PS -nsA in the nominative, illative and instructive (nominative käte-nsä 'her/his hand'), but -Vn (< *-hen) in all other cases (e.g. inessive kädessä-än 'in her/his hand'). New plurality-of-possessed marking has emerged in the Soikkola dialect of Ingrian, suffixing the usual nominative plural marker -t, e.g. venehe-mme-t 'our boats'. [24]
Old Finnish shows two archaic features in the possessive paradigm: the number-of-possessed contrast (singular poikaise-mi 'my son', versus plural luu-ni 'my bones'), and the 2nd person singular ending may attach also to the consonant stem of a nominal, with a non-assibilated ending -ti (the expected regular development before old *s, *t and *h < *š): e.g. rakkaus : rakkaut-ti 'your love', tutkain : tutkain-ti 'your prod' (modern Finnish rakkaute-si, tutkaime-si). [25]
A series of dual possessors has been proposed to account for the two different variants of 3PS, 1PP and 2PP endings; the variants ending in *-n would match with the dual possessor endings in Proto-Samic. [26] This hypothesis has not been generally accepted.
Proto-Finnic inherited at least the following grammatical moods: [17]
The indicative mood distinguished between present (which also functioned as future) and past tense, while the other moods had no tense distinctions. New "perfect" and "pluperfect" tenses had also been formed. These were created using a form of the copula *oldak "to be" and a participle.
There were six forms for each mood, for three persons and two numbers. In addition, there were two more forms. One was a form that is often called "passive" or "fourth person", and indicated an unspecified person. The second was the "connegative" form, which was used together with the negative verb to form negated sentences.
All moods except the imperative shared more or less the same endings: [27] [17] [28]
Singular | Plural | ||
---|---|---|---|
Present | Imperfect | ||
First person | *-n | *-mmek / *-mmak | *-j-mek / *-j-mak |
Second person | *-t | *-ttek / *-ttak | *-j-dek / *-j-dak |
Third person | *-pi (-βi), ∅ | *-βat | *-j-∅ |
Passive | *-tta- + (tense/mood suffix) + *-sen (-hen) | ||
Connegative | *-k |
The first and second person plural endings show evidence (reflected in Savo and Southern Ostrobothnian Finnish and in Karelian) for an earlier present tense marker, assimilated with the following consonant. This is normally reconstructed as *-k- (*-km- > *-mm-, *-kt- > *-tt-), on the assumption of this ending being originally identical with *-k found in the connegative and in the imperative mood.
The variation between forms with *-ek and forms with *-a in the 1st and 2nd person plural reflects a former distinction between the dual and the plural (respectively), although this has not been attested from any Finnic variety. Estonian and Western Finnish continue *-ek, Votic and Eastern Finnic *-a(k).
The third person forms only had an ending in the present indicative. In all other tenses and moods, there was no ending and the singular and plural were identical. The 3rd person singular was entirely unmarked in South Estonian: the Late Proto-Finnic ending had evolved from the participle *-pa during the Middle Proto-Finnic stage, and this innovation had not reached South Estonian, which was already separated.
The imperative had its own set of endings: [17]
Singular | Plural | |
---|---|---|
First person | -ka-da/e-mme/a | |
Second person | -k | -ka-da/e |
Third person | -ka-hen | -ka-hen |
Passive | -tta-ka-hen | |
Connegative | -ga-k |
There is also some evidence of a distinct optative mood, which is preserved in Finnish as -os (second-person singular). It is reconstructed as *-go-s, consisting of the mood suffix *-ko- and the second-person singular ending *-s. This mood suffix gave rise to alternative imperative forms in some languages, such as Finnish third-person singular -koon < *-ko-hen (the plural -koot has -t by analogy) and passive -ttakoon < *-tta-ko-hen.
In addition, there were also several non-finite forms. [29]
Infinitive I | *-tak (-dak) : *-ta- (-da-) |
---|---|
Infinitive II | *-te- (-de-) |
Gerund ("Infinitive III") | *-ma |
Action noun ("Infinitive IV") | *-minen : *-mice- |
Present active participle | *-pa (-ba) |
Present passive participle | *-ttapa (-ttaba) |
Past active participle | *-nut |
Past passive participle | *-ttu |
Proto-Finnic, like its descendants, expressed negation using a special negative verb, functionally highly reminiscent of Indo-European modal auxiliaries. This verb was defective and inflected only in the indicative ("does not", "did not") and the imperative ("do not") moods. The main verb was placed in its special connegative form, and expressed the main mood. The negative verb was also suppletive, having the stem *e- in the indicative and variously *äl-, *al-, *är- in the imperative. This has been partially levelled in Votic and most of Eastern Finnic, which show an imperative stem *el-.
2nd person singular | 3rd person singular | |
---|---|---|
Standard / Northern Estonian | ära | ärgu |
Standard / Western Finnish | älä, äläˣ | älköön, älköhön |
Eastern Finnish | elä | elköön |
Karelian | elä | elkäh, elgäh |
Ingrian | elä, eläg | elkkään |
Votic | elä, älä | elkoo, älkoo |
Veps | ala | algaha, augaha, ougaha |
Livonian | alā | algõ |
Past tense inflection was based on the stem *es-. This is retained as a separate category only in South Estonian and Livonian, but lost in all other Finnic languages. replaced by a construction of present tense of the negative verb, plus past active participle. The distinctive Kodavere dialect of Estonian, however, adopts this and not the present stem as the basic negative verb stem: esin "I didn't", esid "you (sg.) didn't", es "s/he didn't" etc. [30]
Originally, the negative verb may have had participles and other moods as well. However, no clear traces of moods other than the indicative are found in any Finnic language. A remnant of what may be either a present active participle or an archaic third-person singular present form survives in the prefix *epä- "un-, not" (Finnish epä-, Estonian eba-), while a remnant of a 2nd infinitive instructive may survive in dialectal Finnish eten- "without doing". [31]
Negation of non-finite constructions was expressed using the abessive case of the infinitives or participles.
The following is an overview of the more important changes that happened after the Proto-Finnic period.
These changes happened very late in the Proto-Finnic period, but as South Estonian developed somewhat differently, it shows that dialectal diversification was beginning to occur around this time.
In South Estonian, *p and *k assimilate to a following dental obstruent, while *t assimilates to *k, and *čk remains distinct from *tk.
(Pre)-Finnic | South Estonian (Võro) | Other Finnic (North Estonian, Finnish) |
---|---|---|
*čk (*kačku "plague") | *tsk (katsk) | *tk (katk, katku) |
*tk (*itku "cry") | *kk (ikk) | *tk (itk, itku) |
*kt (*koktu "womb") | *tt (kõtt) | stressed *ht (kõht, kohtu) unstressed *tt |
*pt | *tt | stressed *ht unstressed *tt |
*kc (*ükci "one") | *ts (üts') | *ks (üks, yksi) |
*pc (*lapci "child") | *ts (lats') | *ps (laps, lapsi) |
*ks (maksa "liver") | *ss (mass) | *ks (maks, maksa) |
*ps | *ss | *ps |
In all Finnic dialects, original *pt and *kt have the same reflex. It is therefore impossible to distinguish them in reconstruction, unless there is additional internal evidence (in the form of grammatical alternations) or external evidence (from non-Finnic languages).
The non-geminated *c becomes *s generally: Proto-Finnic *veci "water", *cika "pig", *-inen : *-ice- (adjective suffix) > Finnish vesi, sika, -(i)nen : -(i)se-. However, occasionally ts or ds remains in South Estonian: Võro tsiga, -ne : -dse- or -se- (but vesi). The merging of *c and *s often makes it impossible to distinguish the two sounds using Finnic evidence alone, if internal reconstruction is not viable (e. g., from t ~ s alternations from assibilation).
The geminate affricate *cc generally remains, often spelled ⟨ts⟩. In Karelian, Votic and some Finnish dialects, the two grades remain distinguished (in Karelian as ⟨čč⟩ : ⟨č⟩, in Votic as ⟨tts⟩ ~ ⟨ts⟩). In all other Finnic languages the two grades fall together (written in Veps as ⟨c⟩, as ⟨ts⟩ in the others).
In early Finnish, both grades were fronted to interdental θθ : θ, which in most dialects later changed into a variety of other dialect-specific sounds. Examples found are gradation patterns tt : t, ht : h, ht : t, ss : s or non-gradating tt or ht. In early written Finnish, the interdental fricatives are written as ⟨tz⟩ (for both grades) in the earliest records, which in Standard Finnish has led to the spelling pronunciation /ts/ (treated as a consonant cluster and hence no longer subject to consonant gradation).
In the southern Finnic languages, a new back unrounded mid vowel [ɤ] develops from *e in words with back vowel harmony. For example Proto-Finnic *velka "debt" > Estonian võlg, Võro võlg, but > Finnish velka. South Estonian and Votic show this development in all syllables, so that e and õ become a front and back vowel harmony pair. This may have also occurred in the earlier history of north (Standard) Estonian, but vowel harmony was later abandoned, undoing the change if it did occur.
In South Estonian, õ in front of a nasal is raised to a central unrounded vowel [ɨ] (represented orthographically as ⟨y⟩), parallel to the development of the other mid vowels. E.g. Võro ynn', Estonian õnn "luck"; Võro ryngas, Estonian rõngas "ring".
In Estonian and Votic, more rarely Livonian, instances of õ also develop by unrounding of earlier short *o. The detailed history of this change is unclear and shows much variation even between individual dialects of (North) Estonian. [32] The development of *o to õ is the most general in Votic (if recent loanwords from Ingrian, Finnish and Russian are discounted) [33] and in the Kodavere dialect of Estonian. [34] Three main groups can thus be distinguished: [35]
Proto-Finnic | Votic | Estonian | Võro | Livonian | Finnish | Meaning | Unrounding in |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
*hopëda | õpõa | hõbe | hõpõ | õ'bdõ | hopea | "silver" | All Southern Finnic varieties |
*kova | kõva | kõva | kõva | kõvā | kova | "hard" | |
*souta- | sõutaa | sõudma | sõudma | sõidõ | soutaa | "to row" | |
*votta- | võttaa | võtma | võtma | võttõ | ottaa | "to take" | |
*korva | kõrva | kõrv | kõrv | kūora | korva | "ear" | South Estonian, North Estonian and Votic |
*olki | õltši | õlg | olg' | vȯļg | olki | "hay" | |
*pohja | põhja | põhi | põhi, põha | pū'oj | pohja | "bottom" | |
*sormi | sõrmi | sõrm | sõrm' | suoŗm | sormi | "finger" | |
*kota | kõta | koda | koda | kuodā | kota | "house" | Only in Votic and Kodavere Estonian |
*oja | õja | oja | oja, uja | vȯja | oja | "ditch" | |
*oksa | õhsa | oks | oss | oksā | oksa | "branch" | |
*tohti- | tõhtia | tohtima | toht'ma | tū'odõ | tohtia | "to dare" |
A particularly interesting[ editorializing ] example is "to take", which suggests that at least some instances of this change preceded the general Finnic loss of word-initial *v- before rounded vowels, which affected Finnish and the rest of Northern Finnic (which kept a rounded vowel) but not Estonian and the rest of Southern Finnic (which unrounded the vowel). It therefore must have occurred very early, in dialectal Proto-Finnic times.
In a small number of words, Estonian and Votic õ can be additionally found in correspondence to North Finnic a or u. Livonian and South Estonian might align with either side, depending on the word. E.g. [36]
Short final vowels are lost after long syllables (two consonants or a syllable with a long vowel or diphthong) in Veps, partly Ludian, both North and South Estonian, and most Southwestern dialects of Finnish. For example, Proto-Finnic *kakci "two", *neljä "four", *viici "five" > Estonian kaks, neli, viis, Veps kaks', nel'l', viž, Võro katś, nelli, viiś, but > standard Finnish kaksi, neljä, viisi. This change occurred before the loss of final consonants (if any), as vowels that were originally followed by a consonant were not lost. The loss of final *-i leaves phonemic palatalization of the preceding consonant in many languages, on which see below.
Colloquial Finnish loses word-final i under more limited conditions, in particular after s (e.g. kaks "two", viis "5"; inflectional endings such as aamuks "for/to the morning" (translative), talos "your house" (2nd person singular possessive), tulis "would come" (3rd person singular conditional)) as well as word-final a/ä from several inflectional endings (e.g. inessive -s(s), elative -st, adessive -l(l), ablative -lt).
Ingrian also has word-final i-reduction, sometimes obligatory, but more often optional. Thus, *kakci > kaks, *viici > viis, but *suuri > suur, suuri, *peeni > peeni, peen, *keeli > keeli, keel. In the Soikkola, Hevaha and Ylä-Laukaa dialects of Ingrian, the word-final a/ä in inflectional endings often reduce as well (e.g. inessive -s, elative -st, adessive -l, ablative -lt), and a short vowel preceding these endings, unless directly following a short stressed syllable, is elongated. Moreover, the Ala-Laukaa dialect of Ingrian — under influence of Votic — developed a more complex system of vowel reduction in any unstressed syllable unless immediately following a short stressed syllable.
In Livonian, all short final vowels except *a and *ä are lost, thus giving *kakci > kakš as in Estonian, but also *veci "water" > ve'ž, while no vowel was lost in *neljä > nēļa, *kala "fish" > kalā.
Unstressed *o merges into *u in Northern Estonian.
Vowel harmony is lost in Estonian, Livonian and partly Veps, but not South Estonian or Votic. For example, Proto-Finnic *külä "village" > Estonian küla and Livonian kilā, but > Finnish kylä, Veps külä, Votic tšülä, Võro külä. In Finnish and Karelian, vowel harmony was retained and extended to *o as well, creating a new vowel *ö in words with front vowel harmony.
Many languages in the Southern Finnic group, as well as again Veps and Southwestern Finnish, show loss of unstressed vowels in medial syllables. In these languages, vowel length is lost before h early on, while diphthongs are simplified into short vowels.
Palatalized consonants are reintroduced into most varieties other than Western Finnish. The most widespread source is regressive palatalization due to a lost word-final or word-medial *-i (a form of cheshirization), and consonant clusters with *j as a second member. In several varieties, there is also progressive palatalization, where a diphthong ending in *-i and the long vowel *ii causes palatalization of a following consonant.
Estonian, Votic and Finnish do not have general palatalization, and š occurs almost solely in loanwords, most commonly of Russian or German origin. Estonian has positional and phonemic palatalization of medial and final alveolars (except /r/), realized usually as pre-palatalized.
Final *-k was generally lost. It is preserved in some dialects:
Final *-h is widely lost as well. It is preserved:
Traces of both *-k and *-h remain in Finnish, where the consonants became a sandhi effect, assimilating to the initial consonant of the following word and lengthening it. This effect does not occur in all dialects and is not represented orthographically, but is often noted with a superscript "ˣ" in reference works. In Western dialects there was also metathesis of *h, which preserved the original *h along with sandhi lengthening, e.g. Proto-Finnic *mureh "sorrow" > Western Finnish murheˣ (Karelian mureh, Võro murõh/murõq) and Proto-Finnic *veneh "boat" > Western Finnish venheˣ (Karelian/Veps veneh, Võro vineh/vineq). Standard Finnish inconsistently adopts some words in their Western Finnish shape (e.g. murhe; perhe "family", valhe "lie"), some in their Eastern Finnish shape (e.g. vene; huone "room").
Final *-n is lost in most of the South Finnic area (as well as widely in modern-day colloquial Finnish). In Votic this triggers compensatory lengthening of the preceding vowel. The 1st person verbal ending resists the change, and generally remains as -n.
Loss of final consonants followed the loss of final vowels. Thus, vowels followed by a lost consonant were preserved.
The voiced obstruents *b/β, *d/δ and *g/γ that occurred as the weak grades of single plosives were often lost or modified in various ways. The simplest outcomes are in the marginal languages Livonian, Ludic and Veps, where all three are reflected as plain voiced stops b, d and g respectively regardless of environment. The remaining languages show more complex developments.
*b/β develops relatively uniformly:
The development of *d/δ is more diverse:
*g/γ develops somewhat similar to *d/δ, but with several conditional outcomes:
The loss of consonants often created new long vowels and diphthongs, particularly in non-initial syllables. Compare for example:
In all Finnic languages except Finnish, Northern Karelian, the Ala-Laukaa dialect of Ingrian and Votic (partially), the voiceless (strong grade) obstruent consonants *p, *t, *k and *s, are lenited to voiced or lax voiceless obstruents b, d, g, z when occurring between voiced sounds. In Veps and Livonian, these new voiced plosives merge with their weak grade counterparts. In Estonian s remains voiceless and b, d, g are not fully voiced, instead remaining as lax voiceless consonants [b̥], [d̥], [ɡ̊].
In many Finnic languages, long vowels develop into opening diphthongs by raising the onset, or show general raising instead. [37]
The long mid vowels *oo, *öö and *ee become opening diphthongs /uo̯/, /yø̯/, /ie̯/ in Finnish, Karelian, and several marginal dialects of Northern Estonian. In Western Finnish dialects their second component widely becomes more open, producing /uɔ̯/, /yœ̯/, /iɛ̯/ or even /uɑ̯/, /yæ̯/ and either /iæ̯/ or /iɑ̯/ depending on vowel harmony. Diphthongization also occurs in Livonian, but only under certain conditions, and the mid back unrounded long vowel õõ is not affected. In Livonian, the short vowels *o and *e may also diphthongize, leading to a contrast of short uo, ie/wo/, /je/ with long ūo, īe/uːo̯/, /iːe̯/.
In many Ingrian dialects, ee, oo and öö merge with ii/iː/, uu/uː/ and yy/yː/.
In South Estonian, raising only occurs in overlong syllables, and results in long close vowels uu, üü and ii.
In Eastern Finnish and Karelian, the low vowels *aa and *ää also diphthongize, becoming Karelian oa, eä, Savonian ua, iä. In standard Livonian, long *aa of any origin is at a late date generally raised to ǭ/ɔː/.
The diphthong *eü is fully labialized to öü in Northern Finnic and South Estonian. In northern dialects of Veps, new long close vowels are created by the raising of several diphthongs:
North Estonian instead unrounds all diphthongs ending in -ü to -i:
In Savonian Finnish, the second element of all diphthongs is lowered:
In Livonian, *au is labialized to ou, and *äi is palatalized to ei. Following this, the mid diphthongs are smoothed to long vowels under certain conditions:
A variety of languages shows a change of a syllable-final consonant into a vowel. This is not one single change, but several independent developments.
In the Southern Finnic group, *n is lost before *s (< Proto-Finnic *s or *c), with compensatory lengthening of the preceding vowel. For example Proto-Finnic *kanci "lid", *pensas "bush" > Estonian kaas, põõsas, but > Finnish kansi, pensas.
In Western Finnish, stop consonants before a sonorant are vocalized to u. E.g. *kapris "goat", *atra "plough", *kakra "oats" > Finnish kauris, aura, kaura, but > Estonian kaber, ader, kaer, Karelian kapris, atra, kakra. Standard Finnish mostly follows the Western Finnish model. Some notable exceptions include kekri "All Saints' Eve feast", kupla "bubble".
Syllable-final *l is vocalized in Veps at a late date, creating u-final diphthongs in the northern and central dialects, long vowels in the southern.
The Uralic languages, sometimes called the Uralian languages, are spoken predominantly in Europe and North Asia. The Uralic languages with the most native speakers are Hungarian, Finnish, and Estonian. Other languages with speakers above 100,000 are Erzya, Moksha, Mari, Udmurt and Komi spoken in the European parts of the Russian Federation. Still smaller minority languages are Sámi languages of the northern Fennoscandia; other members of the Finnic languages, ranging from Livonian in northern Latvia to Karelian in northwesternmost Russia; and the Samoyedic languages, Mansi and Khanty spoken in Western Siberia.
Unless otherwise noted, statements in this article refer to Standard Finnish, which is based on the dialect spoken in the former Häme Province in central south Finland. Standard Finnish is used by professional speakers, such as reporters and news presenters on television.
Votic or Votian, is a Finnic language spoken by the Votes of Ingria, belonging to the Finnic branch of the Uralic languages. Votic is spoken only in Krakolye and Luzhitsy, two villages in Kingiseppsky District in Leningrad Oblast, Russia. In the 2020–2021 Russian census, 21 people claimed to speak Votic natively, which is an increase from 4 in 2010. Arvo Survo also estimated that around 100 people have knowledge of the language to some degree.
Veps, also known as Vepsian, is a Finnic language from the Uralic language family, that is spoken by Vepsians. The language is written in the Latin script, and is closely related to Finnish and Karelian.
Ingrian, also called Izhorian, is a Finnic language spoken by the Izhorians of Ingria. It has approximately 70 native speakers left, most of whom are elderly.
Consonant gradation is a type of consonant mutation found in some Uralic languages, more specifically in the Finnic, Samic and Samoyedic branches. It originally arose as an allophonic alternation between open and closed syllables, but has become grammaticalised due to changes in the syllable structure of the languages affected.
Northern Sámi or North Sámi is the most widely spoken of all Sámi languages. The area where Northern Sámi is spoken covers the northern parts of Norway, Sweden and Finland. The number of Northern Sámi speakers is estimated to be somewhere between 15,000 and 25,000. About 2,000 of these live in Finland and between 5,000 and 6,000 in Sweden, with the remaining portions being in Norway.
Võro is the language of South Estonia, belonging to the Finnic branch of the Uralic language family. Governmentally, it has been considered a dialect of the Estonian language along with all varieties of South Estonian. However, many linguists consider South Estonian to be an independent Finnic language. It has its own literary standard and efforts have been undertaken to seek official recognition as an indigenous regional language of Estonia. Võro has roughly 75,000 speakers (Võros), mostly in southeastern Estonia, in the eight parishes of the historical Võru County: Karula, Harglõ, Urvastõ, Rõugõ, Kanepi, Põlva, Räpinä and Vahtsõliina. These parishes are currently centred in Võru and Põlva counties, with parts extending into Valga and Tartu counties. Speakers can also be found in the cities of Tallinn and Tartu and the rest of Estonia.
The Finno-Samic languages are a hypothetical subgroup of the Uralic family, and are made up of 22 languages classified into either the Sami languages, which are spoken by the Sami people who inhabit the Sápmi region of northern Fennoscandia, or Finnic languages, which include the major languages Finnish and Estonian. The grouping is not universally recognized as valid.
Proto-Uralic is the unattested reconstructed language ancestral to the modern Uralic language family. The reconstructed language is thought to have been originally spoken in a small area in about 7000–2000 BCE, and then expanded across northern Eurasia, gradually diverging into a dialect continuum and then a language family in the process. The location of the area or Urheimat is not known, and various strongly differing proposals have been advocated, but the vicinity of the Ural Mountains is generally accepted as the most likely.
The Finnic or Baltic Finnic languages constitute a branch of the Uralic language family spoken around the Baltic Sea by the Baltic Finnic peoples. There are around 7 million speakers, who live mainly in Finland and Estonia.
The Savonian dialects are forms of the Finnish language spoken in Savonia and other parts of Eastern Finland. Finnish dialects are grouped broadly into Eastern and Western varieties; Savonian dialects are of the Eastern variety.
Proto-Samoyedic, or Proto-Samoyed, is the reconstructed ancestral language of the Samoyedic languages: Nenets, Enets, Nganasan, Selkup, as well as extinct Kamas and Mator. Samoyedic is one of the principal branches of the Uralic language family, and its ancestor is Proto-Uralic. It has been suggested that Proto-Samoyedic greatly influenced the development of Tocharian, an Indo-European language.
Finnish is a Finnic language of the Uralic language family, spoken by the majority of the population in Finland and by ethnic Finns outside of Finland. Finnish is one of the two official languages of Finland, alongside Swedish. In Sweden, both Finnish and Meänkieli are official minority languages. Kven, which like Meänkieli is mutually intelligible with Finnish, is spoken in the Norwegian counties of Troms and Finnmark by a minority of Finnish descent.
Proto-Sámi is the hypothetical, reconstructed common ancestor of the Sámi languages. It is a descendant of the Proto-Uralic language.
Consonant gradation is the term used for a systematic set of alternations which are widespread in Finnish grammar. These alternations are a form of synchronic lenition. They occur also in other Finnic and Uralic languages; see consonant gradation for a more general overview.
Ingrian is a nearly extinct Finnic language of Russia. The spoken language remains unstandardised, and as such statements below are about the four known dialects of Ingrian and in particular the two extant dialects.
The Ingrian language is a highly endangered language spoken in Ingria, Russia. Ingrian is a Uralic language of the Finnic branch, along with, among others, Finnish and Estonian. Ingrian is an agglutinative language and exhibits both vowel harmony and consonant gradation.
Proto-Karelian, also known as Old Karelian was a language once spoken on the western shore of Lake Ladoga in Karelia, from which the dialects of the Karelian language, Ludic, the Ingrian language, as well as the South Karelian and Savonian dialects of the Finnish language have developed. It was spoken around the 12th and 13th centuries, and the language was likely quite uniform with little regional variance. The Eastern Finnish dialects developed from Proto-Karelian when the language of the inhabitants who had moved to the area around present-day Mikkeli mixed with western, likely Tavastian, speakers of Finnish. The Livvi-Karelian dialect and Ludic developed from the mixture of the old Vepsian language spoken by the Vepsians of the Olonets Isthmus and Proto-Karelian.