Unserdeutsch | |
---|---|
Rabaul Creole German | |
Native to | Papua New Guinea |
Native speakers | 110, almost all in Australia (2016) [1] |
German-based creole | |
Language codes | |
ISO 639-2 | crp |
ISO 639-3 | uln |
Glottolog | unse1236 |
This article should specify the language of its non-English content, using {{ lang }}, {{ transliteration }} for transliterated languages, and {{ IPA }} for phonetic transcriptions, with an appropriate ISO 639 code. Wikipedia's multilingual support templates may also be used - notably uln for Unserdeutsch.(November 2021) |
Unserdeutsch ('Our German'), or Rabaul Creole German, is a German-based creole language that originated in Papua New Guinea as a lingua franca. [2] [3] The substrate language is assumed to be Tok Pisin, [4] while the majority of the lexicon is from German. [5] [6] [7]
German was the language of instruction in Catholic mission schools, which is where the language originated, and children residing in a German-run orphanage later used the language regularly outside of their classrooms. [8] The language developed into a first language for some when these children had families of their own. [9] Oral stories tell a version that Unserdeutsch originated by children sharing stories where they used German vocabulary with Tok Pisin grammar, this change in language is referred to as relexification. [10] [11] [7]
The majority of Unserdeutsch speakers and their families migrated to Australia after Papua New Guinea's independence in 1975. During fieldwork conducted by researchers between 2014 and 2017, there were about 100 speakers found in Australia and around 10 speakers found in Papua New Guinea. [7] [6] The language is no longer learned as a first language. [9]
Most speakers of Unserdeutsch are bilingual; speaking either Standard German, English, Tok Pisin or Kuanua. Most surviving speakers are middle-aged or older, although younger members of the community may comprehend the language. Unserdeutsch is likely a descendant of a pidginised form of Standard German which originated in the Gazelle Peninsula of New Britain during German colonial times among the Catholic mixed-race ( Vunapope ) community. With increased mobility and intermarriage, it has been disappearing in the last few decades.
Unserdeutsch presumably influenced the development of its neighbour, Tok Pisin. Unlike Namibian Black German in Namibia, it is a creole; indeed, it is the only creole that developed from colonial German. [12]
Further, Unserdeutsch is only composed of three characteristics: movement rules, questions and question words, according to Bickerton and his bioprogram hypothesis. [13]
alle | boy-s | raus | schuv | him | boat |
PL | boy-PL | get.out | shove | TR | boat |
German | English | German | Ger/Eng | English | English |
'Everybody get out, shove the boat!' [8]
This article may require cleanup to meet Wikipedia's quality standards. The specific problem is: Contradiction:
|
Unserdeutsch is typical of creole languages [14] in that it reflects the lexicon of one language overlaid upon a substrate grammar – in this case German and Tok Pisin, respectively. [15] Grammatically, Unserdeutsch bears many similarities to L2 varieties of German, suggesting incomplete language acquisition on the part of students in the German-speaking colony. [16] Grammatically, Unserdeutsch morphology adheres to "average" creole characteristics, [17] but because Unserdeutsch was never formally standardized, being used only informally as an in-group register, there is an unusually high degree of grammatical variation among speakers of Unserdeutsch, both between familial groups and individual speakers. [18]
More recently, Australian English has also influenced the language in several ways. Certain syntactic constructions appear to have been borrowed directly from English, [19] including an English-like passive voice construction utilizing a copula. [20] Both of these features are generally rare in creole languages, which indicates an early, pervasive English influence that might have included conscious modeling of English sentence structure. [20]
Unserdeutsch word order is rigidly SVO and lacks the V2 constituent order of subordinate clauses found in Standard German. [21] This tendency extends even to imperative sentences and yes/no questions, which demonstrates a strong substrate influence from Tok Pisin. [21]
Furthermore, even WH-fronting is optional in Unserdeutsch, [16] and these types of interrogatives often come at the end of a sentence, as in Tok Pisin, rather than at the beginning as in Standard German or English. [22] However, some speakers prefer to use a German-modeled sentence pattern in which the interrogative is in head position.
The use of either construction appears variable among speakers of Unserdeutsch. [23]
Unserdeutsch nouns are derived almost exclusively from the German lexicon, [24] but noun morphology is much less synthetic than Standard German. [21] Unserdeutsch nouns do not change to indicate grammatical gender or case, nor is there overt plural marking. [21]
Rather than taking specific singular and plural forms, as in Standard German, Unserdeutsch nouns are pluralized almost exclusively by the pronominal marker alle, unless plurality is already indicated by a numeral or pronoun, in which case it is omitted entirely. [21] [16] [25] Thus, most nouns in Unserdeutsch are pluralized as in the following example:
There are few frequently used nouns which retain Standard German plural forms and are thus double-marked for plural. [21] These irregular forms are retained perhaps owing to frequency of use. [27]
Like German but unlike its substrate language, Tok Pisin, Unserdeutsch uses definite and indefinite articles. However, unlike German, these articles are not inflected for gender or case, and in fact the Standard German articles are merged into a single article de; [28] [29] this is possibly due to influence from English. [30] Articles are normally only used with singular nouns, with the generalized plural marker alle functioning as a definite plural article. [31] [29] The full Unserdeutsch article set is as follows:
Singular | Plural | |
Definite | de | alle |
Indefinite | ein | Ø |
Speakers of Unserdeutsch often omit articles altogether in speech. [31] [32]
Nearly all Unserdeutsch verbs are lexically derived from Standard German, but the Unserdeutsch inflectional system exhibits strong influence from English and Tok Pisin, and is considerably more isolating than Standard German. [33] Many of the distinguishing characteristics of Standard German verbs, such as separable prefixes, second- and third-person stem change and the strong/weak distinction, are not present in Unserdeutsch. [32] [33]
Verbs are generally not inflected according to person or number. [34] Thus, the present tense form of most Unserdeutsch verbs is identical to the Standard German infinitive, which is not conjugated. [21] A small number of verbs take an infinitive form that is instead modeled after the German third-person singular form (geht, 'go') or verb stem (bleib, 'stay'), and some transitive verbs of English or Tok Pisin origin take the Tok Pisin suffix -im (adoptim, 'to adopt'). [21] The following example illustrates verb morphology compared to Standard German:
There is no overt preterite in Unserdeutsch, [16] but a generalized past tense can be indicated through the use of the uninflected verb hat ('have') alongside a highly regularized German participle form, which is constructed by the addition of the prefix ge- to the infinitive. Even verbs borrowed from English or Tok Pisin are prefixed in this way. Thus:
There are few high frequency verbs that exhibit past tense forms closer to their Standard German counterparts, although these forms are lexicalized and non-productive. [35]
There is also a form of weakly grammaticalized future tense marked by the auxiliary wit ('will,' from German wird), which is used with the infinitive [36] in the following way:
Overall, though, the marking of tense is optional, and many speakers of Unserdeutsch do not distinguish between tense-specific forms. [37]
Unusually for a creole language, [34] Unserdeutsch has a copula which is, by contrast to the rest of the verb system, conjugated in present tense. [38] The copular forms are very similar to their Standard German counterparts. In past tense, the copula is simply war; it is not conjugated at all. [34] Thus:
Singular | Plural | |
1st person | i bin | wir bis |
2nd person | du bis | eu seid |
3rd person | er/sie is | die sind |
Singular | Plural | |
1st person | i war | wir war |
2nd person | du war | eu war |
3rd person | er/sie war | die war |
Some speakers preferentially use an uninflected copula, bis, as in:
Notably, whether inflected or not, the copula is frequently deleted in spoken Unserdeutsch. [38]
The Unserdeutsch aspect system is fairly complex. [35] Typical of a creole language, most of these constructions are formed by the addition of preverbal markers. [40]
Progressive or habitual aspect is expressed using the so-called am-construction, formed using the particle am and the infinitive verb. [35] [20]
Habitual past action can be marked using the auxiliary wit (or sometimes wid) along with an infinitive verb, a construction that functions similarly to English past tense would phrases. [35] The wit auxiliary can also express conditional aspect, although this usage is rare. [36]
There is no true imperative in Unserdeutsch. [41] Command statements are formed identically to declarative clauses, and unlike English or German (but similar to Tok Pisin) these constructions retain SVO word order. [42]
It is possible in Unserdeutsch to form directional serial verbs using the so-called komm-construction. This compound verb construction uses the verbs komm and geht, which are reanalyzed as directional markers. [38]
Interestingly, it is also possible to form an English-like passive voice construction [36] using the past tense copula war, the past participle, and the preposition bei.
This passive construction is very rare, and is a clear example of secondary adstrate influence from English. [43] [44]
Verb phrases are negated by the particle ni ('not,' from German nicht), which is usually placed at the beginning of the phrase. In a few idiomatic expressions, the negator is post-verbal, more closely mirroring German negation syntax. [45]
Unserdeutsch uses a hybrid system of personal pronouns, demonstrating heavy influence from both the substrate and lexifier languages. [47]
Singular | Plural | |
1st person | i | wi / uns |
2nd person | du | eu / du |
3rd person | er / sie | die |
Two pronouns, first-person plural wi and third-person singular masculine er, have distinct object forms (uns and ihm, respectively), reflecting Standard German pronominal case marking. [48] It has been suggested by Volker (1982) [25] that, when used in subject position, the first-person plural uns is exclusive, although this is disputed. [49] Volker (1982) also identifies an object form of first-person singular, mi, which is not attested elsewhere. [50]
Use of the second-person plural eu is rare, and du is sometimes substituted. [48] The third-person pronouns mark biological sex only, and there is no equivalent to the German or English neuter forms. [47] There are no formal pronouns in Unserdeutsch, perhaps reflecting non-European cultural conceptions of friendship and acquaintance in the South Pacific. [51] The second-person singular du can be used in place of the Standard German impersonal pronoun man, mirroring English usage. [48] [50] Reflexive and reciprocal pronouns are not used in Unserdeutsch; rarely, these are formed by combining the pronoun with selbs ('self') or direct borrowing of the English "each other." [52] [34] In some cases, personal pronouns can be omitted entirely. [50]
The Unserdeutsch possessive pronouns, while outwardly similar to their Standard German counterparts, do not take case, gender or number endings.
Singular | Plural | |
1st person | mein | Ø |
2nd person | dein | euer |
3rd person | sein / ihre | Ø |
There is no first-person (outside of the name Unserdeutsch) or third-person plural possessive pronoun; in order to express these forms of possession, speakers generally use a prepositional construction with fi. [53]
With very few exceptions, Unserdeutsch adjectives are lexically identical to their German counterparts. [47] As with other parts of speech, these adjectives are considerably more analytical than those of Standard German. [32] Adjectives precede the referent, and show no agreement for case or gender. There is an attributive marker reanalyzed from the Standard German adjective endings [54] into a uniform and invariant -e, which is suffixed to an adjective that precedes a verb. [36] Thus:
A handful of high frequency adjectives, such as gut, retain their Standard German suppletive forms (such as besser and beste). [55]
The comparative forms of most Unserdeutsch adjectives are marked analytically using the particle mehr ('more'). [47] The superlative is formed using the suffix -ste, but without the vowel changes that characterize this process in Standard German. [56] Thus, the comparative and superlative constructions are formed as follows:
A few high frequency adjectives retain their German synthetic comparative and superlative forms. [47]
Aside from possessive pronouns, Unserdeutsch has three constructions that mark possession, each modeled after forms found in one of the dominant languages of the area, Tok Pisin, German and English. [53] [52] [16]
The first of these forms uses a preposition, fi, to express possession, similar to the Tok Pisin bilong-construction.
The second form simply juxtaposes the possessor immediately before the item possessed, as is found is many dialects of German.
The last form is modeled after English, wherein the possessor takes an overt -s genitive suffix.
This English-modeled form is less common than the other two. [57]
In linguistics, a copula /‘kɑpjələ/ is a word or phrase that links the subject of a sentence to a subject complement, such as the word is in the sentence "The sky is blue" or the phrase was not being in the sentence "It was not being cooperative." The word copula derives from the Latin noun for a "link" or "tie" that connects two different things.
A pidgin, or pidgin language, is a grammatically simplified means of communication that develops between two or more groups of people that do not have a language in common: typically, its vocabulary and grammar are limited and often drawn from several languages. It is most commonly employed in situations such as trade, or where both groups speak languages different from the language of the country in which they reside.
English grammar is the set of structural rules of the English language. This includes the structure of words, phrases, clauses, sentences, and whole texts.
Tok Pisin, often referred to by English speakers as New Guinea Pidgin or simply Pidgin, is a creole language spoken throughout Papua New Guinea. It is an official language of Papua New Guinea and the most widely used language in the country. However, in parts of the southern provinces of Western, Gulf, Central, Oro, and Milne Bay, the use of Tok Pisin has a shorter history and is less universal, especially among older people.
In grammar, a future tense is a verb form that generally marks the event described by the verb as not having happened yet, but expected to happen in the future. An example of a future tense form is the French aimera, meaning "will love", derived from the verb aimer ("love"). The "future" expressed by the future tense usually means the future relative to the moment of speaking, although in contexts where relative tense is used it may mean the future relative to some other point in time under consideration.
This article outlines the grammar of the Dutch language, which shares strong similarities with German grammar and also, to a lesser degree, with English grammar.
Verbs constitute one of the main parts of speech in the English language. Like other types of words in the language, English verbs are not heavily inflected. Most combinations of tense, aspect, mood and voice are expressed periphrastically, using constructions with auxiliary verbs.
In some of the Romance languages the copula, the equivalent of the verb to be in English, is relatively complex compared to its counterparts in other languages. A copula is a word that links the subject of a sentence with a predicate. Whereas English has one main copula verb some Romance languages have more complex forms.
Zero copula is a linguistic phenomenon whereby the subject is joined to the predicate without overt marking of this relationship. One can distinguish languages that simply do not have a copula and languages that have a copula that is optional in some contexts.
This article describes the grammar of Afrikaans, a language spoken in South Africa and Namibia which originated from 17th century Dutch.
Belizean Creole is an English-based creole language spoken by the Belizean Creole people. It is closely related to Miskito Coastal Creole, San Andrés-Providencia Creole, and Jamaican Patois.
The Tolai language, or Kuanua, is spoken by the Tolai people of Papua New Guinea, who live on the Gazelle Peninsula in East New Britain Province.
The Mankanya language is spoken by approximately 86,000 people in Guinea-Bissau, Senegal and Gambia primarily belonging to the ethnic group of the same name. It belongs to the Bak branch of the Atlantic–Congo language family.
Papua New Guinea, a sovereign state in Oceania, is the most linguistically diverse country in the world. According to Ethnologue, there are 840 living languages spoken in the country. In 2006, Papua New Guinea Prime Minister Sir Michael Somare stated that "Papua New Guinea has 832 living languages ."
In linguistics, clusivity is a grammatical distinction between inclusive and exclusive first-person pronouns and verbal morphology, also called inclusive "we" and exclusive "we". Inclusive "we" specifically includes the addressee, while exclusive "we" specifically excludes the addressee; in other words, two words that both translate to "we", one meaning "you and I, and possibly someone else", the other meaning "me and some other person or persons, but not you". While imagining that this sort of distinction could be made in other persons is straightforward, in fact the existence of second-person clusivity in natural languages is controversial and not well attested. While clusivity is not a feature of standard English language, it is found in many languages around the world.
Mehek is a Tama language spoken by about 6300 people in a somewhat mountainous area along the southern base of the Torricelli Mountains in northwestern Papua New Guinea. Mehek is spoken in six villages of Sandaun Province: Nuku, Yiminum, Mansuku, Yifkindu, Wilwil, and Kafle. Mehek is most closely related to Pahi, with 51% lexical similarity, and spoken approximately 20 kilometers to the southwest. Mehek is a fairly typical Papuan language, being verb-final, having a relatively simple phonology, and agglutinative morphology. There is very little published information about Mehek. The literacy rate in Tok Pisin, spoken by nearly everyone, is 50-75%. Mehek is not written, so there is no literacy in Mehek. Tok Pisin is primarily used in the schools, with 50% children attending. There is also a sign language used by the large number of deaf people in the Mehek community.
Kiwai is a Papuan language, or languages, of southern Papua New Guinea. Dialects number 1,300 Kope, 700 Gibaio, 1,700 Urama, 700 Arigibi, 3,800 Coast, 1,000 Daru, 4,500 Island, 400 Doumori. Wurm and Hattori (1981) classify Arigibi as a separate language.
Duna is a Papuan language of Papua New Guinea. It may belong to the Trans New Guinea language family and is often further classified as a Duna-Pogaya language, for Bogaya appears to be Duna's closest relative, as evidenced by the similar development of the personal pronouns. Estimates for number of speakers range from 11,000 (1991) to 25,000 (2002).
Qaqet, or Baining, is a non-Austronesian language from the Baining family spoken in East New Britain Province on the island of New Britain, Papua New Guinea.
Hindustani verbs conjugate according to mood, tense, person, number, and gender. Hindustani inflection is markedly simpler in comparison to Sanskrit, from which Hindustani has inherited its verbal conjugation system. Aspect-marking participles in Hindustani mark the aspect. Gender is not distinct in the present tense of the indicative mood, but all the participle forms agree with the gender and number of the subject. Verbs agree with the gender of the subject or the object depending on whether the subject pronoun is in the dative or ergative case or the nominative case.