Lateralization of brain function

Last updated

The human brain is divided into two hemispheres-left and right. Scientists continue to explore how some cognitive functions tend to be dominated by one side or the other; that is, how they are lateralized.
.mw-parser-output .legend{page-break-inside:avoid;break-inside:avoid-column}.mw-parser-output .legend-color{display:inline-block;min-width:1.25em;height:1.25em;line-height:1.25;margin:1px 0;text-align:center;border:1px solid black;background-color:transparent;color:black}.mw-parser-output .legend-text{}
Right cerebral hemisphere
Left cerebral hemisphere Cerebral hemisphere - animation.gif
The human brain is divided into two hemispheresleft and right. Scientists continue to explore how some cognitive functions tend to be dominated by one side or the other; that is, how they are lateralized.
  Right cerebral hemisphere
  Left cerebral hemisphere

The lateralization of brain function (or hemispheric dominance [1] [2] / latralisation [3] [4] ) is the tendency for some neural functions or cognitive processes to be specialized to one side of the brain or the other. The median longitudinal fissure separates the human brain into two distinct cerebral hemispheres, connected by the corpus callosum. Although the macrostructure of the two hemispheres appears to be almost identical, different composition of neuronal networks allows for specialized function that is different in each hemisphere.

Contents

Lateralization of brain structures is based on general trends expressed in healthy patients; however, there are numerous counterexamples to each generalization. Each human's brain develops differently, leading to unique lateralization in individuals. This is different from specialization, as lateralization refers only to the function of one structure divided between two hemispheres. Specialization is much easier to observe as a trend, since it has a stronger anthropological history. [5]

The best example of an established lateralization is that of Broca's and Wernicke's areas, where both are often found exclusively on the left hemisphere. Function lateralization, such as semantics, intonation, accentuation, and prosody, has since been called into question and largely been found to have a neuronal basis in both hemispheres. [6] Another example is that each hemisphere in the brain tends to represent one side of the body. In the cerebellum, this is the same body side, but in the forebrain this is predominantly the contralateral side.

Lateralized functions

Language

Language functions such as grammar, vocabulary and literal meaning are typically lateralized to the left hemisphere, especially in right-handed individuals. [7] While language production is left-lateralized in up to 90% of right-handers, it is more bilateral, or even right-lateralized, in approximately 50% of left-handers. [8]

This photo shows the left and right specialties of the brain. Brain Lateralization.png
This photo shows the left and right specialties of the brain.

Broca's area and Wernicke's area, associated with the production of speech and comprehension of speech, respectively, are located in the left cerebral hemisphere for about 95% of right-handers but about 70% of left-handers. [9] :69 Social interactions, demonstrating fierce emotions, and mathematical information are all provided by the right hemisphere. [10]

Sensory processing

The processing of basic sensory information is lateralized by being divided into left and right sides of the body or the space around the body.

In vision, about half the neurons of the optic nerve from each eye cross to project to the opposite hemisphere, and about half do not cross to project to the hemisphere on the same side. [11] This means that the left side of the visual field is processed largely by the visual cortex of the right hemisphere and vice versa for the right side of the visual field.

In hearing, about 90% of the neurons of the auditory nerve from one ear cross to project to the auditory cortex of the opposite hemisphere.

In the sense of touch, most of the neurons from the skin cross to project to the somatosensory cortex of the opposite hemisphere.

Because of this functional division of the left and right sides of the body and of the space that surrounds it, the processing of information in the sensory cortices is essentially identical. That is, the processing of visual and auditory stimuli, spatial manipulation, facial perception, and artistic ability are represented bilaterally. [8] Numerical estimation, comparison and online calculation depend on bilateral parietal regions [12] [13] while exact calculation and fact retrieval are associated with left parietal regions, perhaps due to their ties to linguistic processing. [12] [13]

Value systems

Rather than just being a series of places where different brain modules occur, there are running similarities in the kind of function seen in each side, for instance how right-side impairment of drawing ability making patients draw the parts of the subject matter with wholly incoherent relationships, or where the kind of left-side damage seen in language impairment not damaging the patient's ability to catch the significance of intonation in speech. [14] This has led British psychiatrist Iain McGilchrist to view the two hemispheres as having different value systems, where the left hemisphere tends to reduce complex matters such as ethics to rules and measures, and the right hemisphere is disposed to the holistic and metaphorical. [15]

Clinical significance

Depression is linked with a hyperactive right hemisphere, with evidence of selective involvement in "processing negative emotions, pessimistic thoughts and unconstructive thinking styles", as well as vigilance, arousal and self-reflection, and a relatively hypoactive left hemisphere, "specifically involved in processing pleasurable experiences" and "relatively more involved in decision-making processes". [16] Additionally, "left hemisphere lesions result in an omissive response bias or error pattern whereas right hemisphere lesions result in a commissive response bias or error pattern." [17] The delusional misidentification syndromes, reduplicative paramnesia and Capgras delusion are also often the result of right hemisphere lesions. [18]

Lateral view of the Brain Blausen 0101 Brain LateralView.png
Lateral view of the Brain

Hemisphere damage

Damage to either the right or left hemisphere, and its resulting deficits provide insight into the function of the damaged area. There is truth to the idea that some brain functions reside more on one side of the brain than the other. We know this in part from what is lost when a stroke affects a particular part of the brain. Left hemisphere damage has many effects on language production and perception. Damage or lesions to the right hemisphere can result in a lack of emotional prosody [19] or intonation when speaking. [20] The left hemisphere is often involved with dealing of detail oriented and perception while the right hemisphere is involved with mostly overview and an overall concept of things. [1]

Right hemisphere damage also has grave effects on understanding discourse. People with damage to the right hemisphere have a reduced ability to generate inferences, comprehend and produce main concepts, and a reduced ability to manage alternative meanings. Furthermore, people with right hemisphere damage often exhibit discourse that is abrupt and perfunctory or verbose and excessive. They can also have pragmatic deficits in situations of turn taking, topic maintenance and shared knowledge. . [20] Although both sides of the hemisphere has different responsibilities and tasks, they both complete each other and create a bigger picture. [2] Lateral brain damage can also affect visual perceptual spatial resolution. People with left hemisphere damage may have impaired perception of high resolution, or detailed, aspects of an image. People with right hemisphere damage may have impaired perception of low resolution, or big picture, aspects of an image.

Plasticity

If a specific region of the brain, or even an entire hemisphere, is injured or destroyed, its functions can sometimes be assumed by a neighboring region in the same hemisphere or the corresponding region in the other hemisphere, depending upon the area damaged and the patient's age. [21] When injury interferes with pathways from one area to another, alternative (indirect) connections may develop to communicate information with detached areas, despite the inefficiencies.

Broca's aphasia

Broca's aphasia is a specific type of expressive aphasia and is so named due to the aphasia that results from damage or lesions to the Broca's area of the brain, that exists most commonly in the left inferior frontal hemisphere. Thus, the aphasia that develops from the lack of functioning of the Broca's area is an expressive and non-fluent aphasia. It is called 'non-fluent' due to the issues that arise because Broca's area is critical for language pronunciation and production. The area controls some motor aspects of speech production and articulation of thoughts to words and as such lesions to the area result in specific non-fluent aphasia. [22]

Wernicke's aphasia

Wernicke's aphasia is the result of damage to the area of the brain that is commonly in the left hemisphere above the Sylvian fissure. Damage to this area causes primarily a deficit in language comprehension. While the ability to speak fluently with normal melodic intonation is spared, the language produced by a person with Wernicke's aphasia is riddled with semantic errors and may sound nonsensical to the listener. Wernicke's aphasia is characterized by phonemic paraphasias, neologism or jargon. Another characteristic of a person with Wernicke's aphasia is that they are unconcerned by the mistakes that they are making.

Society and culture

Possible misapplication

Oversimplification of hemisphericity RightBrainDominant.jpg
Oversimplification of hemisphericity

The concept of "right-brained" or "left-brained" individuals is considered a widespread myth which oversimplifies the true nature of the brain's cerebral hemispheres (for a recent counter position, though, see below). Proof leading to the "mythbuster" of the left-/right-brained concept is increasing as more and more studies are brought to light. Harvard Health Publishing includes a study from the University of Utah in 2013, that exhibited brain scans revealing similarity on both sides of the brain, personality and environmental factors aside. Although certain functions show a degree of lateralization in the brain—with language predominantly processed in the left hemisphere, and spatial and nonverbal reasoning in the right—these functions are not exclusively tied to one hemisphere. [23]

Terence Hines states that the research on brain lateralization is valid as a research program, though commercial promoters have applied it to promote subjects and products far outside the implications of the research. [24] For example, the implications of the research have no bearing on psychological interventions such as eye movement desensitization and reprocessing (EMDR) and neurolinguistic programming, [25] [26] brain-training equipment, or management training. [27]

Counter position

The debate as to being “right-brained” or “left-brained” was reopened anew in 2023. [28] This re-opening is based on neuroscientific research which has refined the underlying primary functions of the two sides of the neocortex. In particular, Elkhonon Goldberg's “novelty-routinization” theory posits that the neocortex's right and left hemispheres primarily process cognitively novel and cognitively routine tasks, respectively. [29]

(Thus, this theory fits with [novel] aspects of visual-spatial processing predominantly occurring in the right hemisphere, as they tend to be relatively novel; similarly, [familiar] aspects of language tend to being predominantly processed in the left hemisphere, as they indeed tend to be cognitively routine to us. New/novel aspects of language, though, will fittingly be processed in the right-brain, just as familiar aspects of visual-spatial processing will be processed in the left-brain. [This explains why these functions are not exclusively tied to one hemisphere.])

As such, right-brained people favor the cognitively novel tasks/information whereas left-brained people favor the cognitively routine tasks/information. Or, in other words, right-brained people tend to prefer new and novel experiences whereas left-brained people favor familiar experiences. (Thus, this theory is the deeper understanding of many of pop psychology's “oversimplifications” of lateralization. For example, it explains why the right hemisphere has been considered creative, as creativity tends to involve new and novel information/tasks, whereas the left hemisphere has been considered analytical, as analysis tends to be the re-processing of already familiar information.)

This is not to say, though, that the tendencies of the left hemisphere cannot be performed well by a right-brained person and vice versa. For instance, right-brained people can exhibit strong analytical skills and attention to detail (generally cognitively routine, left-brained tasks), sometimes even surpassing their left-brained counterparts, but their overall tendency is more so to instead be synthesizing and to attend to the big picture (generally cognitively new, right-hemisphere-tending tasks). (Such nuances are one reason this thinking has been labeled a myth.)

Based on Goldberg's theory, the following table provides newly identified preferences of left- and right-brained people. [28]

Preference CategoryLeft-brained PeopleRight-brained People
Experience TypeFamiliar and ExistingNew and Novel
Learning/Action PatternLearn, then DoDo, then Learn
Improve or Create?ImproveCreate
Review or Discover?ReviewDiscover
Depth or Breadth?DepthBreadth
Analyze or Action?AnalyzeAction

It is vital, though, to remember that these are only tendencies. At times we may contradict our tendency to be left- or right-brained, but often we can come to identify the underlying reason. [28] Furthermore, although less likely, some of us may be more bilobal, but in general it seems most of demonstrate tendencies related to one side or the other—in other words, most of us prefer either new and novel experiences most of the time or we prefer familiar and known experiences most of the time.

This is not to say, though, that we do not use the whole brain. Life and personal growth require the use and development of both the right and left hemispheres, as it requires the processing of novel, unfamiliar situations as well as familiar, routine ones. Without this ability, we would struggle to survive and thrive. Interestingly, as we get older, we tend to become more set in our ways, which fits with many older adults and seniors having struggles to stay up on technology and other new and novel experiences.

The oversimplification of lateralization in pop psychology. This belief was widely held even in the scientific community for some years. Brain Lateralization.svg
The oversimplification of lateralization in pop psychology. This belief was widely held even in the scientific community for some years.

Some popularizations oversimplify the science about lateralization, by presenting the functional differences between hemispheres as being more absolute than is actually the case. [30] :107 [31] Interestingly, research has shown quite opposite function of brain lateralisation, i.e. left hemisphere creatively and chaotically links between concepts and right hemisphere tends to adhere to specific date and time, although generally adhering to the pattern of left-brain as linguistic interpretation and right brain as spatio-temporal. [32] [33] [34]

Sex differences

In the 19th century and to a lesser extent the 20th, it was thought that each side of the brain was associated with a specific gender: the left corresponding with masculinity and the right with femininity and each half could function independently. [35] The right side of the brain was seen as the inferior and thought to be prominent in women, savages, children, criminals, and the insane. A prime example of this in fictional literature can be seen in Robert Louis Stevenson's Strange Case of Dr. Jekyll and Mr. Hyde . [36]

History

Broca

One of the first indications of brain function lateralization resulted from the research of French physician Pierre Paul Broca, in 1861. His research involved the male patient nicknamed "Tan", who had a speech deficit (aphasia); "tan" was one of the few words he could articulate, hence his nickname. In Tan's autopsy, Broca determined he had a syphilitic lesion in the left cerebral hemisphere. This left frontal lobe brain area (Broca's area) is an important speech production region. The motor aspects of speech production deficits caused by damage to Broca's area are known as expressive aphasia. In clinical assessment of this type of aphasia, patients have difficulty producing speech. [37]

Wernicke

German physician Karl Wernicke continued in the vein of Broca's research by studying language deficits unlike expressive aphasia. Wernicke noted that not every deficit was in speech production; some were linguistic. He found that damage to the left posterior, superior temporal gyrus (Wernicke's area) caused language comprehension deficits rather than speech production deficits, a syndrome known as receptive aphasia.

Imaging

These seminal works on hemispheric specialization were done on patients or postmortem brains, raising questions about the potential impact of pathology on the research findings. New methods permit the in vivo comparison of the hemispheres in healthy subjects. Particularly, magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and positron emission tomography (PET) are important because of their high spatial resolution and ability to image subcortical brain structures.

Movement and sensation

In the 1940s, neurosurgeon Wilder Penfield and his neurologist colleague Herbert Jasper developed a technique of brain mapping to help reduce side effects caused by surgery to treat epilepsy. They stimulated motor and somatosensory cortices of the brain with small electrical currents to activate discrete brain regions. They found that stimulation of one hemisphere's motor cortex produces muscle contraction on the opposite side of the body. Furthermore, the functional map of the motor and sensory cortices is fairly consistent from person to person; Penfield and Jasper's famous pictures of the motor and sensory homunculi were the result.

Split-brain patients

Research by Michael Gazzaniga and Roger Wolcott Sperry in the 1960s on split-brain patients led to an even greater understanding of functional laterality. Split-brain patients are patients who have undergone corpus callosotomy (usually as a treatment for severe epilepsy), a severing of a large part of the corpus callosum. The corpus callosum connects the two hemispheres of the brain and allows them to communicate. When these connections are cut, the two halves of the brain have a reduced capacity to communicate with each other. This led to many interesting behavioral phenomena that allowed Gazzaniga and Sperry to study the contributions of each hemisphere to various cognitive and perceptual processes. One of their main findings was that the right hemisphere was capable of rudimentary language processing, but often has no lexical or grammatical abilities. [38] Eran Zaidel also studied such patients and found some evidence for the right hemisphere having at least some syntactic ability.[ citation needed ]

Language is primarily localized in the left hemisphere. While the left hemisphere has proven to be more optimized for language, the right hemisphere has the capacity with emotions, such as sarcasm, that can express prosody in sentences when speaking. According to Sheppard and Hillis, "The right hemisphere is critical for perceiving sarcasm (Davis et al., 2016), integrating context required for understanding metaphor, inference, and humour, as well as recognizing and expressing affective or emotional prosody—changes in pitch, rhythm, rate, and loudness that convey emotions". [39] One of the experiments carried out by Gazzaniga involved a split-brain male patient sitting in front of a computer screen while having words and images presented on either side of the screen, and the visual stimuli would go to either the right or left visual field, and thus the left or right brain, respectively. It was observed that if the patient was presented with an image to his left visual field (right brain), he would report not seeing anything. If he was able to feel around for certain objects, he could accurately pick out the correct object, despite not having the ability to verbalize what he saw.

Additional images

See also

[40]

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Aphasia</span> Inability to comprehend or formulate language

In aphasia, a person may be unable to comprehend or unable to formulate language because of damage to specific brain regions. The major causes are stroke and head trauma; prevalence is hard to determine but aphasia due to stroke is estimated to be 0.1–0.4% in the Global North. Aphasia can also be the result of brain tumors, epilepsy, autoimmune neurological diseases, brain infections, or neurodegenerative diseases.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Expressive aphasia</span> Language disorder involving inability to produce language

Expressive aphasia, also known as Broca's aphasia, is a type of aphasia characterized by partial loss of the ability to produce language, although comprehension generally remains intact. A person with expressive aphasia will exhibit effortful speech. Speech generally includes important content words but leaves out function words that have more grammatical significance than physical meaning, such as prepositions and articles. This is known as "telegraphic speech". The person's intended message may still be understood, but their sentence will not be grammatically correct. In very severe forms of expressive aphasia, a person may only speak using single word utterances. Typically, comprehension is mildly to moderately impaired in expressive aphasia due to difficulty understanding complex grammar.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Language center</span> Speech processing areas of the brain

In neuroscience and psychology, the term language center refers collectively to the areas of the brain which serve a particular function for speech processing and production. Language is a core system that gives humans the capacity to solve difficult problems and provides them with a unique type of social interaction. Language allows individuals to attribute symbols to specific concepts, and utilize them through sentences and phrases that follow proper grammatical rules. Finally, speech is the mechanism by which language is orally expressed.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Receptive aphasia</span> Language disorder involving inability to understand language

Wernicke's aphasia, also known as receptive aphasia, sensory aphasia, fluent aphasia, or posterior aphasia, is a type of aphasia in which individuals have difficulty understanding written and spoken language. Patients with Wernicke's aphasia demonstrate fluent speech, which is characterized by typical speech rate, intact syntactic abilities and effortless speech output. Writing often reflects speech in that it tends to lack content or meaning. In most cases, motor deficits do not occur in individuals with Wernicke's aphasia. Therefore, they may produce a large amount of speech without much meaning. Individuals with Wernicke's aphasia are typically unaware of their errors in speech and do not realize their speech may lack meaning. They typically remain unaware of even their most profound language deficits.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Broca's area</span> Speech production region in the dominant hemisphere of the hominid brain

Broca's area, or the Broca area, is a region in the frontal lobe of the dominant hemisphere, usually the left, of the brain with functions linked to speech production.

Aphasiology is the study of language impairment usually resulting from brain damage, due to neurovascular accident—hemorrhage, stroke—or associated with a variety of neurodegenerative diseases, including different types of dementia. These specific language deficits, termed aphasias, may be defined as impairments of language production or comprehension that cannot be attributed to trivial causes such as deafness or oral paralysis. A number of aphasias have been described, but two are best known: expressive aphasia and receptive aphasia.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Brain damage</span> Destruction or degeneration of brain cells

Neurotrauma, brain damage or brain injury (BI) is the destruction or degeneration of brain cells. Brain injuries occur due to a wide range of internal and external factors. In general, brain damage refers to significant, undiscriminating trauma-induced damage.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Anomic aphasia</span> Medical condition

Anomic aphasia is a mild, fluent type of aphasia where individuals have word retrieval failures and cannot express the words they want to say. By contrast, anomia is a deficit of expressive language, and a symptom of all forms of aphasia, but patients whose primary deficit is word retrieval are diagnosed with anomic aphasia. Individuals with aphasia who display anomia can often describe an object in detail and maybe even use hand gestures to demonstrate how the object is used, but cannot find the appropriate word to name the object. Patients with anomic aphasia have relatively preserved speech fluency, repetition, comprehension, and grammatical speech.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Cognitive neuropsychology</span>

Cognitive neuropsychology is a branch of cognitive psychology that aims to understand how the structure and function of the brain relates to specific psychological processes. Cognitive psychology is the science that looks at how mental processes are responsible for the cognitive abilities to store and produce new memories, produce language, recognize people and objects, as well as our ability to reason and problem solve. Cognitive neuropsychology places a particular emphasis on studying the cognitive effects of brain injury or neurological illness with a view to inferring models of normal cognitive functioning. Evidence is based on case studies of individual brain damaged patients who show deficits in brain areas and from patients who exhibit double dissociations. Double dissociations involve two patients and two tasks. One patient is impaired at one task but normal on the other, while the other patient is normal on the first task and impaired on the other. For example, patient A would be poor at reading printed words while still being normal at understanding spoken words, while the patient B would be normal at understanding written words and be poor at understanding spoken words. Scientists can interpret this information to explain how there is a single cognitive module for word comprehension. From studies like these, researchers infer that different areas of the brain are highly specialised. Cognitive neuropsychology can be distinguished from cognitive neuroscience, which is also interested in brain-damaged patients, but is particularly focused on uncovering the neural mechanisms underlying cognitive processes.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Temporal lobe</span> One of the four lobes of the mammalian brain

The temporal lobe is one of the four major lobes of the cerebral cortex in the brain of mammals. The temporal lobe is located beneath the lateral fissure on both cerebral hemispheres of the mammalian brain.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Wernicke's area</span> Speech comprehension region in the dominant hemisphere of the hominid brain

Wernicke's area, also called Wernicke's speech area, is one of the two parts of the cerebral cortex that are linked to speech, the other being Broca's area. It is involved in the comprehension of written and spoken language, in contrast to Broca's area, which is primarily involved in the production of language. It is traditionally thought to reside in Brodmann area 22, which is located in the superior temporal gyrus in the dominant cerebral hemisphere, which is the left hemisphere in about 95% of right-handed individuals and 70% of left-handed individuals.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Conduction aphasia</span> Medical condition

Conduction aphasia, also called associative aphasia, is an uncommon form of difficulty in speaking (aphasia). It is caused by damage to the parietal lobe of the brain. An acquired language disorder, it is characterised by intact auditory comprehension, coherent speech production, but poor speech repetition. Affected people are fully capable of understanding what they are hearing, but fail to encode phonological information for production. This deficit is load-sensitive as the person shows significant difficulty repeating phrases, particularly as the phrases increase in length and complexity and as they stumble over words they are attempting to pronounce. People have frequent errors during spontaneous speech, such as substituting or transposing sounds. They are also aware of their errors and will show significant difficulty correcting them.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Global aphasia</span> Medical condition

Global aphasia is a severe form of nonfluent aphasia, caused by damage to the left side of the brain, that affects receptive and expressive language skills as well as auditory and visual comprehension. Acquired impairments of communicative abilities are present across all language modalities, impacting language production, comprehension, and repetition. Patients with global aphasia may be able to verbalize a few short utterances and use non-word neologisms, but their overall production ability is limited. Their ability to repeat words, utterances, or phrases is also affected. Due to the preservation of the right hemisphere, an individual with global aphasia may still be able to express themselves through facial expressions, gestures, and intonation. This type of aphasia often results from a large lesion of the left perisylvian cortex. The lesion is caused by an occlusion of the left middle cerebral artery and is associated with damage to Broca's area, Wernicke's area, and insular regions which are associated with aspects of language.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Arcuate fasciculus</span> Neural pathway connecting Brocas area and Wernickes area

In neuroanatomy, the arcuate fasciculus is a bundle of axons that generally connects the Broca's area and the Wernicke's area in the brain. It is an association fiber tract connecting caudal temporal cortex and inferior frontal lobe.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Brodmann area 22</span>

Brodmann area 22 is a Brodmann's area that is cytoarchitecturally located in the posterior superior temporal gyrus of the brain. In the left cerebral hemisphere, it is one portion of Wernicke's area. The left hemisphere BA22 helps with generation and understanding of individual words. On the right side of the brain, BA22 helps to discriminate pitch and sound intensity, both of which are necessary to perceive melody and prosody. Wernicke's area is active in processing language and consists of the left Brodmann area 22 and Brodmann area 40, the supramarginal gyrus.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Dissociation (neuropsychology)</span>

In neuropsychology, dissociation involves identifying the neural substrate of a particular brain function through identification of case studies, neuroimaging, or neuropsychological testing.

Aprosodia is a neurological condition characterized by the inability of a person to properly convey or interpret emotional prosody. Prosody in language refers to the ranges of rhythm, pitch, stress, intonation, etc. These neurological deficits can be the result of damage of some form to the non-dominant hemisphere areas of language production. The prevalence of aprosodias in individuals is currently unknown, as testing for aprosodia secondary to other brain injury is only a recent occurrence.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Brain asymmetry</span> Term in human neuroanatomy referring to several things

In human neuroanatomy, brain asymmetry can refer to at least two quite distinct findings:

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Sign language in the brain</span>

Sign language refers to any natural language which uses visual gestures produced by the hands and body language to express meaning. The brain's left side is the dominant side utilized for producing and understanding sign language, just as it is for speech. In 1861, Paul Broca studied patients with the ability to understand spoken languages but the inability to produce them. The damaged area was named Broca's area, and located in the left hemisphere’s inferior frontal gyrus. Soon after, in 1874, Carl Wernicke studied patients with the reverse deficits: patients could produce spoken language, but could not comprehend it. The damaged area was named Wernicke's area, and is located in the left hemisphere’s posterior superior temporal gyrus.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Disconnection syndrome</span> Collection of neurological symptoms

Disconnection syndrome is a general term for a collection of neurological symptoms caused – via lesions to associational or commissural nerve fibres – by damage to the white matter axons of communication pathways in the cerebrum, independent of any lesions to the cortex. The behavioral effects of such disconnections are relatively predictable in adults. Disconnection syndromes usually reflect circumstances where regions A and B still have their functional specializations except in domains that depend on the interconnections between the two regions.

References

  1. "Left brain vs. right brain: How does one dominate?". www.medicalnewstoday.com. 24 December 2018. Retrieved 10 January 2023.
  2. "Hemispheric differences and hemispheric dominance (video)". Khan Academy. Retrieved 10 January 2023.
  3. "Localisation of Function in the brain and Hemispheric Lateralisation: motor, somatosensory, visual, auditory and language centres; Broca's and Wernicke's areas, split brain research. Plasticity and Functional Recovery of the brain after trauma". Psychology Hub. Retrieved 10 January 2023.
  4. "Lateralization of Brain Function - Simply Psychology". www.simplypsychology.org. Retrieved 10 January 2023.
  5. Halpern ME, Güntürkün O, Hopkins WD, Rogers LJ (November 2005). "Lateralization of the vertebrate brain: taking the side of model systems". The Journal of Neuroscience. 25 (45): 10351–10357. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3439-05.2005. PMC   2654579 . PMID   16280571.
  6. Riès SK, Dronkers NF, Knight RT (April 2016). "Choosing words: left hemisphere, right hemisphere, or both? Perspective on the lateralization of word retrieval". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1369 (1): 111–131. Bibcode:2016NYASA1369..111R. doi:10.1111/nyas.12993. PMC   4874870 . PMID   26766393.
  7. Taylor I, Taylor MM (1990). Psycholinguistics: Learning and using Language. Pearson. ISBN   978-0-13-733817-7. p. 367
  8. 1 2 Beaumont JG (2008). "Chapter 7". Introduction to Neuropsychology (Second ed.). The Guilford Press. ISBN   978-1-59385-068-5.
  9. Griggs RA (2012). Psychology : a concise introduction (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Worth Publishers. ISBN   978-1429261555.
  10. Rogers, Lesley J. (July 2021). "Brain Lateralization and Cognitive Capacity". Animals. 11 (7): 1996. doi: 10.3390/ani11071996 . ISSN   2076-2615. PMC   8300231 . PMID   34359124.
  11. "The Optic Nerve - Human Anatomy". Photius Coutsoukis.
  12. 1 2 Dehaene S, Spelke E, Pinel P, Stanescu R, Tsivkin S (May 1999). "Sources of mathematical thinking: behavioral and brain-imaging evidence" (PDF). Science. 284 (5416): 970–974. Bibcode:1999Sci...284..970D. doi:10.1126/science.284.5416.970. PMID   10320379. Archived (PDF) from the original on 19 July 2011.
  13. 1 2 Dehaene S, Piazza M, Pinel P, Cohen L (May 2003). "Three parietal circuits for number processing" (PDF). Cognitive Neuropsychology. 20 (3): 487–506. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.4.8178 . doi:10.1080/02643290244000239. PMID   20957581. S2CID   13458123. Archived (PDF) from the original on 19 July 2011.
  14. McGilchrist (2009) provides an extensive survey of the relevant literature in chapter two.
  15. McGilchrist I, Vedantam S (4 February 2019). "One Head, Two Brains: How The Brain's Hemispheres Shape The World We See" (Audio podcast with transcript). NPR Hidden Brain .
  16. Hecht D (October 2010). "Depression and the hyperactive right-hemisphere". Neuroscience Research. 68 (2): 77–87. doi:10.1016/j.neures.2010.06.013. PMID   20603163. S2CID   39453265.
  17. Braun CM, Delisle J, Guimond A, Daigneault R (March 2009). "Post unilateral lesion response biases modulate memory: crossed double dissociation of hemispheric specialisations". Laterality. 14 (2): 122–164. doi:10.1080/13576500802328613. PMID   18991140. S2CID   28225385.
  18. Devinsky O (January 2009). "Delusional misidentifications and duplications: right brain lesions, left brain delusions". Neurology. 72 (1): 80–87. doi:10.1212/01.wnl.0000338625.47892.74. PMID   19122035. S2CID   207103708.
  19. Patel, Sona; Oishi, Kenichi; Wright, Amy; Sutherland-Foggio, Harry; Saxena, Sadhvi; Sheppard, Shannon M.; Hillis, Argye E. (2018). "Right Hemisphere Regions Critical for Expression of Emotion Through Prosody". Frontiers in Neurology. 9: 224. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.00224 . ISSN   1664-2295. PMC   5897518 . PMID   29681885.
  20. 1 2 Zickert, Nele; Geuze, Reint H.; Beking, Tess; Groothuis, Ton G. G. (20 August 2021). "Testing the Darwinian function of lateralization. Does separation of workload between brain hemispheres increase cognitive performance?". Neuropsychologia. 159: 107884. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2021.107884 . ISSN   0028-3932. PMID   34090868.
  21. Pulsifer MB, Brandt J, Salorio CF, Vining EP, Carson BS, Freeman JM (March 2004). "The cognitive outcome of hemispherectomy in 71 children". Epilepsia. 45 (3): 243–254. doi: 10.1111/j.0013-9580.2004.15303.x . PMID   15009226. S2CID   15894321.
  22. Pinel PJ (2011). Biopsychology (8th ed.). Allyn & Bacon. ISBN   978-0-205-83256-9.
  23. Creative Commons by small.svg  This article incorporates text available under the CC BY 4.0 license.Betts, J Gordon; Desaix, Peter; Johnson, Eddie; Johnson, Jody E; Korol, Oksana; Kruse, Dean; Poe, Brandon; Wise, James; Womble, Mark D; Young, Kelly A (8 June 2023). Anatomy & Physiology. Houston: OpenStax CNX. 13.2 The Central Nervous System. ISBN   978-1-947172-04-3.
  24. Hines T (1987). "Left Brain/Right Brain Mythology and Implications for Management and Training". The Academy of Management Review. 12 (4): 600–606. doi:10.2307/258066. JSTOR   258066.
  25. Drenth JD (2003). "Growing anti-intellectualism in Europe; a menace to science". Studia Psychologica. 45 (1): 5–13., available in ALLEA Annual Report 2003 Archived 16 June 2011 at the Wayback Machine , pp. 61–72
  26. Nielsen JA, Zielinski BA, Ferguson MA, Lainhart JE, Anderson JS (14 August 2013). "An evaluation of the left-brain vs. right-brain hypothesis with resting state functional connectivity magnetic resonance imaging". PLOS ONE. 8 (8): e71275. Bibcode:2013PLoSO...871275N. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0071275 . PMC   3743825 . PMID   23967180.
  27. Della Sala S (1999). Mind Myths: Exploring Popular Assumptions about the Mind and Brain. New York: Wiley. ISBN   978-0-471-98303-3. Archived from the original on 9 May 2018.
  28. 1 2 3 Schroeder, Thomas (15 July 2023). "Left-Brain, Right-Brain Reconceptualized: A New Neuroscientific Understanding of an Old Divide". Medium.
  29. Goldberg, Elkhonon (2018). "A New Look at the Old Riddle: Novelty, Routines and the Evolution of the Bicameral Brain". Japanese Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 20 (3–4): 129–138.
  30. Westen D, Burton L, Kowalski K (2006). Psychology : Australian and New Zealand edition. Milton, Qld.: John Wiley & Sons. ISBN   9780470805527.
  31. Toga AW, Thompson PM (January 2003). "Mapping brain asymmetry". Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 4 (1): 37–48. doi:10.1038/nrn1009. PMID   12511860. S2CID   15867592.
  32. "Cognitive psychology - Brain Right hemisphere is random and left hemisphere is linear? Really?". Stack Exchange, Inc.
  33. Gazzaniga MS (July 2000). "Cerebral specialization and interhemispheric communication: does the corpus callosum enable the human condition?". Brain: A Journal of Neurology. 123 ( Pt 7) (7): 1293–326. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.7.1293 . PMID   10869045.
  34. "Schematic drawing showing how the left hemisphere differs from the right in mnemonic functions". The left is specialized for semantic processing while the right appears to be specialized for episodic memory.
  35. Harrington A (1 January 1989). Medicine, Mind, and the Double Brain: A Study in Nineteenth-Century Thought. Princeton University Press. pp. 87–90. ISBN   978-0691024226.
  36. Stiles A (2006). "Robert Louis Stevenson's "Jekyll and Hyde" and the Double Brain". SEL: Studies in English Literature 1500–1900. 46 (4): 879–900. doi:10.1353/sel.2006.0043. JSTOR   4127513. S2CID   161889732.
  37. Broca P (1865). "Sur le siège de la faculté du langage articulé". Bulletins et Mémoires de la Société d'Anthropologie de Paris. 6 (1): 377–393. doi:10.3406/bmsap.1865.9495. hdl: 11858/00-001M-0000-002A-E780-7 .
  38. Kandel E, Schwartz J, Jessel T. Principles of Neural Science. 4th ed. p1182. New York: McGrawHill; 2000. ISBN   0-8385-7701-6
  39. Sheppard SM, Hillis AE (December 2018). "That's right! Language comprehension beyond the left hemisphere". Brain: A Journal of Neurology. 141 (12): 3280–3289. doi:10.1093/brain/awy291. PMC   6262217 . PMID   30496360.
  40. Shmerling, Robert (25 August 2017). "Right brain/left brain, right?". Harvard Health Publishing. Harvard Medical School. Retrieved 24 March 2022.

Bibliography

Further resources