Autistic masking, also referred to as camouflaging, is the conscious or subconscious suppression of autistic behaviors and compensation of difficulties in social interaction by autistic people, with the goal of being perceived as neurotypical. [1] [2] Masking behavior is a learned coping strategy [3] [4] that can be successful from the perspective of some autistic people (e.g., in reducing the chances of being stigmatized [5] ), but can also lead to adverse mental health outcomes. [1] [6]
Autistic people have cited social acceptance, the need to get a job, and the avoidance of ostracism or verbal or physical abuse as reasons for masking. [7]
The process of consciously reducing masking tendencies or not masking in some contexts, which some autistic people see as a desirable goal, is referred to as unmasking. [2] [8] [9] Motivations for unmasking include no longer hiding one's true identity and avoiding adverse mental health outcomes. [2] [10] [8]
There is no universally agreed-upon terminology for the concept. [1] [6] : 16–17 [11] While some use the terms masking and camouflaging synonymously, [1] [2] [6] : 16–17 others distinguish between masking (the suppression of behaviors) and compensation (of social difficulties) as the two main forms of camouflaging. [1] [3] [12] Among autistic people, masking is the most commonly used umbrella term. [6] : 16 [10] Autistic researcher Wenn Lawson has proposed that adaptive morphing is a more fitting term. [13]
I camouflage by putting on a character… I treat my clothes rather like costumes, and certain items of clothing help me to uphold certain personality characteristics of which character I am on that occasion. I have a repertoire of roles for: cafe work, bar work, uni, various groups of friends, etc. They are all me at the core, but they are edited versions of me, designed to not stand out for the "wrong" reasons.
— (Female, 22), [3]
Typical examples of autistic masking include the suppression of stimming (in general or in public [5] ) and reactions to sensory overload. [3] To compensate difficulties in social interaction with neurotypical peers, autistic people might maintain eye contact despite discomfort [14] [15] or mirror the body language and tone of others. [1] [2] [3] [16]
Autistic people with conversational difficulties may also use more complex strategies such as scripting a conversation outline, developing conscious "rules" for conversations, carefully monitoring if these are being followed. [3] Many autistic people learn conversational rules and social behaviors by watching television shows and other media and by observing and mimicking a character's behavior. [16] Masking may also include refraining from talking about passionate interests. [2] [3]
Masking requires an exceptional effort [5] [3] and is a main cause of autistic burnout. [17] [18] It is linked with adverse mental health outcomes [19] [20] [21] [22] such as stress, [23] anxiety, depression, and other psychological disorders, [23] loss of identity, [23] and suicidality. [24] [25] [26] According to a recent meta analysis, the association between masking and depression, general anxiety, and social anxiety appear consistent across different age groups (children, adolescents, and adults). [22] Some studies find that compensation strategies are seen as contributing to leading a successful life. [5] [1] [12] Since many studies on masking focus on autistic adolescents or adults without cognitive impairments, the generalizability of such findings across the autism spectrum is uncertain. [1]
Masking may conceal the person's need for support. [7] It can complicate a diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder (ASD), for example, underdiagnosis for females, particularly past childhood, as relevant symptoms are suppressed or compensated for. [27] [28] : 60–62 The diagnostic criteria for ASD in the DSM-5 published in 2013 explicitly state that while symptoms "must be present in the early developmental period", these "may be masked by learned strategies in later life", allowing for a diagnosis even if autistic behaviors and difficulties are successfully masked. [28] : 57 [29] Addition of such a formulation was proposed to the workgroup drafting the criteria by representatives of the Autistic Self Advocacy Network including Ari Ne'eman and Steven Kapp. [29] The diagnostic criteria for ASD in the ICD-11 (2022) contain a similar provision. [30]
It has been hypothesized that masking may play an important role in explaining why autistic women, AFAB people and non-binary persons [2] are significantly less often recognized and diagnosed as autistic compared to men. [2] [3] [16] This hypothesis was put forward by Lorna Wing as early as 1981 [6] : 20 [31] : 134 and is recognized in the DSM-5-TR published in 2022. [28] : 65
While masking was written about and discussed among autistic people, it has only become a focus of academic research since the 2010s. [6] : 18 The Camouflaging Autistic Traits Questionnaire (CAT-Q), the first self-report measure for camouflaging, was published in 2018. [32] [33] Across 25 items, it measures the extent to which a person utilizes strategies to actively compensate for difficulties in social situations (Compensation, 9 items), uses strategies to hide autistic characteristics or portray a non-autistic persona (Masking, 8 items), and employs strategies to fit in with others in social situations (Assimilation, 8 items). [32] Other researchers have criticized the use of self-report measures, arguing that self-report may exclude understudied groups within autism, such as individuals with linguistic disabilities. [34]
In light of rising awareness of the adverse mental health outcomes of masking and insight into the double empathy problem, therapies and interventions with implicit or explicit targets of instilling neurotypical behavior and suppressing autistic traits that can be adaptive in autistic people are controversial and often criticized by some researchers, neurodiversity proponents, and autistic self-advocates from the autism rights movement. [35] [36] [37] Some autistic adults who experienced applied behavior analysis therapy as children describe being forced to behave like neurotypical peers with detrimental effects on their mental and overall well-being. [38] [39] [40] In response to these concerns and accounts regarding risks of harm, some forms of ABA interventions have been reforming to mitigate risks of encouraging masking. [35] [41] [42] Additionally, some researchers and/or practitioners have called for reforms in some other forms of interventions, such as social skills training, speech-language therapy, and occupational therapy to mitigate such risks, with some therapists implementing such reforms. [43] [44] [45] [46] [47]
There are some research studies centered around the experiences of masking by comparing different groups of neurotypes. In 2021, researchers conducted an online survey comparing masking experiences between autistic, non-autistic neurodivergent, and neurotypical groups. [5] They found that the behavior of masking is shared across all types of people, but some aspects of masking are more specific to autism, such as sensory suppression and suppression of stimming. Researchers also recreated this study in a workplace context and examined workplace masking experiences for autistic, non-autistic, and neurotypical adults in the UK. They identified eight emerging themes from the survey and reported large overlap among three groups such as reasonings and perceptions of benefits and drawbacks: both neurodivergent and neurotypical people adopted masking strategies to achieve social goals, indicating that masking is more like a common rather exclusive experience. [34]
There has also been qualitative research focused on the autistic experiences of masking. A study in 2022 conducted semi-structured interviews with twenty autistic teenagers and observed that masking is associated with mental health (but not necessarily in linear relationship) and how both of them are affected by social and environmental factors. Researchers stressed the need to approach masking, authenticity, and mental health through the context of people's identities and the environment, providing implications for diagnostic services and interventions. [48] There have been comparisons between masking and passing. [34]
In addition to masking, researchers investigated the "authenticity" autistic people feel while socializing and observed that supportive environments, such as being around people who accept and understand them, can lead to self-awareness and create more positive socializing experiences than camouflaging. [49] However, this doesn't imply that autistic "masking" is equivalent to non-authenticity. Researchers proposed that the focus should not be encouraging masking but promoting autistic authenticity, creating a more positive self-image and better mental health. [48]
Asperger syndrome (AS), also known as Asperger's syndrome or Asperger's, is a diagnostic label that has been used to describe a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by significant difficulties in social interaction and nonverbal communication, along with restricted, repetitive patterns of behavior and interests. Asperger syndrome has been merged with other conditions into autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and is no longer a diagnosis in the WHO's ICD-11 or the APA's DSM-5-TR. It was considered milder than other diagnoses which were merged into ASD due to relatively unimpaired spoken language and intelligence.
Diagnoses of autism have become more frequent since the 1980s, which has led to various controversies about both the cause of autism and the nature of the diagnoses themselves. Whether autism has mainly a genetic or developmental cause, and the degree of coincidence between autism and intellectual disability, are all matters of current scientific controversy as well as inquiry. There is also more sociopolitical debate as to whether autism should be considered a disability on its own.
The neurodiversity paradigm is a framework for understanding human brain function that considers the diversity within sensory processing, motor abilities, social comfort, cognition, and focus as neurobiological differences. This diversity falls on a spectrum of neurocognitive differences. The neurodiversity paradigm argues that diversity in neurocognition is part of humanity and that some neurodivergences generally classified as disorders, such as autism, are differences with strengths and weaknesses as well as disabilities that are not necessarily pathological. Neurotypical individuals are those who fall within the average range of functioning and thinking.
The autism rights movement, also known as the autistic acceptance movement, is a social movement allied with the disability rights movement. It emphasizes the neurodiversity paradigm, viewing autism as a set of naturally occurring variations in human cognition, a disability with both strengths and weaknesses, rather than as a disease to be cured or a medical disorder. This paradigm contradicts and diverges from the medical model of disability, without opposing all aspects of it.
Ole Ivar Løvaas was a Norwegian-American clinical psychologist and professor at the University of California, Los Angeles. He is most well known for his research on what is now called applied behavior analysis (ABA) to teach autistic children through prompts, modeling, and positive reinforcement. His application of the science was also noted for its use of aversives (punishment) to reduce undesired behavior, which are no longer supported as a part of most ABA treatment plans.
High-functioning autism (HFA) was historically an autism classification to describe a person who exhibited no intellectual disability but otherwise showed autistic traits, such as difficulty in social interaction and communication. The term was often applied to verbal autistic people of at least average intelligence. However, many in medical and autistic communities have called to stop using the term, finding it simplistic and unindicative of the difficulties some autistic people face.
In psychology and sociology, masking, also known as social camouflaging, is a defensive behavior in which an individual conceals their natural personality or behavior in response to social pressure, abuse, or harassment. Masking can be strongly influenced by environmental factors such as authoritarian parents, social rejection, and emotional, physical, or sexual abuse.
Autism therapies include a wide variety of therapies that help people with autism, or their families. Such methods of therapy seek to aid autistic people in dealing with difficulties and increase their functional independence.
Self-stimulatory behavior, also known as "stimming" and self-stimulation, is the repetition of physical movements, sounds, words, moving objects, or other behaviors. Stimming is a type of restricted and repetitive behavior (RRB). Such behaviors are found to some degree in all people, but is especially intense and frequent in those with developmental disabilities, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) or autism. People diagnosed with sensory processing disorder are also known to potentially exhibit more stimming behaviors.
The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ) is a questionnaire published in 2001 by Simon Baron-Cohen and his colleagues at the Autism Research Centre in Cambridge, UK. Consisting of fifty questions, it aims to investigate whether adults of average intelligence have symptoms of autism spectrum conditions. More recently, versions of the AQ for children and adolescents have also been published.
Classic autism, also known as childhood autism, autistic disorder, or Kanner's syndrome, is a formerly diagnosed neurodevelopmental disorder first described by Leo Kanner in 1943. It is characterized by atypical and impaired development in social interaction and communication as well as restricted, repetitive behaviors, activities, and interests. These symptoms first appear in early childhood and persist throughout life.
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD), or simply autism, is a neurodevelopmental disorder "characterized by persistent deficits in social communication and social interaction across multiple contexts" and "restricted, repetitive patterns of behavior, interests, or activities". Sensory abnormalities are also included in the diagnostic manuals. Common associated traits such as motor coordination impairment are typical of the condition but not required for diagnosis. A formal diagnosis requires that symptoms cause significant impairment in multiple functional domains; in addition, the symptoms must be atypical or excessive for the person's age and sociocultural context.
Sex and gender differences in autism exist regarding prevalence, presentation, and diagnosis.
Discrimination against autistic people involves any form of discrimination, persecution, or oppression against people who are autistic.
Nonverbal autism, also called nonspeaking autism, is a subset of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) where the person does not learn how to speak.
Marian Diamond Sigman (1941–2012) was a developmental and child clinical psychologist known for her research on autism spectrum disorder (ASD). At the time of her death, she was Professor Emeritus of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences and Psychology at the University of California, Los Angeles (UCLA).
Current research indicates that autistic people have higher rates of LGBTQ identities and feelings than the general population. A variety of explanations for the increased prevalence of LGBTQ identities have been proposed, such as prenatal hormonal exposure, which has been linked with sexual orientation, gender dysphoria and autism. Alternatively, autistic people may be less reliant on social norms and thus are more open about their orientation or gender identity. A narrative review published in 2016 stated that while various hypotheses have been proposed for an association between autism and gender dysphoria, they lack strong evidence.
The theory of the double empathy problem is a psychological and sociological theory first coined in 2012 by Damian Milton, an autistic autism researcher. This theory proposes that many of the difficulties autistic individuals face when socializing with non-autistic individuals are due, in part, to a lack of mutual understanding between the two groups, meaning that most autistic people struggle to understand and empathize with non-autistic people, whereas most non-autistic people also struggle to understand and empathize with autistic people. This lack of mutual understanding may stem from bidirectional differences in dispositions, and experiences between autistic and non-autistic individuals, as opposed to always being an inherent deficit.
Autistic burnout is a prolonged state of intense fatigue, decreased executive functioning or life skills, and increased sensory processing sensitivity experienced by autistic people. Autistic burnout is thought to be caused by stress arising from masking or living in a neurotypical environment that is not autism-friendly. While not formally recognized as a medical condition, autistic burnout has become increasingly recognized within the autistic community and has gained attention among researchers, clinicians, and autistic people themselves as a significant aspect of the autistic experience.
The diagnosis of autism is based on a person's reported and directly observed behavior. There are no known biomarkers for autism spectrum conditions that allow for a conclusive diagnosis.