Autistic masking

Last updated

Autistic masking, also referred to as camouflaging or neurodivergent masking, is the conscious or subconscious suppression of autistic behaviors and compensation of difficulties in social interaction by autistic people with the goal of being perceived as neurotypical. [1] [2] Masking is a learned coping strategy [3] [4] that can be successful from the perspective of autistic people, but can also lead to adverse mental health outcomes. [1] [5]

Contents

Terminology

There is no universally agreed-upon terminology for the concept. [1] [5] :16–17 [6] While some use the terms masking and camouflaging synonymously, [1] [2] [5] :16–17 others distinguish between masking (the suppression of behaviors) and compensation (of social difficulties) as the two main forms of camouflaging. [1] [3] [7] Among autistic people, masking is the most commonly used umbrella term. [5] :16 [8]

The process of consciously giving up masking, which some autistic people see as a desirable goal, is referred to as unmasking. [2] [9] [10] Motivations for unmasking include no longer hiding one's true identity and avoiding adverse mental health outcomes. [2] [8] [9]

Forms

Typical examples of autistic masking include the suppression of stimming and reactions to sensory overload. [3] To compensate difficulties in social interaction with neurotypical peers, autistic people might maintain eye contact despite discomfort or mirror the body language and tone of others. [1] [2] [3] [11]

Autistic people with conversational difficulties may also use more complex strategies such as scripting a conversation outline, developing conscious "rules" for conversations, carefully monitoring if these are being followed. [3] Many autistic people learn conversational rules and social behaviors by watching television shows and other media and by observing and mimicking a character's behavior. [11] Masking may also include refraining from talking about passionate interests. [2] [3]

Autistic people have cited social acceptance, the need to get a job, and the avoidance of ostracism or verbal or physical abuse as reasons for masking. [12]

Consequences

Masking requires an exceptional effort [3] [13] and is a main cause of autistic burnout. [14] [15] It is linked with adverse mental health outcomes [16] [17] such as stress, [18] anxiety, depression, and other psychological disorders, [18] loss of identity, [18] and suicidality. [19] [20] [21] Some studies find that compensation strategies are seen as contributing to leading a successful and satisfactory life. [1] [7] [13] Since many studies on masking focus on autistic adults without cognitive impairments only, it is questionable whether their findings generalize across the autism spectrum. [1]

Masking may conceal the person's need for support. [12] It can complicate a diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder (ASD), particularly past childhood, as relevant symptoms are suppressed or compensated for. [22] :60–62 The diagnostic criteria for ASD in the DSM-5 published in 2013 explicitly state that while symptoms "must be present in the early developmental period", these "may be masked by learned strategies in later life", allowing for a diagnosis even if autistic behaviors and difficulties are successfully masked. [22] :57 [23] Addition of such a formulation was proposed to the workgroup drafting the criteria by representatives of the Autistic Self Advocacy Network. [23] The diagnostic criteria for ASD in the ICD-11 (2022) contain a similar provision. [16]

It has been hypothesized that masking may play an important role in explaining why autistic women and non-binary persons [2] are significantly less often recognized and diagnosed as autistic compared to men. [2] [3] [11] This hypothesis was put forward by Lorna Wing as early as 1981 [5] :20 [24] :134 and is recognized in the DSM-5-TR published in 2022. [22] :65

Research

While masking was written about and discussed among autistic people, it has only become a focus of academic research since the 2010s. [5] :18 The Camouflaging Autistic Traits Questionnaire (CAT-Q), a first self-report measure for camouflaging, was published in 2018. [25] [26] Across 25 items, it measures the extent to which a person utilizes strategies to actively compensate for difficulties in social situations (Compensation, 9 items), uses strategies to hide autistic characteristics or portray a non-autistic persona (Masking, 8 items), and employs strategies to fit in with others in social situations (Assimilation, 8 items). [25]

In light of rising awareness of the adverse mental health outcomes of masking and insight into the double empathy problem, therapies and interventions with implicit or explicit targets of instilling neurotypical behavior in autistic people and suppression of autistic traits are controversial. [27] [28] Some autistic adults subjected to applied behavior analysis therapies as children describe being forced to behave like neurotypical peers, contrary to their true identity, with detrimental effects on their mental and overall well-being. [29] [30]

There are some research studies centering around the experiences of masking by comparing different groups of people. In 2021, researchers conducted an online survey that compares the experiences of masking in people with autism, neurodivergent people without autism, and neurotypical people. [31] They found that the behavior of masking is shared across all types of people, but some aspects of masking are more specific to people with autism, such as sensory suppression and suppression of stimming. Researchers also situated this into a workplace context and examined workplace masking experiences for autistic, non-autistic, and neurotypical adults in the UK. They identified eight emerging themes from the survey and reported large overlap among three groups such as reasonings and perceptions of benefits and drawbacks: both neurodivergent and neurotypical people adopted masking strategies to achieve social goals, indicating that masking is more like a common rather exclusive experience. [32]

Some qualitative research focused more on the masking experiences specifically for autistic people. A study in 2022 conducted semi-structured interviews with twenty autistic teenagers and observed how masking is associated with mental health (but not necessarily in linear relationship) and how both of them are affected by social and environmental factors. They stressed the need of approaching masking, authenticity, and mental health through the context of people's identities and also the environment, additionally providing some implications to diagnostic services and interventions. [33] Across autism spectrum, there's also an overview of masking/camouflaging characterizations which explores the analogy between camouflaging and passing and argues how masking may be further different for understudied groups across the spectrum like children and adults with linguistic disabilities. [34]

In addition to masking, researchers investigated the 'authenticity' people with autism feel while socializing and observed how supportive environments like being around people who accept and understand them can lead to their self-awareness and bring more positive socializing experiences than camouflaging. [35] However, this doesn't imply that autistic 'masking' is equivalent to non-authenticity. Researchers proposed that the focus should not be encouraging masking, but promoting autistic authenticity which brings more positive self-image and better mental health. [33]

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Asperger syndrome</span> Neurodevelopmental diagnosis now categorized under Autism Spectrum Disorder

Asperger syndrome (AS), also known as Asperger's syndrome, formerly described a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by significant difficulties in social interaction and nonverbal communication, along with restricted and repetitive patterns of behavior, interests, and activities. The syndrome has been merged with other disorders into autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and is no longer considered a stand-alone diagnosis. It was considered milder than other diagnoses that were merged into ASD due to relatively unimpaired spoken language and intelligence.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Controversies in autism</span> Controversies about both the cause of autism and the nature of the diagnoses themselves

Diagnoses of autism have become more frequent since the 1980s, which has led to various controversies about both the cause of autism and the nature of the diagnoses themselves. Whether autism has mainly a genetic or developmental cause, and the degree of coincidence between autism and intellectual disability, are all matters of current scientific controversy as well as inquiry. There is also more sociopolitical debate as to whether autism should be considered a disability on its own.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Neurodiversity</span> Non-pathological explanation of variations in mental functions

Neurodiversity is a framework for understanding human brain function and mental illness. It argues that diversity in human cognition is normal and that some conditions classified as mental disorders are differences and disabilities that are not necessarily pathological.

Ole Ivar Løvaas was a Norwegian-American clinical psychologist and professor at the University of California, Los Angeles. He is most well known for his research on what is now called applied behavior analysis (ABA) to teach autistic children through prompts, modeling, and positive reinforcement. The therapy is also noted for its use of aversives (punishment) to reduce undesired behavior, however these are now used less commonly than in the past.

Mind-blindness, mindblindness or mind blindness is a theory initially proposed in 1990 that claims that all autistic people have a lack or developmental delay of theory of mind (ToM), meaning they are unable to attribute mental states to others. According to the theory, a lack of ToM is considered equivalent to a lack of both cognitive and affective empathy. In the context of the theory, mind-blindness implies being unable to predict behavior and attribute mental states including beliefs, desires, emotions, or intentions of other people. The mind-blindness theory asserts that children who delay in this development will often develop autism.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Masking (personality)</span> Social process

In psychology and sociology, masking is the process in which an individual camouflages their natural personality or behavior to conform to social pressures, abuse, or harassment. Masking can be strongly influenced by environmental factors such as authoritative parents, rejection, and emotional, physical, or sexual abuse. Masking can be a behavior individuals adopt subconsciously as coping mechanisms or a trauma response, or it can be a conscious behavior an individual adopts to fit in within perceived societal norms. Masking is interconnected with maintaining performative behavior within social structures and cultures.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Autism therapies</span> Therapy aimed at autistic people

Autism therapies include a wide variety of therapies that help people with autism, or their families. Such methods of therapy seek to aid autistic people in dealing with difficulties and increase their functional independence.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Stimming</span> Repetitive self-stimulatory behaviour common in neurodevelopmental disorders

Self-stimulatory behavior, also known as "stimming" and self-stimulation, is the repetition of physical movements, sounds, words, moving objects, or other repetitive behaviors. Such behaviors are found to some degree in all people, especially those with developmental disabilities such as ADHD, as well as autistic people. People diagnosed with sensory processing disorder are also known to potentially exhibit stimming behaviors.

The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ) is a questionnaire published in 2001 by Simon Baron-Cohen and his colleagues at the Autism Research Centre in Cambridge, UK. Consisting of fifty questions, it aims to investigate whether adults of average intelligence have symptoms of autism spectrum conditions. More recently, versions of the AQ for children and adolescents have also been published.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Autistic art</span> Art created by autistic artists to capture the autistic experience

Autistic art is artwork created by autistic artists that captures or conveys a variety of autistic experiences. According to a 2021 article in Cognitive Processing, autistic artists with improved linguistic and communication skills often show a greater degree of originality and attention to detail than their neurotypical counterparts, with a positive correlation between artistic talent and high linguistic functioning. Autistic art is often considered outsider art. Art by autistic artists has long been shown in separate venues from artists without disabilities. The works of some autistic artists have featured in art publications and documentaries and been exhibited in mainstream galleries. Although autistic artists seldom received formal art education in the past, recent inclusivity initiatives have made it easier for autistic artists to get a formal college education. The Aspergers/Autism Network's AANE Artist Collaborative is an example of an art organization for autistic adults.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Societal and cultural aspects of autism</span>

Societal and cultural aspects of autism or sociology of autism come into play with recognition of autism, approaches to its support services and therapies, and how autism affects the definition of personhood. The autistic community is divided primarily into two camps; the autism rights movement and the Pathology paradigm. The pathology paradigm advocates for supporting research into therapies, treatments, and/or a cure to help minimize or remove autistic traits, seeing treatment as vital to help individuals with autism, while the neurodiversity movement believes autism should be seen as a different way of being and advocates against a cure and interventions that focus on normalization, seeing it as trying to exterminate autistic people and their individuality. Both are controversial in autism communities and advocacy which has led to significant infighting between these two camps. While the dominant paradigm is the pathology paradigm and is followed largely by autism research and scientific communities, the neurodiversity movement is highly popular among most autistic people, within autism advocacy, autism rights organizations, and related neurodiversity approaches have been rapidly growing and applied in the autism research field in the last few years.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Classic autism</span> Neurodevelopmental condition

Classic autism, also known as childhood autism, autistic disorder, (early) infantile autism, infantile psychosis, Kanner's autism,Kanner's syndrome, or (formerly) just autism, is a neurodevelopmental condition first described by Leo Kanner in 1943. It is characterized by atypical and impaired development in social interaction and communication as well as restricted, repetitive behaviors, activities, and interests. These symptoms first appear in early childhood and persist throughout life.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Autism spectrum</span> Neurodevelopmental disorder

Autism, formally called autism spectrum disorder (ASD) or autism spectrum condition (ASC), is a neurodevelopmental disorder marked by deficits in reciprocal social communication and the presence of restricted and repetitive patterns of behavior. Other common signs include difficulties with social interaction, verbal and nonverbal communication, along with perseverative interests, stereotypic body movements, rigid routines, and hyper- or hyporeactivity to sensory input. Autism is clinically regarded as a spectrum disorder, meaning that it can manifest very differently in each person. For example, some are nonspeaking, while others have proficient spoken language. Because of this, there is wide variation in the support needs of people across the autism spectrum.

Diagnosis, treatment, and experiences of autism varies globally. Although the diagnosis of autism is rising in post-industrial nations, diagnosis rates are much lower in developing nations.

Sex and gender differences in autism exist regarding prevalence, presentation, and diagnosis.

Discrimination against autistic people is the discrimination, persecution, and oppression that autistic people have been subjected to. Discrimination against autistic people is a form of ableism.

Marian Diamond Sigman (1941–2012) was a developmental and child clinical psychologist known for her research on autism spectrum disorder (ASD). At the time of her death, she was Professor Emeritus of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences and Psychology at the University of California, Los Angeles (UCLA).

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Autism and LGBT identities</span>

Current research indicates that autistic people have higher rates of LGBT identities and feelings than the general population. A variety of explanations for this have been proposed, such as prenatal hormonal exposure, which has been linked with both sexual orientation, gender dysphoria and autism. Alternatively, autistic people may be less reliant on social norms and thus are more open about their orientation or gender identity. A narrative review published in 2016 stated that while various hypotheses have been proposed for an association between autism and gender dysphoria, they lack strong evidence.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Double empathy problem</span> Psychological theory regarding individuals on the autism spectrum

The theory of the double empathy problem is a psychological and sociological theory first coined in 2012 by Damian Milton, an autistic autism researcher. This theory proposes that many of the difficulties autistic individuals face when socializing with non-autistic individuals are due, in part, to a lack of mutual understanding between the two groups, meaning that most autistic people struggle to understand and empathize with non-autistic people, whereas most non-autistic people also struggle to understand and empathize with autistic people. This lack of understanding may stem from bidirectional differences in communication style, social-cognitive characteristics, and experiences between autistic and non-autistic individuals, but not necessarily an inherent deficiency. Recent studies have shown that most autistic individuals are able to socialize, communicate effectively, empathize well, and display social reciprocity with most other autistic individuals. This theory and subsequent findings challenge the commonly held belief that the social skills of autistic individuals are inherently impaired, as well as the theory of "mind-blindness" proposed by prominent autism researcher Simon Baron-Cohen in the mid-1980s, which suggested that empathy and theory of mind are universally impaired in autistic individuals.

Autistic burnout is a prolonged state of intense fatigue, decreased executive functioning or life skills, and increased sensory processing sensitivity experienced by autistic people. Autistic burnout is thought to be caused by stress arising from masking or living in a neurotypical environment that does not accommodate needs of autistic people.

References

  1. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Petrolini, Valentina; Rodríguez-Armendariz, Ekaine; Vicente, Agustín (2023). "Autistic camouflaging across the spectrum". New Ideas in Psychology . 68: 100992. doi:10.1016/j.newideapsych.2022.100992. hdl: 10810/59712 . S2CID   253316582.
  2. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Pearson, Amy; Rose, Kieran (2021). "A Conceptual Analysis of Autistic Masking: Understanding the Narrative of Stigma and the Illusion of Choice". Autism in Adulthood . 3 (1): 52–60. doi:10.1089/aut.2020.0043. PMC   8992880 . PMID   36601266.
  3. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Hull, Laura; Petrides, K. V.; Allison, Carrie; Smith, Paula; Baron-Cohen, Simon; Lai, Meng-Chuan; Mandy, William (2017). ""Putting on My Best Normal": Social Camouflaging in Adults with Autism Spectrum Conditions". Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders . 47 (8): 2519–2534. doi:10.1007/s10803-017-3166-5. PMC   5509825 . PMID   28527095.
  4. Lawson, Wenn B. (2020). "Adaptive Morphing and Coping with Social Threat in Autism: An Autistic Perspective". Journal of Intellectual Disability - Diagnosis and Treatment. 8 (3): 519–526. doi:10.6000/2292-2598.2020.08.03.29. ISSN   2292-2598. S2CID   224896658.
  5. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Sedgewick, Felicity; Hull, Laura; Ellis, Helen (2021). Autism and Masking: How and Why People Do It, and the Impact It Can Have. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers. ISBN   978-1-78775-580-2. OCLC   1287133295.
  6. Cook, Julia; Hull, Laura; Crane, Laura; Mandy, William (2021). "Camouflaging in autism: A systematic review". Clinical Psychology Review. 89: 102080. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2021.102080. ISSN   0272-7358. PMID   34563942. S2CID   237942158.
  7. 1 2 Livingston, Lucy Anne; Shah, Punit; Happé, Francesca (2019). "Compensatory strategies below the behavioural surface in autism: a qualitative study". The Lancet Psychiatry . 6 (9): 766–777. doi:10.1016/s2215-0366(19)30224-x. ISSN   2215-0366. PMC   6706698 . PMID   31350208.
  8. 1 2 Cassidy, Elizabeth (2018-08-16). "Autistic Adults Start Campaign to Put an End to This 'Harmful' Behavior". The Mighty. Retrieved 2023-05-08. Cited as an example in Petrolini, Valentina; Rodríguez-Armendariz, Ekaine; Vicente, Agustín (2023). "Autistic camouflaging across the spectrum". New Ideas in Psychology . 68: 100992. doi:10.1016/j.newideapsych.2022.100992. hdl: 10810/59712 . S2CID   253316582.
  9. 1 2 Price, Devon (2022). Unmasking Autism: The Power of Embracing Our Hidden Neurodiversity. London: Monoray. ISBN   978-1-80096-054-1. OCLC   1321047301.
  10. Mandy, Will (2019). "Social camouflaging in autism: Is it time to lose the mask?". Autism . 23 (8): 1879–1881. doi: 10.1177/1362361319878559 . PMID   31552745. S2CID   202762080.
  11. 1 2 3 Hull, Laura; Petrides, K. V.; Mandy, William (2020). "The Female Autism Phenotype and Camouflaging: a Narrative Review". Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 7 (4): 306–317. doi: 10.1007/s40489-020-00197-9 . S2CID   256402443.
  12. 1 2 Haelle, Tara (2018-04-18). "The Consequences of Compensation in Autism". Neurology Advisor. Haymarket Media Group . Retrieved 2023-05-08.
  13. 1 2 Miller, Danielle; Rees, Jon; Pearson, Amy (2021-12-01). ""Masking Is Life": Experiences of Masking in Autistic and Nonautistic Adults". Autism in Adulthood . 3 (4): 330–338. doi:10.1089/aut.2020.0083. ISSN   2573-9581. PMC   8992921 . PMID   36601640.
  14. Deweerdt, Sarah (2020-03-30). "Autistic burnout, explained". Spectrum. Simons Foundation. doi: 10.53053/bpzp2355 . S2CID   251634477 . Retrieved 2023-05-09.
  15. Arnold, Samuel RC; Higgins, Julianne M; Weise, Janelle; Desai, Aishani; Pellicano, Elizabeth; Trollor, Julian N (2023). "Confirming the nature of autistic burnout". Autism . 27 (7): 1906–1918. doi:10.1177/13623613221147410. PMID   36637293. S2CID   255773489.
  16. 1 2 "6A02 Autism spectrum disorder". ICD-11 for Mortality and Morbidity Statistics . World Health Organization. 2023. Retrieved 2023-05-05.
  17. Russo, Francine (21 February 2018). "The Costs of Camouflaging Autism". Spectrum. Simons Foundation . Retrieved 2023-05-08.
  18. 1 2 3 Radulski, Elizabeth M. (2022). "Conceptualising Autistic Masking, Camouflaging, and Neurotypical Privilege: Towards a Minority Group Model of Neurodiversity". Human Development . 66 (2): 113–127. doi:10.1159/000524122. S2CID   248864273.
  19. Cassidy, Sarah; Bradley, Louise; Shaw, Rebecca; Baron-Cohen, Simon (2018). "Risk markers for suicidality in autistic adults". Molecular Autism . 9 (1): 42. doi: 10.1186/s13229-018-0226-4 . ISSN   2040-2392. PMC   6069847 . PMID   30083306.
  20. Cassidy, S. A.; Gould, K.; Townsend, E.; Pelton, M.; Robertson, A. E.; Rodgers, J. (2020). "Is Camouflaging Autistic Traits Associated with Suicidal Thoughts and Behaviours? Expanding the Interpersonal Psychological Theory of Suicide in an Undergraduate Student Sample". Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders . 50 (10): 3638–3648. doi:10.1007/s10803-019-04323-3. ISSN   1573-3432. PMC   7502035 . PMID   31820344.
  21. Cage, Eilidh (2017). "Experiences of Autism Acceptance and Mental Health in Autistic Adults". Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders . 49 (2): 473–484. doi: 10.1007/s10803-017-3342-7 . PMC   5807490 . PMID   29071566.
  22. 1 2 3 American Psychiatric Association (2022). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition, Text Revision . Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. doi:10.1176/appi.books.9780890425787. ISBN   978-0-89042-575-6. S2CID   249488050.
  23. 1 2 Kapp, Steven K.; Ne'eman, Ari (2020), Kapp, Steven K. (ed.), "Lobbying Autism's Diagnostic Revision in the DSM-5" (PDF), Autistic Community and the Neurodiversity Movement: Stories from the Frontline, Singapore: Palgrave Macmillan, pp. 167–194, doi:10.1007/978-981-13-8437-0_13, ISBN   978-981-13-8437-0, S2CID   209465381 , retrieved 2023-06-08
  24. Wing, Lorna (1981). "Sex ratios in early childhood autism and related conditions". Psychiatry Research . 5 (2): 129–137. doi:10.1016/0165-1781(81)90043-3. PMID   6945608. S2CID   41912698.
  25. 1 2 Hull, Laura; Mandy, William; Lai, Meng-Chuan; Baron-Cohen, Simon; Allison, Carrie; Smith, Paula; Petrides, K. V. (2019) [Published 25 October 2018]. "Development and Validation of the Camouflaging Autistic Traits Questionnaire (CAT-Q)". Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders . 49 (3): 819–833. doi:10.1007/s10803-018-3792-6. PMC   6394586 . PMID   30361940.
  26. Williams, Zachary J. (2022). "Commentary: The construct validity of 'camouflaging' in autism: psychometric considerations and recommendations for future research ‐ reflection on Lai et al. (2020)". Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry . 63 (1): 118–121. doi:10.1111/jcpp.13468. PMC   8678389 . PMID   34145574.
  27. Schuck, Rachel K.; Tagavi, Daina M.; Baiden, Kaitlynn M. P.; Dwyer, Patrick; Williams, Zachary J.; Osuna, Anthony; Ferguson, Emily F.; Jimenez Muñoz, Maria; Poyser, Samantha K.; Johnson, Joy F.; Vernon, Ty W. (2022). "Neurodiversity and Autism Intervention: Reconciling Perspectives Through a Naturalistic Developmental Behavioral Intervention Framework". Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders . 52 (10): 4625–4645. doi:10.1007/s10803-021-05316-x. PMC   9508016 . PMID   34643863.
  28. Dawson, Geraldine; Franz, Lauren; Brandsen, S. (2022). "At a Crossroads—Reconsidering the Goals of Autism Early Behavioral Intervention From a Neurodiversity Perspective". JAMA Pediatrics . 176 (9): 839–840. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2022.2299. PMC   10069446 . PMID   35816341.
  29. McGill, Owen; Robinson, Anna (2020). ""Recalling hidden harms": autistic experiences of childhood applied behavioural analysis (ABA)" (PDF). Advances in Autism. 7 (4): 269–282. doi:10.1108/AIA-04-2020-0025. S2CID   225282499 via Strathprints (University of Strathclyde).
  30. Anderson, Laura K (2023). "Autistic experiences of applied behavior analysis". Autism . 27 (3): 737–750. doi:10.1177/13623613221118216. ISSN   1362-3613. PMID   35999706. S2CID   251766661.
  31. Miller, Danielle; Rees, Jon; Pearson, Amy (2021-12-01). ""Masking Is Life": Experiences of Masking in Autistic and Nonautistic Adults". Autism in Adulthood. 3 (4): 330–338. doi:10.1089/aut.2020.0083. ISSN   2573-9581. PMC   8992921 . PMID   36601640.
  32. Pryke-Hobbes, Amber; Davies, Jade; Heasman, Brett; Livesey, Adam; Walker, Amy; Pellicano, Elizabeth; Remington, Anna (2023-09-06). Rosenbaum, Janet E. (ed.). "The workplace masking experiences of autistic, non-autistic neurodivergent and neurotypical adults in the UK". PLOS ONE. 18 (9): e0290001. Bibcode:2023PLoSO..1890001P. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0290001 . ISSN   1932-6203. PMC   10482295 . PMID   37672533.
  33. 1 2 Chapman, Louise; Rose, Kieran; Hull, Laura; Mandy, William (2022-11-01). ""I want to fit in… but I don't want to change myself fundamentally": A qualitative exploration of the relationship between masking and mental health for autistic teenagers". Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders. 99: 102069. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2022.102069 . ISSN   1750-9467. S2CID   253495529.
  34. Petrolini, Valentina; Rodríguez-Armendariz, Ekaine; Vicente, Agustín (2023-01-01). "Autistic camouflaging across the spectrum". New Ideas in Psychology. 68: 100992. doi: 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2022.100992 . hdl: 10810/59712 . ISSN   0732-118X. S2CID   253316582.
  35. Cook, Julia M; Crane, Laura; Mandy, William (2023-07-05). "Dropping the mask: It takes two". Autism. doi: 10.1177/13623613231183059 . ISSN   1362-3613. PMID   37403917. S2CID   259335250.

Further reading