Southeast Babar language

Last updated
Southeast Babar
Native to Indonesia
Region Maluku
Native speakers
4,500 (2007) [1]
Austronesian
Language codes
ISO 639-3 vbb
Glottolog sout2883

Southeast Babar is an Austronesian language spoken on Babar Island in South Maluku, Indonesia. [2]

Contents

Phonology

Consonants

The following consonant inventory is provided by Steinhauer (2009).

Labial Alveolar Dorsal
Plosive voicelessptk
voicedbd(ɡ)
Nasal mn(ŋ)
Fricative (f)(s)x
Trill r
Lateral l
Approximant wj

Other consonants in Taber's wordlist

A wordlist collected in a 1993 article by Mark Taber records words with extra distinct sounds that are not recorded nor accorded phonemic status in Steinhauer's study.

Consonants exclusively appearing in Taber's wordlist
SoundWords with the soundComments
[utʰ] "dog, banana"Corresponds to word-final /tj/ clusters recorded by Steinhauer, e.g. Steinhauer records "dog" and "banana" as uty.
h[taˈha] "this", [ɛhɛlˈlei] "here", [ɛhɛllɛˈnei] "there", [hlil] "ear"Should have been deleted by regular sound laws. Might correspond to /x/ in some cases (like in "ear", which is recorded by Steinhauer as xlil)
ʔ[ˈtaʔanɛ] "that"Steinhauer records glottal stops as occurring non-phonemically at the starts of otherwise vowel-initial words.
d͡ʒ[d͡ʒai] "what?"

Vowels

Front Central Back
Close i iːu
Mid ɛ ɛːɔ ɔː
Open a aː

Phonotactics

Due to extensive syncope of vowels in both prefixes and original final syllables, Southeast Babar admits a wide variety of consonant clusters both in the onsets and codas of syllables. One stark example of permitted consonant clusters is xweapk "we (inclusive) speak".

Word-final clusters of a consonant followed by /j/ may be optionally subject to epenthesis, with a non-phonemic [ə] being inserted either between the two consonants or after them. Both schwa positions can be used by the same speaker in free variation.

Phonological history

Southeast Babar is notable for its drastic phonetic reshapings of inherited Austronesian vocabulary, with extensive consonant loss, unusual reflexes of surviving consonants, and syncope and apocope of vowels. Many of these changes are outlined and exemplified by Hein Steinhauer as follows: [3]

Changes to consonants

Consonant loss

Elision of consonants in all historical positions is extensive throughout Southeast Babar. Ancestral Proto-Malayo-Polynesian sounds like *p, *k, *q, *R, *h*j and *z were simply lost in Southeast Babar with some exceptions. Examples of consonant deletions include:

  • *q
    • Initially: *qapuR > uir "chalk", *qatəluR > kely "egg".
    • Medially: *ma-qitem > mexm "black", ma-qudip > -mory "(a)live"
    • Finally: *tanaq > kal "soil", *buaq "fruit" > wu, *bunuq > -wuly "to kill".
  • *h
    • *hikan > eːl "fish", *hapuy > uy "fire".
  • *z
    • *zalan > al "road"
  • *k
    • Initially: *kutu > oxy "louse", *kita > ixy "we (inclusive)", *kayu > ay "wood", *kaRat > -ax "to bite"
    • Medially: *i-kau > yow "you (sg. polite), *hikan > *ial > eːl "fish"
    • Finally: *burak > wo-wor "white", *tasik > kat "sea", *utak "brain" > ox "head"
  • *R
    • Initially: *Rumaq > em "house"
    • Medially: *kaRat > -ax "to bite", *maRi > -moy "to come", *daRaq > ra "blood".
    • Finally: *qatəluR > kely "egg".
    • Preserved as r: *qapuR > uir "chalk", *ma-bəʀ(əq)at > berk "heavy"
  • *p
    • Initially: *pitu > wo-exy "7", *punti "banana" > uty
    • Medially: *hapuy > uy "fire", *nipən > *liəl > lil "tooth"
    • Finally: ma-qudip > -mory "(a)live", *malip > -moly "laugh".
    • Preserved as p: *panas > pant "warm"
  • *j
    • *qaləjaw > le "day", *ŋajan > non "name"
    • Preserved as r: *ijuŋ > irl "nose"

Chain shift of *s, *t, and *k

The loss of *k led to a pull chain shift. Subsequently, *t shifted to k except if the *t was directly preceded by original *n; unshifted *t before *n is known from *punti > uty "banana". In turn, k produced from *t lenited to /x/ unless the *k was:

  • Word- or root-initial and followed by a vowel:
    • *tanaq > kal "soil", *ma-takut > mkak "afraid" (root *takut "to be afraid"), *təlu > wo-kely "3"
  • Word-final when protected by a consonant that now immediately precedes the k due to syncope:
    • *laŋit > lalk "sky, heavens", *ma-bəʀ(əq)at > berk "heavy"

Lenited reflexes of *t > k > x include *teliŋa > xlil "ear", *mata > mox "eye", *ma-qitəm > mexm "black", *matay > -moxy "to die", *batu > waxy "stone", and *(h)əpat > wo-ax "4".

Afterwards, *s subsequently underwent fortition to t, with examples including:

  • *sulu > tuly "torch"
  • *susu > -tuty "milk"
  • *asu > uty "dog".
  • *panas > pant "warm"

Merger of *n, and *l

The inherited Malayo-Polynesian nasal consonants *n and merge with each other as *n, followed by a merger of that merged phoneme with *l, generally surfacing as /l/.

  • *ta-kaən > *ka-an > kaːl "you and I eat"
  • *teliŋa > xlil "ear"
  • *nipən > *lipəl > *liəl > lil "tooth"
  • *laŋit > lalk "sky, heavens".

Post-merger /l/ subsequently underwent an inverse development to n when adjacent to t either originating from *s or borrowed from another language. This circular development leads to roots and inflectional affixes to synchronically contain alternations between l and n.

  • English bottle > Indonesian botol > Southeast Babar potn
  • *panas > *palt > pant "hot" (intermediate *l preserved in reduplicated intensive derivative pal-pant "very hot")

A few cases of n failing to merge with l are known, mainly in monosyllabic words where, due to medial consonant deletion and resulting vowel coalescence, there is simultaneously there is one n in the onset and a second n in the coda.

  • *ŋajan > non "name"
  • *na-kaən > *na-an > noːn "(s)he eats"

Due to *nipən "tooth" surfacing as lil and not **nin, Steinhauer suggests that the loss of *p occurred after the loss of *j and *k.

Other consonants

*b and *d generally become w and r, merging with original *w and *r.

  • Original *w and *r: *waiR > wey "water", *burak > wo-wor "white"
  • Originally *b and *d: *batu > waxy "stone", *babuy > wawy "pig", *balik > -waly "to turn", *daləm > ralm "inside", *dua > ru "2"
  • Irregularly unchanged *b: *ma-bəRat > berk "heavy"

Changes to vowels

Reduction and loss of final vowels

All word-final and *a, whether inherited as word-final or secondarily word-final due to the loss of a following consonant, are deleted in Southeast Babar.

  • Originally word-final: *lima > lim "hand", *mata > mox "eye", *dua > wu-ru "2"
  • Secondarily word-final: *tanaq > *kala > kal "land", *dəŋəR > rel "to hear"

Word-final high vowels *-u and *-i generally reduce to the glide /j/. Like with *a, the loss of a following consonant will make the high vowel count as word-final for the purposes of this reduction.

  • From *u: *batu > waxy "stone", *sulu > tuly "torch", *baqəRu > wa-way "new"
  • From *i: *malip > -moly "to laugh", *balik > -waly "to turn", *waiR > wey "water"
  • From *-ay: *matay > -moxy "to die"

Syncope of vowels between two consonants

Vowels often faced deletion in final syllables between two surviving consonants. Examples cited by Steinhauer include laŋit > lalk "heavens", *daləm > ralm "inside", *inum > *imun > -iml "to drink", *panas > pant "warm", and *matay > -moxy "to die".

Reflexes of *a

*a turns into u if it becomes the first phoneme of a word at any point in its evolution to Southeast Babar.

  • Original word-initial *a: *asu > uty "dog", *ama > um "father"
  • Secondarily word-initial *a: *hapuy > uy "fire", *qabu > uwy "ash"
  • Exceptions: *zalan > al "road"

*a surfaces as o after nasal consonants. However, this change is blocked in the first-person singular of verbs, where a /j/ is infixed in the verbal root between the nasal and the vowel. Contrast:

  • *ku-malip > i-myaly "I laugh" (rounding blocked by infixed -y-)
  • *na-malip > l-moly "(s)he laughs" (with rounding after nasal).

If an *a is either not word-initial, not preceded by a nasal, or not subject to apocope or syncope, it will remain as a.

  • Not word initial nor following a nasal: *daləm > ralm "inside", *babuy > wawy "pig"
  • Not syncopated nor apocopated: *panas > pant "warm"

Reflexes of other vowels

*u remained as u unless before a syllable containing a non-high vowel, which lowered it to o. The difference in reflexes can be starkly contrasted with *buaq "fruit", which became the numeral prefix wu- or wo- depending on the vowel of the following numeral.

  • With no lowering: wu-ru "2", wu-lim "5"
  • With lowering: wo-kely "3", wo-ax "4", wo-lem "6"

*u also lowered to o before /x/, such as in *mutaq > mox "to vomit" and *kutu > oxy "louse".

is often deleted in various places in trisyllabic words. However, if an survives these deletions, it is reflected as e.

  • Deleted: *təliŋa > xlil "ear", *baqəRu > wa-way "new"
  • Surfaces as e: *dəŋəR > -rel "to hear", *ənəm > -lem "6", *təlu > -kely "3"

Morphology

Morphophonological processes

Some conditioned sound changes have led to phonetic mutations of morphemes when subject to morphological processes, including affixation and reduplication.

Change of l to n before t

Southeast Babar morphemes ending in l generally change the l to n whenever it precedes a t.

  • l- (3sg. verbal prefix) + -tol "to see" + tel (perfect particle) > nton tel "(s)he has seen"
  • lewal "language" + toːl "our (inclusive)" > lewan toːl "our language"
  • -tol "ill" > -tontol "very ill" (reduplicated)

Glide metathesis and insertion

On verbs whose roots end in the glide -y, the glide and the first consonant of the following morpheme undergo metathesis. For instance, l-moxy "(s)he dies" becomes lmox tyel "(s)he has died", with the y from the verb root and the t of the particle tel switching places.

The metathesis process also occurs when a morpheme ending in -y is reduplicated, such as -kary "to work" reduplicating to -karkyary "to be working".

The 1st-person singular, 2nd-person singular, and 2nd-person plural also trigger the insertion of a glide after the first consonant of the following verb stem; the presence of glide insertion is governed by the same restrictions as the application of glide metathesis.

Glide metathesis and insertion have some restrictions on their application.

  • The morpheme the glide might move to or be inserted at cannot have a high vowel after its first consonant.
  • The initial consonant of the next morpheme cannot be w.

If metathesis is blocked by one of these two restrictions, the glide is simply lost unless a vowel follows the glide.

If an original Malayo-Polynesian *a was rounded to o after a nasal in a base morpheme, the vowel is reverted to a if the nasal is subject to the glide metathesis or insertion process. The conjugation of -moly "to laugh" demonstrates this:

  • Unreduplicated l-moly "(s)he laughs" and i-myaly "I laugh" (with glide insertion)
  • Reduplicated l-molmyaly "(s)he keeps laughing" and i-myalmyaly "I keep laughing"

Verb conjugation

Southeast Babar verbs are conjugated for three grammatical persons (first, second, and third persons) and two grammatical numbers (singular and plural). First-person plural conjugation also distinguishes clusivity, with exclusive 1st-person plural excluding the addressee but the inclusive 1st-person plural including them. In addition, verbs can additionally inflect for at least three known grammatical aspects, namely perfect, progressive aspect, and inchoative aspect.

Person-number prefixes

Southeast Babar conjugates verbs for person and number via a series of prefixes attached to verb stems, either to an unmarked stem or the progressive stem.

There are two basic classes of person-number prefixes in the language. One class has the person-number prefixes all contain a vowel, and the other class where most of the prefixes do not. The prefixes for each class are as follows:

Southeast Babar person-number verbal markers
Person and numberVocalic prefixesConsonantal prefixesProto-forms [note 1]
1st sg.o-i- [note 2] *ku-
2nd sg.mo-m- [note 2] *mu-
3rd sg.le-l- [note 3] *na-
1st pl. inclusiveke-x-*ta-
1st pl. exclusiveme-m-*ma-
2nd pl.mi-m- [note 2] *mi-
3rd pl.te-t-*sida-
  1. Steinhauer posits that these proto-forms can only account directly for the consonantal prefixes, and finds it more difficult to envision how the vocalic prefixes can descend from these.
  2. 1 2 3 Inserts a glide after a following consonant if this consonant is not w nor is followed by a high vowel.
  3. Surfaces as n- in verbs whose stem begins with t, like n-tol "(s)he sees", and also in the irregular verb n-oːn "(s)he eats".

Verbal aspect marking

A particle tel follows the conjugated verb to mark the perfect aspect. Another postverbal particle kay marks the inchoative aspect. The two particles can stack together to form an "inchoative perfect", so to speak. Steinbauer demonstrates the two particles with the following examples.

  • lmoxy "(s)he dies"
  • lmox tyel "(s)he has died"
  • lmok kyay tyel "(s)he has already died"

Progressive aspect is expressed by a special verbal stem for each verb formed via reduplication of the verb root.

  • lkary "(s)he works" > lkarkyary "(s)he is working"
  • lmoly "(s)he laughs" > lmolmyaly "(s)he keeps laughing".
  • Proto-Malayo-Polynesian *kaən > reduplicated *kakan > -kkol "is eating" (present -o:l ~ -a:l-)

Pronouns

The basic personal pronouns in Southeast Babar are:

Southeast Babar pronouns
Person and numberPronoun
1st sg.um
2nd sg.(y)ow
3rd sg.iy
1st pl. inclusiveixy
1st pl. exclusiveam
2nd pl.miy
3rd pl.ity

Possessive markers

There are also at least two series of possessive markers, one series ending in u and another one ending in oːl. They all are stressed when attached to a possessed noun. Possessive markers documented by Steinhauer are first-person singular u and oːl, second-person singular mu and moːl, and first-person inclusive plural toːl.

Possessive markers follow the noun that is possessed. Examples of this behaviour include:

Numerals

Cardinal numerals from 2 to 9 have an obligatory prefix wu-/wo- that is derived from *buaq "fruit", which on the way to Southeast Babar evolved into a classifier and then a prefix.

The cardinals from 2-7 are given by Steinhauer as wu-ru "2", wo-kely "3", wo-ax "4", wu-lim "5", wo-lem "6", and wo-exy "7". Other numerals are only recorded in Taber's wordlist, including metl "1", wo-ka "8", wu-si "9", and wu-ki "10".

Related Research Articles

The Ho-Chunk language, also known as Winnebago, is the language of the Ho-Chunk people of the Ho-Chunk Nation of Wisconsin and Winnebago Tribe of Nebraska. The language is part of the Siouan language family and is closely related to other Chiwere Siouan dialects, including those of the Iowa, Missouria, and Otoe.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Crow language</span> Missouri Valley Siouan language of Montana, US

Crow is a Missouri Valley Siouan language spoken primarily by the Crow Nation in present-day southeastern Montana. The word Apsáalooke translates to "Children of the Large Beaked Bird", which was later incorrectly translated into English as 'Crow'. It is one of the larger populations of American Indian languages with 4,160 speakers according to the 2015 US Census.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Tonkawa language</span> Native American language

The Tonkawa language was spoken in Oklahoma, Texas, and New Mexico by the Tonkawa people. A language isolate, with no known related languages, Tonkawa has not had L1 speakers since the mid 1900s. Most Tonkawa people now only speak English, but revitalization is underway.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Tuscarora language</span> Extinct language

Tuscarora, sometimes called Skarò˙rə̨ˀ, was the Iroquoian language of the Tuscarora people, spoken in southern Ontario, Canada, North Carolina and northwestern New York around Niagara Falls, in the United States, before becoming extinct in late 2020. The historic homeland of the Tuscarora was in eastern North Carolina, in and around the Goldsboro, Kinston, and Smithfield areas.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Eastern Pomo language</span> Pomoan language

Eastern Pomo, also known as Clear Lake Pomo, is a nearly extinct Pomoan language spoken around Clear Lake in Lake County, California by one of the Pomo peoples.

Tübatulabal is an Uto-Aztecan language, traditionally spoken in Kern County, California, United States. It is the traditional language of the Tübatulabal, who still speak the traditional language in addition to English. The language originally had three main dialects: Bakalanchi, Pakanapul and Palegawan.

Pohnpeian is a Micronesian language spoken as the indigenous language of the island of Pohnpei in the Caroline Islands. Pohnpeian has approximately 30,000 (estimated) native speakers living in Pohnpei and its outlying atolls and islands with another 10,000-15,000 (estimated) living off island in parts of the US mainland, Hawaii and Guam. It is the second-most widely spoken native language of the Federated States of Micronesia the first being Chuukese.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Aguaruna language</span> Chicham language of Peru

Aguaruna is an indigenous American language of the Chicham family spoken by the Aguaruna people in Northern Peru. According to Ethnologue, based on the 2007 Census, 53,400 people out of the 55,700 ethnic group speak Aguaruna, making up almost the entire population. It is used vigorously in all domains of life, both written and oral. It is written with the Latin script. The literacy rate in Aguaruna is 60-90%. However, there are few monolingual speakers today; nearly all speakers also speak Spanish. The school system begins with Aguaruna, and as the students progress, Spanish is gradually added. There is a positive outlook and connotation in regard to bilingualism. 50 to 75% of the Aguaruna population are literate in Spanish. A modest dictionary of the language has been published.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Nukak language</span> Endangered indigenous language of Colombia

The Nukak language is a language of uncertain classification, perhaps part of the macrofamily Puinave-Maku. It is very closely related to Kakwa.

Taos is a Tanoan language spoken by several hundred people in New Mexico, in the United States. The main description of its phonology was contributed by George L. Trager in a (pre-generative) structuralist framework. Earlier considerations of the phonetics-phonology were by John P. Harrington and Jaime de Angulo. Trager's first account was in Trager (1946) based on fieldwork 1935-1937, which was then substantially revised in Trager (1948). The description below takes Trager (1946) as the main point of departure and notes where this differs from the analysis of Trager (1948). Harrington's description is more similar to Trager (1946). Certain comments from a generative perspective are noted in a comparative work Hale (1967).

Leti is an Austronesian language spoken on the island of Leti in Maluku. Although it shares much vocabulary with the neighboring Luang language, it is marginally mutually intelligible.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Paya language</span> Chibchan language of Honduras

Pech or Pesh is a Chibchan language spoken in Honduras. It was formerly known as Paya, and continues to be referred to in this manner by several sources, though there are negative connotations associated with this term. It has also been referred to as Seco. There are 300 speakers according to Yasugi (2007). It is spoken near the north-central coast of Honduras, in the Dulce Nombre de Culmí municipality of Olancho Department.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Enggano language</span> Austronesian language spoken in Indonesia

The Enggano language, or Engganese, is an Austronesian language spoken on Enggano Island off the southwestern coast of Sumatra, Indonesia.

Warndarrang (waɳʈaraŋ), also spelt Warndarang, Wanderang, Wandaran, and other variants is an extinct Aboriginal Australian language in the Arnhem family, formerly spoken by the Warndarrang people in southern Arnhem Land, along the Gulf of Carpentaria. The last speaker was Isaac Joshua, who died in 1974, while working with the linguist Jeffrey Heath.

Maia is a Papuan language spoken in the Madang Province of Papua New Guinea, and is a member of the Trans-New Guinea language family. It has a language endangerment status of 6a, which means that it is a vigorous and sustainable language spoken by all generations. According to a 2000 census, there are approximately 4,500 living speakers of the language, who are split between twenty-two villages in the Almani district of the Bogia sub-district.

Wandala, also known as Mandara or Mura', is a language in the Chadic branch of the Afro-Asiatic language family, spoken in Cameroon and Nigeria.

Wamesa is an Austronesian language of Indonesian New Guinea, spoken across the neck of the Doberai Peninsula or Bird's Head. There are currently 5,000–8,000 speakers. While it was historically used as a lingua franca, it is currently considered an under-documented, endangered language. This means that fewer and fewer children have an active command of Wamesa. Instead, Papuan Malay has become increasingly dominant in the area.

Maskelynes, or Kuliviu (Uliveo), is an Oceanic language spoken on the Maskelyne Islands off south Malekula, Vanuatu.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Matlatzinca language</span> Endangered Oto-Manguean language of Mexico

Matlatzinca, or more specifically San Francisco Matlatzinca, is an endangered Oto-Manguean language of Western Central Mexico.[3] The name of the language in the language itself is pjiekak'joo.[4] The term "Matlatzinca" comes from the town's name in Nahuatl, meaning "the lords of the network." At one point, the Matlatzinca groups were called "pirindas," meaning "those in the middle."[5]

The phonology of Māori is typical for a Polynesian language, with its phonetic inventory being one of the smallest in the world with considerable variation in realisation. The Māori language retains the Proto-Polynesian syllable structure: (C)V(V ), with no closed syllables. The stress pattern is unpredictable, unlike in many other Polynesian languages.

References

  1. Southeast Babar at Ethnologue (18th ed., 2015) (subscription required)
  2. Taber, Mark (1993). "Toward a better understanding of the Indigenous Languages of Southwestern Maluku". Oceanic Linguistics. 32 (2): 389–441. JSTOR   3623199.
  3. 1 2 Steinhauer, Hein (2009). The sounds of Southeast Babar. Adelaar, K. Alexander and Pawley, Andrew (eds.), Austronesian historical linguistics and culture history: a festschrift for Robert Blust: Canberra: Research School of Pacific and Asian Studies, Australian National University. pp. 399–409.