Pain

Last updated

Pain
Wrist pain.jpg
An illustration of wrist pain
Specialty Neurology
Pain medicine
Symptoms Unpleasant sensory and emotional sensations [1]
DurationTypically depends on the cause
TypesPhysical, psychological, psychogenic
Medication Analgesic

Pain is a distressing feeling often caused by intense or damaging stimuli. The International Association for the Study of Pain defines pain as "an unpleasant sensory and emotional experience associated with, or resembling that associated with, actual or potential tissue damage." [1]

Contents

Pain motivates organisms to withdraw from damaging situations, to protect a damaged body part while it heals, and to avoid similar experiences in the future. [2] Most pain resolves once the noxious stimulus is removed and the body has healed, but it may persist despite removal of the stimulus and apparent healing of the body. Sometimes pain arises in the absence of any detectable stimulus, damage or disease. [3]

Pain is the most common reason for physician consultation in most developed countries. [4] [5] It is a major symptom in many medical conditions, and can interfere with a person's quality of life and general functioning. [6] People in pain experience impaired concentration, working memory, mental flexibility, problem solving and information processing speed, and are more likely to experience irritability, depression, and anxiety.

Simple pain medications are useful in 20% to 70% of cases. [7] Psychological factors such as social support, cognitive behavioral therapy, excitement, or distraction can affect pain's intensity or unpleasantness. [8] [9]

Etymology

First attested in English in 1297, the word peyn comes from the Old French peine, in turn from Latin poena meaning "punishment, penalty" [10] [11] (also meaning "torment, hardship, suffering" in Late Latin) and that from Greek ποινή (poine), generally meaning "price paid, penalty, punishment". [12] [13]

Classification

The International Association for the Study of Pain recommends using specific features to describe a patient's pain:

  1. Region of the body involved (e.g. abdomen, lower limbs)
  2. System whose dysfunction may be causing the pain (e.g., nervous, gastrointestinal)
  3. Duration and pattern of occurrence
  4. Intensity
  5. Cause [14]

Chronic versus acute

Pain is usually transitory, lasting only until the noxious stimulus is removed or the underlying damage or pathology has healed. But some painful conditions, such as rheumatoid arthritis, peripheral neuropathy, cancer, and idiopathic pain, may persist for years. Pain that lasts a long time is called "chronic" or "persistent", and pain that resolves quickly is called "acute". Traditionally, the distinction between acute and chronic pain has relied upon an arbitrary interval of time between onset and resolution; the two most commonly used markers being 3 months and 6 months since the onset of pain, [15] though some theorists and researchers have placed the transition from acute to chronic pain at 12 months. [16] :93 Others apply "acute" to pain that lasts less than 30 days, "chronic" to pain of more than six months' duration, and "subacute" to pain that lasts from one to six months. [17] A popular alternative definition of "chronic pain", involving no arbitrarily fixed duration, is "pain that extends beyond the expected period of healing". [15] Chronic pain may be classified as "cancer-related" or "benign." [17]

Allodynia

Allodynia is pain experienced in response to a normally painless stimulus. [18] It has no biological function and is classified by characteristics of the stimuli as cold, heat, touch, pressure or a pinprick. [18] [19]

Phantom

Phantom pain is pain felt in a part of the body that has been amputated, or from which the brain no longer receives signals. It is a type of neuropathic pain. [20]

The prevalence of phantom pain in upper limb amputees is nearly 82%, and in lower limb amputees is 54%. [20] One study found that eight days after amputation, 72% of patients had phantom limb pain, and six months later, 67% reported it. [21] [22] Some amputees experience continuous pain that varies in intensity or quality; others experience several bouts of pain per day, or it may reoccur less often. It is often described as shooting, crushing, burning or cramping. If the pain is continuous for a long period, parts of the intact body may become sensitized, so that touching them evokes pain in the phantom limb. Phantom limb pain may accompany urination or defecation. [23] :61–69

Local anesthetic injections into the nerves or sensitive areas of the stump may relieve pain for days, weeks, or sometimes permanently, despite the drug wearing off in a matter of hours; and small injections of hypertonic saline into the soft tissue between vertebrae produces local pain that radiates into the phantom limb for ten minutes or so and may be followed by hours, weeks, or even longer of partial or total relief from phantom pain. Vigorous vibration or electrical stimulation of the stump, or current from electrodes surgically implanted onto the spinal cord, all produce relief in some patients. [23] :61–69

Mirror box therapy produces the illusion of movement and touch in a phantom limb which in turn may cause a reduction in pain. [24]

Paraplegia, the loss of sensation and voluntary motor control after serious spinal cord damage, may be accompanied by girdle pain at the level of the spinal cord damage, visceral pain evoked by a filling bladder or bowel, or, in five to ten percent of paraplegics, phantom body pain in areas of complete sensory loss. This phantom body pain is initially described as burning or tingling but may evolve into severe crushing or pinching pain, or the sensation of fire running down the legs or of a knife twisting in the flesh. Onset may be immediate or may not occur until years after the disabling injury. Surgical treatment rarely provides lasting relief. [23] :61–69

Breakthrough

Breakthrough pain is transitory pain that comes on suddenly and is not alleviated by the patient's regular pain management. It is common in cancer patients who often have background pain that is generally well-controlled by medications, but who also sometimes experience bouts of severe pain that from time to time "breaks through" the medication. The characteristics of breakthrough cancer pain vary from person to person and according to the cause. Management of breakthrough pain can entail intensive use of opioids, including fentanyl. [25] [26]

Asymbolia and insensitivity

A patient and doctor discuss congenital insensitivity to pain.

The ability to experience pain is essential for protection from injury, and recognition of the presence of injury. Episodic analgesia may occur under special circumstances, such as in the excitement of sport or war: a soldier on the battlefield may feel no pain for many hours from a traumatic amputation or other severe injury. [27]

Although unpleasantness is an essential part of the IASP definition of pain, [28] it is possible in some patients to induce a state known as pain asymbolia, described as intense pain devoid of unpleasantness, with morphine injection or psychosurgery. [29] Such patients report that they have pain but are not bothered by it; they recognize the sensation of pain but suffer little, or not at all. [30] Indifference to pain can also rarely be present from birth; these people have normal nerves on medical investigations, and find pain unpleasant, but do not avoid repetition of the pain stimulus. [31]

Insensitivity to pain may also result from abnormalities in the nervous system. This is usually the result of acquired damage to the nerves, such as spinal cord injury, diabetes mellitus (diabetic neuropathy), or leprosy in countries where that disease is prevalent. [32] These individuals are at risk of tissue damage and infection due to undiscovered injuries. People with diabetes-related nerve damage, for instance, sustain poorly-healing foot ulcers as a result of decreased sensation. [33]

A much smaller number of people are insensitive to pain due to an inborn abnormality of the nervous system, known as "congenital insensitivity to pain". [31] Children with this condition incur carelessly-repeated damage to their tongues, eyes, joints, skin, and muscles. Some die before adulthood, and others have a reduced life expectancy. [34] Most people with congenital insensitivity to pain have one of five hereditary sensory and autonomic neuropathies (which includes familial dysautonomia and congenital insensitivity to pain with anhidrosis). [35] These conditions feature decreased sensitivity to pain together with other neurological abnormalities, particularly of the autonomic nervous system. [31] [35] A very rare syndrome with isolated congenital insensitivity to pain has been linked with mutations in the SCN9A gene, which codes for a sodium channel (Nav1.7) necessary in conducting pain nerve stimuli. [36]

Functional effects

Experimental subjects challenged by acute pain and patients in chronic pain experience impairments in attention control, working memory capacity, mental flexibility, problem solving, and information processing speed. [37] Pain is also associated with increased depression, anxiety, fear, and anger. [38]

If I have matters right, the consequences of pain will include direct physical distress, unemployment, financial difficulties, marital disharmony, and difficulties in concentration and attention…

Harold Merskey 2000 [39]

On subsequent negative emotion

Although pain is considered to be aversive and unpleasant and is therefore usually avoided, a meta-analysis which summarized and evaluated numerous studies from various psychological disciplines, found a reduction in negative affect. Across studies, participants that were subjected to acute physical pain in the laboratory subsequently reported feeling better than those in non-painful control conditions, a finding which was also reflected in physiological parameters. [40] A potential mechanism to explain this effect is provided by the opponent-process theory.

Theory

Historical

Before the relatively recent discovery of neurons and their role in pain, various body functions were proposed to account for pain. There were several competing early theories of pain among the ancient Greeks: Hippocrates believed that it was due to an imbalance in vital fluids. [41] In the 11th century, Avicenna theorized that there were a number of feeling senses, including touch, pain, and titillation. [42]

Portrait of Rene Descartes by Jan Baptist Weenix, 1647-1649 Jan Baptist Weenix - Portrait of Rene Descartes.jpg
Portrait of René Descartes by Jan Baptist Weenix, 1647–1649

In 1644, René Descartes theorized that pain was a disturbance that passed along nerve fibers until the disturbance reached the brain. [41] [43] The work of Descartes and Avicenna prefigured the 19th-century development of specificity theory. Specificity theory saw pain as "a specific sensation, with its own sensory apparatus independent of touch and other senses". [44] Another theory that came to prominence in the 18th and 19th centuries was intensive theory, which conceived of pain not as a unique sensory modality, but an emotional state produced by stronger than normal stimuli such as intense light, pressure or temperature. [45] By the mid-1890s, specificity was backed primarily by physiologists and physicians, and psychologists mostly backed the intensive theory. However, after a series of clinical observations by Henry Head and experiments by Max von Frey, the psychologists migrated to specificity almost en masse. By the century's end, most physiology and psychology textbooks presented pain specificity as fact. [42] [44]

Modern

Regions of the cerebral cortex associated with pain Schematic of cortical areas involved with pain processing and fMRI.jpg
Regions of the cerebral cortex associated with pain

Some sensory fibers do not differentiate between noxious and non-noxious stimuli, while others (i.e., nociceptors) respond only to noxious, high-intensity stimuli. At the peripheral end of the nociceptor, noxious stimuli generate currents that, above a given threshold, send signals along the nerve fiber to the spinal cord. The "specificity" (whether it responds to thermal, chemical, or mechanical features of its environment) of a nociceptor is determined by which ion channels it expresses at its peripheral end. So far, dozens of types of nociceptor ion channels have been identified, and their exact functions are still being determined. [46]

The pain signal travels from the periphery to the spinal cord along A-delta and C fibers. Because the A-delta fiber is thicker than the C fiber, and is thinly sheathed in an electrically insulating material (myelin), it carries its signal faster (5–30  m/s) than the unmyelinated C fiber (0.5–2 m/s). [47] Pain evoked by the A-delta fibers is described as sharp and is felt first. This is followed by a duller pain—often described as burning—carried by the C fibers. [48] These A-delta and C fibers enter the spinal cord via Lissauer's tract and connect with spinal cord nerve fibers in the central gelatinous substance of the spinal cord. These spinal cord fibers then cross the cord via the anterior white commissure and ascend in the spinothalamic tract. Before reaching the brain, the spinothalamic tract splits into the lateral, neospinothalamic tract and the medial, paleospinothalamic tract. The neospinothalamic tract carries the fast, sharp A-delta signal to the ventral posterolateral nucleus of the thalamus. The paleospinothalamic tract carries the slow, dull C fiber pain signal. Some of the paleospinothalamic fibers peel off in the brain stem—connecting with the reticular formation or midbrain periaqueductal gray—and the remainder terminate in the intralaminar nuclei of the thalamus. [49]

Pain-related activity in the thalamus spreads to the insular cortex (thought to embody, among other things, the feeling that distinguishes pain from other homeostatic emotions such as itch and nausea) and anterior cingulate cortex (thought to embody, among other things, the affective/motivational element, the unpleasantness of pain), [50] and pain that is distinctly located also activates the primary and secondary somatosensory cortex. [51]

Spinal cord fibers dedicated to carrying A-delta fiber pain signals and others that carry both A-delta and C fiber pain signals to the thalamus have been identified. Other spinal cord fibers, known as wide dynamic range neurons, respond to A-delta and C fibers and the much larger, more heavily myelinated A-beta fibers that carry touch, pressure, and vibration signals. [47]

Ronald Melzack and Patrick Wall introduced their gate control theory in the 1965 Science article "Pain Mechanisms: A New Theory". [52] The authors proposed that the thin C and A-delta (pain) and large diameter A-beta (touch, pressure, vibration) nerve fibers carry information from the site of injury to two destinations in the dorsal horn of the spinal cord, and that A-beta fiber signals acting on inhibitory cells in the dorsal horn can reduce the intensity of pain signals sent to the brain. [43]

Three dimensions of pain

In 1968, Ronald Melzack and Kenneth Casey described chronic pain in terms of its three dimensions:

  • "sensory-discriminative" (sense of the intensity, location, quality, and duration of the pain),
  • "affective-motivational" (unpleasantness and urge to escape the unpleasantness) and
  • "cognitive-evaluative" (cognitions such as appraisal, cultural values, distraction, and hypnotic suggestion).

They theorized that pain intensity (the sensory discriminative dimension) and unpleasantness (the affective-motivational dimension) are not simply determined by the magnitude of the painful stimulus, but "higher" cognitive activities can influence perceived intensity and unpleasantness. Cognitive activities may affect both sensory and affective experience, or they may modify primarily the affective-motivational dimension. Thus, excitement in games or war appears to block both the sensory-discriminative and affective-motivational dimensions of pain, while suggestion and placebos may modulate only the affective-motivational dimension and leave the sensory-discriminative dimension relatively undisturbed. [53] (p. 432)

The paper ends with a call to action: "Pain can be treated not only by trying to cut down the sensory input by anesthetic block, surgical intervention and the like, but also by influencing the motivational-affective and cognitive factors as well." [53] (p. 435)

Evolutionary and behavioral role

Pain is part of the body's defense system, producing a reflexive retraction from the painful stimulus, and tendencies to protect the affected body part while it heals, and avoid that harmful situation in the future. [54] [55] It is an important part of animal life, vital to healthy survival. People with congenital insensitivity to pain have reduced life expectancy. [31]

In The Greatest Show on Earth: The Evidence for Evolution , biologist Richard Dawkins addresses the question of why pain should have the quality of being painful. He describes the alternative as a mental raising of a "red flag". To argue why that red flag might be insufficient, Dawkins argues that drives must compete with one another within living beings. The most "fit" creature would be the one whose pains are well balanced. Those pains which mean certain death when ignored will become the most powerfully felt. The relative intensities of pain, then, may resemble the relative importance of that risk to our ancestors. [a] This resemblance will not be perfect, however, because natural selection can be a poor designer. This may have maladaptive results such as supernormal stimuli. [56]

Pain, however, does not only wave a "red flag" within living beings but may also act as a warning sign and a call for help to other living beings. Especially in humans who readily helped each other in case of sickness or injury throughout their evolutionary history, pain might be shaped by natural selection to be a credible and convincing signal of the need for relief, help, and care. [57]

Idiopathic pain (pain that persists after the trauma or pathology has healed, or that arises without any apparent cause) may be an exception to the idea that pain is helpful to survival, although some psychodynamic psychologists argue that such pain is psychogenic, enlisted as a protective distraction to keep dangerous emotions unconscious. [58]

Thresholds

In pain science, thresholds are measured by gradually increasing the intensity of a stimulus in a procedure called quantitative sensory testing which involves such stimuli as electric current, thermal (heat or cold), mechanical (pressure, touch, vibration), ischemic, or chemical stimuli applied to the subject to evoke a response. [59] The "pain perception threshold" is the point at which the subject begins to feel pain, and the "pain threshold intensity" is the stimulus intensity at which the stimulus begins to hurt. The "pain tolerance threshold" is reached when the subject acts to stop the pain. [59]

Assessment

A person's self-report is the most reliable measure of pain. [60] [61] [62] Some health care professionals may underestimate pain severity. [63] A definition of pain widely employed in nursing, emphasizing its subjective nature and the importance of believing patient reports, was introduced by Margo McCaffery in 1968: "Pain is whatever the experiencing person says it is, existing whenever he says it does". [64] To assess intensity, the patient may be asked to locate their pain on a scale of 0 to 10, with 0 being no pain at all, and 10 the worst pain they have ever felt. Quality can be established by having the patient complete the McGill Pain Questionnaire indicating which words best describe their pain. [6]

Visual analogue scale

The visual analogue scale is a common, reproducible tool in the assessment of pain and pain relief. [65] The scale is a continuous line anchored by verbal descriptors, one for each extreme of pain where a higher score indicates greater pain intensity. It is usually 10 cm in length with no intermediate descriptors as to avoid marking of scores around a preferred numeric value. When applied as a pain descriptor, these anchors are often 'no pain' and 'worst imaginable pain". Cut-offs for pain classification have been recommended as no pain (0–4mm), mild pain (5–44mm), moderate pain (45–74mm) and severe pain (75–100mm). [66] [ check quotation syntax ]

Multidimensional pain inventory

The Multidimensional Pain Inventory (MPI) is a questionnaire designed to assess the psychosocial state of a person with chronic pain. Combining the MPI characterization of the person with their IASP five-category pain profile is recommended for deriving the most useful case description. [15]

Assessment in non-verbal people

Non-verbal people cannot use words to tell others that they are experiencing pain. However, they may be able to communicate through other means, such as blinking, pointing, or nodding. [67]

With a non-communicative person, observation becomes critical, and specific behaviors can be monitored as pain indicators. Behaviors such as facial grimacing and guarding (trying to protect part of the body from being bumped or touched) indicate pain, as well as an increase or decrease in vocalizations, changes in routine behavior patterns and mental status changes. Patients experiencing pain may exhibit withdrawn social behavior and possibly experience a decreased appetite and decreased nutritional intake. A change in condition that deviates from baseline, such as moaning with movement or when manipulating a body part, and limited range of motion are also potential pain indicators. In patients who possess language but are incapable of expressing themselves effectively, such as those with dementia, an increase in confusion or display of aggressive behaviors or agitation may signal that discomfort exists, and further assessment is necessary. Changes in behavior may be noticed by caregivers who are familiar with the person's normal behavior. [67]

Infants do feel pain, but lack the language needed to report it, and so communicate distress by crying. A non-verbal pain assessment should be conducted involving the parents, who will notice changes in the infant which may not be obvious to the health care provider. Pre-term babies are more sensitive to painful stimuli than those carried to full term. [68]

Another approach, when pain is suspected, is to give the person treatment for pain, and then watch to see whether the suspected indicators of pain subside. [67]

Other reporting barriers

The way in which one experiences and responds to pain is related to sociocultural characteristics, such as gender, ethnicity, and age. [69] [70] An aging adult may not respond to pain in the same way that a younger person might. Their ability to recognize pain may be blunted by illness or the use of medication. Depression may also keep older adult from reporting they are in pain. Decline in self-care may also indicate the older adult is experiencing pain. They may be reluctant to report pain because they do not want to be perceived as weak, or may feel it is impolite or shameful to complain, or they may feel the pain is a form of deserved punishment. [71] [72]

Cultural barriers may also affect the likelihood of reporting pain. Patients may feel that certain treatments go against their religious beliefs. They may not report pain because they feel it is a sign that death is near. Many people fear the stigma of addiction, and avoid pain treatment so as not to be prescribed potentially addicting drugs. Many Asians do not want to lose respect in society by admitting they are in pain and need help, believing the pain should be borne in silence, while other cultures feel they should report pain immediately to receive immediate relief. [68]

Gender can also be a perceived factor in reporting pain. Gender differences can be the result of social and cultural expectations, with women expected to be more emotional and show pain, and men more stoic. [68] As a result, female pain is often stigmatized, leading to less urgent treatment of women based on social expectations of their ability to accurately report it. [73] This leads to extended emergency room wait times for women and frequent dismissal of their ability to accurately report pain. [74] [75]

Diagnostic aid

Pain is a symptom of many medical conditions. Knowing the time of onset, location, intensity, pattern of occurrence (continuous, intermittent, etc.), exacerbating and relieving factors, and quality (burning, sharp, etc.) of the pain will help the examining physician to accurately diagnose the problem. For example, chest pain described as extreme heaviness may indicate myocardial infarction, while chest pain described as tearing may indicate aortic dissection. [76] [77]

Physiological measurement

Functional magnetic resonance imaging brain scanning has been used to measure pain, and correlates well with self-reported pain. [78] [79] [80]

Mechanisms

Nociceptive

Mechanism of nociceptive pain Nociceptive pain.jpg
Mechanism of nociceptive pain

Nociceptive pain is caused by stimulation of sensory nerve fibers that respond to stimuli approaching or exceeding harmful intensity (nociceptors), and may be classified according to the mode of noxious stimulation. The most common categories are "thermal" (e.g. heat or cold), "mechanical" (e.g. crushing, tearing, shearing, etc.) and "chemical" (e.g. iodine in a cut or chemicals released during inflammation). Some nociceptors respond to more than one of these modalities and are consequently designated polymodal.

Nociceptive pain may also be classed according to the site of origin and divided into "visceral", "deep somatic" and "superficial somatic" pain. Visceral structures (e.g., the heart, liver and intestines) are highly sensitive to stretch, ischemia and inflammation, but relatively insensitive to other stimuli that normally evoke pain in other structures, such as burning and cutting. Visceral pain is diffuse, difficult to locate and often referred to a distant, usually superficial, structure. It may be accompanied by nausea and vomiting and may be described as sickening, deep, squeezing, and dull. [81] Deep somatic pain is initiated by stimulation of nociceptors in ligaments, tendons, bones, blood vessels, fasciae and muscles, and is dull, aching, poorly-localized pain. Examples include sprains and broken bones. Superficial somatic pain is initiated by activation of nociceptors in the skin or other superficial tissue, and is sharp, well-defined and clearly located. Examples of injuries that produce superficial somatic pain include minor wounds and minor (first degree) burns. [16]

Neuropathic

Neuropathic pain is caused by damage or disease affecting any part of the nervous system involved in bodily feelings (the somatosensory system). [82] Neuropathic pain may be divided into peripheral, central, or mixed (peripheral and central) neuropathic pain. Peripheral neuropathic pain is often described as "burning", "tingling", "electrical", "stabbing", or "pins and needles". [83] Bumping the "funny bone" elicits acute peripheral neuropathic pain.

Some manifestations of neuropathic pain include: traumatic neuropathy, tic douloureux, painful diabetic neuropathy, and postherpetic neuralgia. [84]

Nociplastic

Nociplastic pain is pain characterized by a changed nociception (but without evidence of real or threatened tissue damage, or without disease or damage in the somatosensory system). [9]

Psychogenic

Psychogenic pain, also called psychalgia or somatoform pain, is pain caused, increased or prolonged by mental, emotional or behavioral factors. [85] Headaches, back pain and stomach pain are sometimes diagnosed as psychogenic. [85] Those affected are often stigmatized, because both medical professionals and the general public tend to think that pain from a psychological source is not "real". However, specialists consider that it is no less actual or hurtful than pain from any other source. [29]

People with long-term pain frequently display psychological disturbance, with elevated scores on the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory scales of hysteria, depression and hypochondriasis (the "neurotic triad"). Some investigators have argued that it is this neuroticism that causes acute pain to turn chronic, but clinical evidence points in the other direction, to chronic pain causing neuroticism. When long-term pain is relieved by therapeutic intervention, scores on the neurotic triad and anxiety fall, often to normal levels. Self-esteem, often low in chronic pain patients, also shows improvement once pain has resolved. [23] :31–32

Management

Pain can be treated through a variety of methods. The most appropriate method depends upon the situation. Management of chronic pain can be difficult and may require the coordinated efforts of a pain management team, which typically includes medical practitioners, clinical pharmacists, clinical psychologists, physiotherapists, occupational therapists, physician assistants, and nurse practitioners. [86]

Inadequate treatment of pain is widespread throughout surgical wards, intensive care units, and accident and emergency departments, in general practice, in the management of all forms of chronic pain including cancer pain, and in end of life care. [87] [88] [89] [90] [91] [92] [93] This neglect extends to all ages, from newborns to medically frail elderly. [94] [95] In the US, African and Hispanic Americans are more likely than others to suffer unnecessarily while in the care of a physician; [96] [97] and women's pain is more likely to be undertreated than men's. [98]

The International Association for the Study of Pain advocates that the relief of pain should be recognized as a human right, that chronic pain should be considered a disease in its own right, and that pain medicine should have the full status of a medical specialty. [99] It is a specialty only in China and Australia at this time. [100] Elsewhere, pain medicine is a subspecialty under disciplines such as anesthesiology, physiatry, neurology, palliative medicine and psychiatry. [101] In 2011, Human Rights Watch alerted that tens of millions of people worldwide are still denied access to inexpensive medications for severe pain. [102]

Medication

Acute pain is usually managed with medications such as analgesics and anesthetics. [103] Caffeine when added to pain medications such as ibuprofen, may provide some additional benefit. [104] [105] Ketamine can be used instead of opioids for short-term pain. [106] Pain medications can cause paradoxical side effects, such as opioid-induced hyperalgesia (severe generalized pain caused by long-term opioid use). [107] [108]

Sugar (sucrose) when taken by mouth reduces pain in newborn babies undergoing some medical procedures (a lancing of the heel, venipuncture, and intramuscular injections). Sugar does not remove pain from circumcision, and it is unknown if sugar reduces pain for other procedures. [109] Sugar did not affect pain-related electrical activity in the brains of newborns one second after the heel lance procedure. [110] Sweet liquid by mouth moderately reduces the rate and duration of crying caused by immunization injection in children between one and twelve months of age. [111]

Psychological

Individuals with more social support experience less cancer pain, take less pain medication, report less labor pain and are less likely to use epidural anesthesia during childbirth, or suffer from chest pain after coronary artery bypass surgery. [8]

Suggestion can significantly affect pain intensity. About 35% of people report marked relief after receiving a saline injection they believed to be morphine. This placebo effect is more pronounced in people who are prone to anxiety, and so anxiety reduction may account for some of the effect, but it does not account for all of it. Placebos are more effective for intense pain than mild pain; and they produce progressively weaker effects with repeated administration. [23] :26–28 It is possible for many with chronic pain to become so absorbed in an activity or entertainment that the pain is no longer felt, or is greatly diminished. [23] :22–23

A number of meta-analyses have found clinical hypnosis to be effective in controlling pain associated with diagnostic and surgical procedures in both adults and children, as well as pain associated with cancer and childbirth. [112] A 2007 review of 13 studies found evidence for the efficacy of hypnosis in the reduction of chronic pain under some conditions, though the number of patients enrolled in the studies was low, raising issues related to the statistical power to detect group differences, and most lacked credible controls for placebo or expectation. The authors concluded that "although the findings provide support for the general applicability of hypnosis in the treatment of chronic pain, considerably more research will be needed to fully determine the effects of hypnosis for different chronic-pain conditions." [113]

Alternative medicine

An analysis of the 13 highest quality studies of pain treatment with acupuncture, published in January 2009, concluded there was little difference in the effect of real, fake and no acupuncture. [114] However, more recent reviews have found some benefit. [115] [116] [117]

Additionally, there is tentative evidence for a few herbal medicines. [118]

For chronic (long-term) lower back pain, spinal manipulation produces tiny, clinically insignificant, short-term improvements in pain and function, compared with sham therapy and other interventions. [119] Spinal manipulation produces the same outcome as other treatments, such as general practitioner care, pain-relief drugs, physical therapy, and exercise, for acute (short-term) lower back pain. [119]

There has been some interest in the relationship between vitamin D and pain, but the evidence so far from controlled trials for such a relationship, other than in osteomalacia, is inconclusive. [120]

The International Association for the Study of Pain (IASP) says that due to a lack of evidence from high quality research, it does not endorse the general use of cannabinoids to treat pain. [121]

Epidemiology

Pain is the main reason for visiting an emergency department in more than 50% of cases, [122] and is present in 30% of family practice visits. [123] Several epidemiological studies have reported widely varying prevalence rates for chronic pain, ranging from 12 to 80% of the population. [124] It becomes more common as people approach death. A study of 4,703 patients found that 26% had pain in the last two years of life, increasing to 46% in the last month. [125]

A survey of 6,636 children (0–18 years of age) found that, of the 5,424 respondents, 54% had experienced pain in the preceding three months. A quarter reported having experienced recurrent or continuous pain for three months or more, and a third of these reported frequent and intense pain. The intensity of chronic pain was higher for girls, and girls' reports of chronic pain increased markedly between ages 12 and 14. [126]

Society and culture

Physical pain is a universal experience, and a strong motivator of human and animal behavior. As such, physical pain is used politically in relation to various issues such as pain management policy, drug control, animal rights or animal welfare, torture, and pain compliance. The deliberate infliction of pain and the medical management of pain are both important aspects of biopower, a concept that encompasses the "set of mechanisms through which the basic biological features of the human species became the object of a political strategy". [127]

In various contexts, the deliberate infliction of pain in the form of corporal punishment is used as retribution for an offence, for the purpose of disciplining or reforming a wrongdoer, or to deter attitudes or behaviour deemed unacceptable. In Western societies, the intentional infliction of severe pain (torture) was principally used to extract confession prior to its abolition in the latter part of the 19th century. Torture as a means to punish the citizen has been reserved for offences posing a severe threat to the social fabric (for example, treason). [128]

The administration of torture on bodies othered by the cultural narrative, those observed as not 'full members of society' [128] :101–121[AD1] met a resurgence in the 20th century, possibly due to the heightened warfare. [128] :101–121 [AD2]

Many cultures use painful ritual practices as a catalyst for psychological transformation. [129] The use of pain to transition to a 'cleansed and purified' state is seen in religious self-flagellation practices (particularly those of Christianity and Islam), or personal catharsis in neo-primitive body suspension experiences. [130]

Beliefs about pain play an important role in sporting cultures. Pain may be viewed positively, exemplified by the 'no pain, no gain' attitude, with pain seen as an essential part of training. Sporting culture tends to normalise experiences of pain and injury and celebrate athletes who 'play hurt'. [131]

Pain has psychological, social, and physical dimensions, and is greatly influenced by cultural factors. [132]

Non-humans

René Descartes argued that animals lack consciousness and therefore do not experience pain and suffering in the way that humans do. [133] Bernard Rollin of Colorado State University, the principal author of two U.S. federal laws regulating pain relief for animals, [b] wrote that researchers remained unsure into the 1980s as to whether animals experience pain, and that veterinarians trained in the U.S. before 1989 were simply taught to ignore animal pain. [135] [136] The ability of invertebrate species of animals, such as insects, to feel pain and suffering is unclear. [137] [138] [139]

Specialists believe that all vertebrates can feel pain, and that certain invertebrates, like the octopus, may also. [137] [140] [141] The presence of pain in animals is unknown, but can be inferred through physical and behavioral reactions, [142] such as paw withdrawal from various noxious mechanical stimuli in rodents. [143]

While plants, as living beings, can perceive and communicate physical stimuli and damage, they do not feel pain simply because of the lack of any pain receptors, nerves, or a brain, [144] and, by extension, a lack of consciousness. [145] Many plants are known to perceive and respond to mechanical stimuli at a cellular level, and some plants such as the venus flytrap or touch-me-not, are known for their "obvious sensory abilities". [144] Nevertheless, no member of the plant kingdom does feel pain notwithstanding their abilities to respond to sunlight, gravity, wind, and any external stimuli such as insect bites since they lack any nervous system. The primary reason for this is that, unlike the members of the animal kingdom whose evolutionary successes and failures are shaped by suffering, the evolution of plants are simply shaped by life and death. [144]

See also

Explanatory notes

  1. For example, lack of food, extreme cold, or serious injuries are felt as exceptionally painful, whereas minor damage is felt as mere discomfort.
  2. Rollin drafted the 1985 Health Research Extension Act and an animal welfare amendment to the 1985 Food Security Act. [134]

Related Research Articles

In physiology, nociception, also nocioception; from Latin nocere 'to harm/hurt') is the sensory nervous system's process of encoding noxious stimuli. It deals with a series of events and processes required for an organism to receive a painful stimulus, convert it to a molecular signal, and recognize and characterize the signal to trigger an appropriate defensive response.

Chronic pain or chronic pain syndrome is a type of pain that is also known by other titles such as gradual burning pain, electrical pain, throbbing pain, and nauseating pain. This type of pain is sometimes confused with acute pain and can last from three months to several years; various diagnostic manuals such as DSM-5 and ICD-11 have proposed several definitions of chronic pain, but the accepted definition is that it is "pain that lasts longer than the expected period of recovery."

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Phantom limb</span> Sensation that an amputated or missing limb is attached

A phantom limb is the sensation that an amputated or missing limb is still attached. It is a chronic condition which is often resistant to treatment. When the cut ends of sensory fibres are stimulated during thigh movements, the patient feels as if the sensation is arising from the non-existent limb. Sometimes the patient might feel pain in the non-existent limb. Approximately 80–100% of individuals with an amputation experience sensations in their amputated limb. However, only a small percentage will experience painful phantom limb sensation. These sensations are relatively common in amputees and usually resolve within two to three years without treatment. Research continues to explore the underlying mechanisms of phantom limb pain (PLP) and effective treatment options.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Pain management</span> Interdisciplinary approach for easing pain

Pain management is an aspect of medicine and health care involving relief of pain in various dimensions, from acute and simple to chronic and challenging. Most physicians and other health professionals provide some pain control in the normal course of their practice, and for the more complex instances of pain, they also call on additional help from a specific medical specialty devoted to pain, which is called pain medicine.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Itch</span> Uncomfortable skin sensation

An itch is a sensation that causes a strong desire or reflex to scratch. Itches have resisted many attempts to be classified as any one type of sensory experience. Itches have many similarities to pain, and while both are unpleasant sensory experiences, their behavioral response patterns are different. Pain creates a withdrawal reflex, whereas itches leads to a scratch reflex.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Nociceptor</span> Sensory neuron that detects pain

A nociceptor is a sensory neuron that responds to damaging or potentially damaging stimuli by sending "possible threat" signals to the spinal cord and the brain. The brain creates the sensation of pain to direct attention to the body part, so the threat can be mitigated; this process is called nociception.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Peripheral neuropathy</span> Nervous system disease affecting nerves beyond the brain and spinal cord

Peripheral neuropathy, often shortened to neuropathy, refers to damage or disease affecting the nerves. Damage to nerves may impair sensation, movement, gland function, and/or organ function depending on which nerve fibers are affected. Neuropathies affecting motor, sensory, or autonomic nerve fibers result in different symptoms. More than one type of fiber may be affected simultaneously. Peripheral neuropathy may be acute or chronic, and may be reversible or permanent.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Sensory neuron</span> Nerve cell that converts environmental stimuli into corresponding internal stimuli

Sensory neurons, also known as afferent neurons, are neurons in the nervous system, that convert a specific type of stimulus, via their receptors, into action potentials or graded receptor potentials. This process is called sensory transduction. The cell bodies of the sensory neurons are located in the dorsal root ganglia of the spinal cord.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Hyperalgesia</span> Abnormally increased sensitivity to pain

Hyperalgesia is an abnormally increased sensitivity to pain, which may be caused by damage to nociceptors or peripheral nerves and can cause hypersensitivity to stimulus. Prostaglandins E and F are largely responsible for sensitizing the nociceptors. Temporary increased sensitivity to pain also occurs as part of sickness behavior, the evolved response to infection.

Neuropathic pain is pain caused by a lesion or disease of the somatosensory nervous system. Neuropathic pain may be associated with abnormal sensations called dysesthesia or pain from normally non-painful stimuli (allodynia). It may have continuous and/or episodic (paroxysmal) components. The latter resemble stabbings or electric shocks. Common qualities include burning or coldness, "pins and needles" sensations, numbness and itching.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Referred pain</span> Pain perceived at a location other than the site of the painful stimulus

Referred pain, also called reflective pain, is pain perceived at a location other than the site of the painful stimulus. An example is the case of angina pectoris brought on by a myocardial infarction, where pain is often felt in the left side of neck, left shoulder, and back rather than in the thorax (chest), the site of the injury. The International Association for the Study of Pain has not officially defined the term; hence, several authors have defined it differently. Referred pain has been described since the late 1880s. Despite an increasing amount of literature on the subject, the biological mechanism of referred pain is unknown, although there are several hypotheses.

Phantom pain is a painful perception that an individual experiences relating to a limb or an organ that is not physically part of the body, either because it was removed or was never there in the first place.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Allodynia</span> Feeling of pain from stimuli which do not normally elicit pain

Allodynia is a condition in which pain is caused by a stimulus that does not normally elicit pain. For example, sunburn can cause temporary allodynia, so that usually painless stimuli, such as wearing clothing or running cold or warm water over it, can be very painful. It is different from hyperalgesia, an exaggerated response from a normally painful stimulus. The term comes from Ancient Greek άλλος (állos) 'other' and οδύνη (odúnē) 'pain'.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Group C nerve fiber</span> One of three classes of nerve fiber in the nervous system

Group C nerve fibers are one of three classes of nerve fiber in the central nervous system (CNS) and peripheral nervous system (PNS). The C group fibers are unmyelinated and have a small diameter and low conduction velocity, whereas Groups A and B are myelinated. Group C fibers include postganglionic fibers in the autonomic nervous system (ANS), and nerve fibers at the dorsal roots. These fibers carry sensory information.

Na<sub>v</sub>1.8 Protein-coding gene in the species Homo sapiens

Nav1.8 is a sodium ion channel subtype that in humans is encoded by the SCN10A gene.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Wide dynamic range neuron</span>

The wide dynamic range (WDR) neuron was first discovered by Mendell in 1966. Early studies of this neuron established what is known as the gate control theory of pain. The basic concept is that non-painful stimuli block the pathways for painful stimuli, inhibiting possible painful responses. This theory was supported by the fact that WDR neurons are responsible for responses to both painful and non-painful stimuli, and the idea that these neurons could not produce more than one of these responses simultaneously. WDR neurons respond to all types of somatosensory stimuli, make up the majority of the neurons found in the posterior grey column, and have the ability to produce long range responses including those responsible for pain and itch.

Pain in cancer may arise from a tumor compressing or infiltrating nearby body parts; from treatments and diagnostic procedures; or from skin, nerve and other changes caused by a hormone imbalance or immune response. Most chronic (long-lasting) pain is caused by the illness and most acute (short-term) pain is caused by treatment or diagnostic procedures. However, radiotherapy, surgery and chemotherapy may produce painful conditions that persist long after treatment has ended.

Zucapsaicin (Civanex) is a medication used to treat osteoarthritis of the knee and other neuropathic pain. Zucapsaicin is a member of phenols and a member of methoxybenzenes. It is a modulator of transient receptor potential cation channel subfamily V member 1 (TRPV-1), also known as the vanilloid or capsaicin receptor 1 that reduces pain, and improves articular functions. It is the cis-isomer of capsaicin. Civamide, manufactured by Winston Pharmaceuticals, is produced in formulations for oral, nasal, and topical use.

Tactile induced analgesia is the phenomenon where concurrent touch and pain on the skin reduces the intensity of pain that is felt.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Nociplastic pain</span> Symptom

Nociplastic pain, also known as central sensitisation, is a third category of pain that is mechanistically distinct from nociceptive pain, which is due to inflammation and tissue damage, and neuropathic pain, which is due to nerve damage. It may occur in combination with the other types of pain or in isolation. Its location may be generalised or multifocal and it can be more intense than would be expected from any associated physical cause.

References

  1. 1 2 Raja SN, Carr DB, Cohen M, Finnerup NB, Flor H, Gibson S, et al. (September 2020). "The revised International Association for the Study of Pain definition of pain: concepts, challenges, and compromises". Pain. 161 (9): 1976–1982. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001939 . PMC   7680716 . PMID   32694387.
  2. Cervero F (2012). Understanding Pain : Exploring the Perception of Pain. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. pp. Chapter 1. ISBN   9780262305433. OCLC   809043366.
  3. Raj PP (2007). "Taxonomy and classification of pain". In: The Handbook of Chronic Pain. Nova Biomedical Books. ISBN   9781600210440. Archived from the original on 30 March 2021. Retrieved 3 February 2016.
  4. Debono DJ, Hoeksema LJ, Hobbs RD (August 2013). "Caring for patients with chronic pain: pearls and pitfalls". The Journal of the American Osteopathic Association. 113 (8): 620–7. doi: 10.7556/jaoa.2013.023 . PMID   23918913.
  5. Turk DC, Dworkin RH (2004). "What should be the core outcomes in chronic pain clinical trials?". Arthritis Research & Therapy. 6 (4): 151–4. doi: 10.1186/ar1196 . PMC   464897 . PMID   15225358.
  6. 1 2 Breivik H, Borchgrevink PC, Allen SM, Rosseland LA, Romundstad L, Hals EK, Kvarstein G, Stubhaug A (July 2008). "Assessment of pain". British Journal of Anaesthesia. 101 (1): 17–24. doi: 10.1093/bja/aen103 . PMID   18487245.
  7. Moore RA, Wiffen PJ, Derry S, Maguire T, Roy YM, Tyrrell L (November 2015). "Non-prescription (OTC) oral analgesics for acute pain – an overview of Cochrane reviews". The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. 11 (11): CD010794. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD010794.pub2. PMC   6485506 . PMID   26544675.
  8. 1 2 Eisenberger NI, Lieberman M (2005). "Why it hurts to be left out: The neurocognitive overlap between physical and social pain". In Williams KD (ed.). The Social Outcast: Ostracism, Social Exclusion, Rejection, & Bullying (Sydney Symposium of Social Psychology). East Sussex: Psychology Press. p. 210. ISBN   9781841694245.
  9. 1 2 Garland EL, Brintz CE, Hanley AW, Roseen EJ, Atchley RM, Gaylord SA, et al. (January 2020). "Mind-Body Therapies for Opioid-Treated Pain: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis". JAMA Internal Medicine. 180 (1): 91–105. doi:10.1001/jamainternmed.2019.4917. PMC   6830441 . PMID   31682676.
  10. Lewis CT, Short C. "Poena". A Latin Dictionary. Archived from the original on 13 May 2011 via Perseus Digital Library.
  11. Lavoie, Anne; Toledo, Paloma (1 September 2013). "Multimodal Postcesarean Delivery Analgesia". Clinics in Perinatology. Pain Management in the Peripartum Period. 40 (3): 443–455. doi:10.1016/j.clp.2013.05.008. ISSN   0095-5108. PMID   23972750.
  12. Liddell HG, Scott R. "ποινή". A Greek-English Lexicon. Archived from the original on 13 May 2011 via Perseus Digital Library.
  13. "Pain". Archived from the original on 28 July 2011 via Online Etymology Dictionary.
  14. Merskey H, Bogduk N (1994). Classification of Chronic Pain (2 nd ed.). Seattle: International Association for the Study of Pain. pp.  3 & 4. ISBN   978-0931092053.
  15. 1 2 3 Turk DC, Okifuji A (2001). "Pain terms and taxonomies of pain". In Bonica JJ, Loeser JD, Chapman CR, Turk DC (eds.). Bonica's management of pain. Hagerstwon, MD: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. ISBN   978-0781768276.
  16. 1 2 Coda BA, Bonica JJ (2000). "General considerations of acute pain" . In Panswick CC, Main CJ (eds.). Pain management: an interdisciplinary approach. Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone. ISBN   978-0443056833.
  17. 1 2 Thienhaus O, Cole BE (2002). "Classification of pain". In Weiner R (ed.). Pain management: a practical guide for clinicians . Boca Raton: CRC Press. pp.  28. ISBN   978-0849322624.
  18. 1 2 Jensen TS, Finnerup NB (September 2014). "Allodynia and hyperalgesia in neuropathic pain: clinical manifestations and mechanisms". The Lancet. Neurology. 13 (9): 924–935. doi:10.1016/s1474-4422(14)70102-4. PMID   25142459. S2CID   25011309.
  19. Lolignier S, Eijkelkamp N, Wood JN (January 2015). "Mechanical allodynia". Pflügers Archiv. 467 (1): 133–139. doi:10.1007/s00424-014-1532-0. PMC   4281368 . PMID   24846747.
  20. 1 2 Kooijman CM, Dijkstra PU, Geertzen JH, Elzinga A, van der Schans CP (July 2000). "Phantom pain and phantom sensations in upper limb amputees: an epidemiological study". Pain. 87 (1): 33–41. doi:10.1016/S0304-3959(00)00264-5. PMID   10863043. S2CID   7565030.[ permanent dead link ]
  21. Jensen TS, Krebs B, Nielsen J, Rasmussen P (November 1983). "Phantom limb, phantom pain and stump pain in amputees during the first 6 months following limb amputation". Pain. 17 (3): 243–256. doi:10.1016/0304-3959(83)90097-0. PMID   6657285. S2CID   10304696.
  22. Jensen TS, Krebs B, Nielsen J, Rasmussen P (March 1985). "Immediate and long-term phantom limb pain in amputees: incidence, clinical characteristics and relationship to pre-amputation limb pain". Pain. 21 (3): 267–278. doi:10.1016/0304-3959(85)90090-9. PMID   3991231. S2CID   24358789.
  23. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Wall PD, Melzack R (1996). The challenge of pain (2nd ed.). New York: Penguin Books. ISBN   978-0140256703.
  24. Ramachandran VS, Rogers-Ramachandran D (April 1996). "Synaesthesia in phantom limbs induced with mirrors". Proceedings. Biological Sciences. 263 (1369): 377–386. Bibcode:1996RSPSB.263..377R. doi:10.1098/rspb.1996.0058. PMID   8637922. S2CID   4819370.
  25. Mishra S, Bhatnagar S, Chaudhary P, Rana SP (January 2009). "Breakthrough cancer pain: review of prevalence, characteristics and management". Indian Journal of Palliative Care. 15 (1): 14–18. doi: 10.4103/0973-1075.53506 . PMC   2886208 . PMID   20606850.
  26. Caraceni A, Hanks G, Kaasa S, Bennett MI, Brunelli C, Cherny N, Dale O, De Conno F, Fallon M, Hanna M, Haugen DF, Juhl G, King S, Klepstad P, Laugsand EA, Maltoni M, Mercadante S, Nabal M, Pigni A, Radbruch L, Reid C, Sjogren P, Stone PC, Tassinari D, Zeppetella G (February 2012). "Use of opioid analgesics in the treatment of cancer pain: evidence-based recommendations from the EAPC" (PDF). The Lancet. Oncology. 13 (2): e58–68. doi:10.1016/S1470-2045(12)70040-2. PMID   22300860. Archived from the original (PDF) on 19 October 2014. Retrieved 7 March 2022.
  27. Beecher HK (1959). Measurement of subjective responses . New York: Oxford University Press. cited in Melzack R, Wall PD (1996). The challenge of pain (2nd ed.). London: Penguin. p. 7. ISBN   978-0140256703.
  28. "International Association for the Study of Pain: Pain Definitions". Archived from the original on 13 January 2015. Retrieved 12 January 2015. Pain is an unpleasant sensory and emotional experience associated with actual or potential tissue damage, or described in terms of such damage Alt URL [ permanent dead link ] Derived from Bonica JJ (June 1979). "The need of a taxonomy". Pain. 6 (3): 247–248. doi:10.1016/0304-3959(79)90046-0. PMID   460931. S2CID   53161389.
  29. 1 2 "International Association for the Study of Pain | Pain Definitions".. Retrieved 12 October 2010.
  30. Grahek N (2007). Feeling pain and being in pain (2nd ed.). Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. ISBN   978-0262517324. Archived from the original on 27 September 2008.
  31. 1 2 3 4 Nagasako EM, Oaklander AL, Dworkin RH (February 2003). "Congenital insensitivity to pain: an update". Pain. 101 (3): 213–219. doi:10.1016/S0304-3959(02)00482-7. PMID   12583863. S2CID   206055264.
  32. Brand PW, Yancey P (1997). The gift of pain: why we hurt & what we can do about it. Grand Rapids, Mich: Zondervan Publ. ISBN   978-0310221449.
  33. Lipsky BA, Berendt AR, Deery HG, Embil JM, Joseph WS, Karchmer AW, LeFrock JL, Lew DP, Mader JT, Norden C, Tan JS (October 2004). "Diagnosis and treatment of diabetic foot infections". Clinical Infectious Diseases. 39 (7): 885–910. doi: 10.1086/424846 . PMID   15472838.
  34. Raina*, Triveni; Dash, Bhagya Ranjan. "AN INTRODUCTORY APPROACH TO PAIN MANAGEMENT THROUGH AYURVEDA WITH BRIEF HOLISTIC REVIEW".{{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help)
  35. 1 2 Axelrod FB, Hilz MJ (December 2003). "Inherited autonomic neuropathies". Seminars in Neurology. 23 (4): 381–390. doi:10.1055/s-2004-817722. PMID   15088259. S2CID   260317729.
  36. Raouf R, Quick K, Wood JN (November 2010). "Pain as a channelopathy". The Journal of Clinical Investigation. 120 (11): 3745–3752. doi:10.1172/JCI43158. PMC   2965577 . PMID   21041956.
  37. Hart RP, Wade JB, Martelli MF (April 2003). "Cognitive impairment in patients with chronic pain: the significance of stress". Current Pain and Headache Reports. 7 (2): 116–126. doi:10.1007/s11916-003-0021-5. PMID   12628053. S2CID   14104974.
  38. Bruehl S, Burns JW, Chung OY, Chont M (March 2009). "Pain-related effects of trait anger expression: neural substrates and the role of endogenous opioid mechanisms". Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 33 (3): 475–491. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.12.003. PMC   2756489 . PMID   19146872.
  39. Merskey H (2000). "The History of Psychoanalytic Ideas Concerning Pain" . In Weisberg JN, Gatchel RJ (eds.). Personality Characteristics of Patients With Pain. American Psychological Association (APA). ISBN   978-1557986467.
  40. Bresin K, Kling L, Verona E (2018). "The effect of acute physical pain on subsequent negative emotional affect: A meta-analysis". Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment. 9 (3): 273–283. doi:10.1037/per0000248. PMC   5624817 . PMID   28368146.
  41. 1 2 Linton. Models of Pain Perception. Elsevier Health, 2005. Print.
  42. 1 2 Dallenbach KM (July 1939). "Pain: History and present status". American Journal of Psychology. 52 (3): 331–347. doi:10.2307/1416740. JSTOR   1416740.
  43. 1 2 Melzack R, Katz J (2004). "The Gate Control Theory: Reaching for the Brain". In Craig KD, Hadjistavropoulos T (eds.). Pain: psychological perspectives. Mahwah, N.J: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers. ISBN   978-0415650618.
  44. 1 2 Bonica JJ (1990). "History of pain concepts and therapies". The management of pain. Vol. 1 (2 ed.). London: Lea & Febiger. p. 7. ISBN   978-0812111224.
  45. Finger S (2001). Origins of neuroscience: a history of explorations into brain function. US: Oxford University Press. p. 149. ISBN   978-0195146943.
  46. Woolf CJ, Ma Q (August 2007). "Nociceptors noxious stimulus detectors". Neuron. 55 (3): 353–364. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.07.016 . PMID   17678850. S2CID   13576368.
  47. 1 2 Marchand S (2010). "Applied pain neurophysiology". In Beaulieu P, Lussier D, Porreca F, Dickenson A (eds.). Pharmacology of pain. Seattle: International Association for the Study of Pain Press. pp. 3–26. ISBN   978-0931092787.
  48. Skevington S (1995). Psychology of pain. New York: Wiley. p.  9. ISBN   978-0471957737.
  49. Skevington SM (1995). Psychology of pain. Chichester, UK: Wiley. p.  18. ISBN   978-0471957737.
  50. Craig AD (2003). "Pain mechanisms: labeled lines versus convergence in central processing". Annual Review of Neuroscience. 26: 1–30. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.26.041002.131022. PMID   12651967. S2CID   12387848.
  51. Romanelli P, Esposito V (July 2004). "The functional anatomy of neuropathic pain". Neurosurgery Clinics of North America. 15 (3): 257–268. doi:10.1016/j.nec.2004.02.010. PMID   15246335.
  52. Melzack R, Wall PD (November 1965). "Pain mechanisms: a new theory" (PDF). Science. 150 (3699): 971–979. Bibcode:1965Sci...150..971M. doi:10.1126/science.150.3699.971. PMID   5320816. Archived from the original (PDF) on 14 January 2012. Retrieved 7 March 2022.
  53. 1 2 Melzack, Ronald; Casey, Kenneth (1968). "Sensory, Motivational, and Central Control Determinants of Pain". In Kenshalo, Dan (ed.). The Skin Senses. Springfield, Illinois: Charles C Thomas.
  54. Lynn B (1984). "Cutaneous nociceptors". In Winlow W, Holden AV (eds.). The neurobiology of pain: Symposium of the Northern Neurobiology Group, held at Leeds on 18 April 1983. Manchester: Manchester University Press. p. 106. ISBN   978-0719009969. Archived from the original on 30 March 2021. Retrieved 3 February 2016.
  55. Bernston GG, Cacioppo JT (2007). "The neuroevolution of motivation". In Gardner WL, Shah JY (eds.). Handbook of Motivation Science. New York: The Guilford Press. p. 191. ISBN   978-1593855680. Archived from the original on 30 March 2021. Retrieved 18 November 2020.
  56. Dawkins R (2009). The Greatest Show on Earth . Free Press. pp.  392–395. ISBN   978-1416594789.
  57. Steinkopf L (June 2016). "An Evolutionary Perspective on Pain Communication". Evolutionary Psychology. 14 (2): 100. doi: 10.1177/1474704916653964 .
  58. Sarno JE (2006). The divided mind: the epidemic of mindbody disorders . New York: ReganBooks. ISBN   978-0061174308.
  59. 1 2 Fillingim RB, Loeser JD, Baron R, Edwards RR (September 2016). "Assessment of Chronic Pain: Domains, Methods, and Mechanisms". The Journal of Pain. 17 (9 Suppl): T10–20. doi:10.1016/j.jpain.2015.08.010. PMC   5010652 . PMID   27586827.
  60. Amico D (2016). Health & physical assessment in nursing. Boston: Pearson. p. 173. ISBN   978-0133876406.
  61. Taylor C (2015). Fundamentals of nursing : the art and science of person-centered nursing care. Philadelphia: Wolters Kluwer Health. p. 241. ISBN   978-1451185614.
  62. Venes D (2013). Taber's cyclopedic medical dictionary. Philadelphia: F.A. Davis. p. 1716. ISBN   978-0803629776.
  63. Prkachin KM, Solomon PE, Ross J (June 2007). "Underestimation of pain by health-care providers: towards a model of the process of inferring pain in others". The Canadian Journal of Nursing Research. 39 (2): 88–106. PMID   17679587.
  64. McCaffery M. (1968). Nursing practice theories related to cognition, bodily pain, and man-environment interactions. Los Angeles: UCLA Students Store.
    More recently, McCaffery defined pain as "whatever the experiencing person says it is, existing whenever the experiencing person says it does." Pasero C, McCaffery M (1999). Pain: clinical manual. St. Louis: Mosby. ISBN   978-0815156093.
  65. Kelly AM (May 2001). "The minimum clinically significant difference in visual analogue scale pain score does not differ with severity of pain". Emergency Medicine Journal. 18 (3): 205–207. doi:10.1136/emj.18.3.205. PMC   1725574 . PMID   11354213.
  66. Hawker GA, Mian S, Kendzerska T, French M (November 2011). "Measures of adult pain: Visual Analog Scale for Pain (VAS Pain), Numeric Rating Scale for Pain (NRS Pain), McGill Pain Questionnaire (MPQ), Short-Form McGill Pain Questionnaire (SF-MPQ), Chronic Pain Grade Scale (CPGS), Short Form-36 Bodily Pain Scale (SF-36 BPS), and Measure of Intermittent and Constant Osteoarthritis Pain (ICOAP)". Arthritis Care & Research. 63 (S11): S240–252. doi:10.1002/acr.20543. PMID   22588748.
  67. 1 2 3 Lewis SM, Bucher L, Heitkemper MM, Harding M (2017). Medical-surgical nursing: Assessment and management of clinical problems (10th ed.). St. Louis, Missouri: Elsevier. p. 126. ISBN   978-0323328524. OCLC   944472408.
  68. 1 2 3 Jarvis C (2007). Physical examination & health assessment. St. Louis, Mo: Elsevier Saunders. pp. 180–192. ISBN   978-1455728107.
  69. Encandela JA (March 1993). "Social science and the study of pain since Zborowski: a need for a new agenda". Social Science & Medicine. 36 (6): 783–791. doi:10.1016/0277-9536(93)90039-7. PMID   8480223.
  70. Zborowski M. People in Pain. 1969, San Francisco, CA:Josey-Bass[ ISBN missing ][ page needed ]
  71. Encandela JA (1997). "Social Construction of pain and aging: Individual artfulness within interpretive structures". Symbolic Interaction. 20 (3): 251–273. doi:10.1525/si.1997.20.3.251.
  72. Lawhorne L, Passerini J (1999). Chronic Pain Management in the Long Term Care Setting: Clinical Practice Guidelines. Baltimore, Maryland: American Medical Directors Association. pp. 1–27.
  73. Epstein R (19 March 2018). "When Doctors Don't Listen to Women". The New York Times. Archived from the original on 9 May 2019. Retrieved 20 July 2019.
  74. Fasslet J (15 October 2015). "How Doctors Take Women's Pain Less Seriously". The Atlantic. Archived from the original on 17 July 2019. Retrieved 20 July 2019.
  75. "Stories of Misunderstanding Women's Pain". The Atlantic. 15 March 2016. Archived from the original on 15 April 2019. Retrieved 20 July 2019.
  76. Panju AA, Hemmelgarn BR, Guyatt GH, Simel DL (October 1998). "The rational clinical examination. Is this patient having a myocardial infarction?". JAMA. 280 (14): 1256–1263. doi:10.1001/jama.280.14.1256. PMID   9786377.
  77. Slater EE, DeSanctis RW (May 1976). "The clinical recognition of dissecting aortic aneurysm". The American Journal of Medicine. 60 (5): 625–633. doi:10.1016/0002-9343(76)90496-4. PMID   1020750.
  78. Brown JE, Chatterjee N, Younger J, Mackey S (September 2011). "Towards a physiology-based measure of pain: patterns of human brain activity distinguish painful from non-painful thermal stimulation". PLOS ONE. 6 (9): e24124. Bibcode:2011PLoSO...624124B. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0024124 . PMC   3172232 . PMID   21931652.
  79. Paddock C (15 September 2011). "Tool That Measures Pain Objectively Under Way". Medical News Today. Archived from the original on 25 September 2017. Retrieved 25 September 2017.
  80. "Feeling pain? The computer can tell". Reuters. 13 September 2011. Archived from the original on 17 June 2015. Retrieved 25 September 2017. Editorial
  81. Urch CE, Suzuki R (26 September 2008). "Pathophysiology of somatic, visceral, and neuropathic cancer pain". In Sykes N, Bennett MI & Yuan C-S (ed.). Clinical pain management: Cancer pain (2d ed.). London: Hodder Arnold. pp. 3–12. ISBN   978-0340940075.
  82. Treede RD, Jensen TS, Campbell JN, Cruccu G, Dostrovsky JO, Griffin JW, Hansson P, Hughes R, Nurmikko T, Serra J (April 2008). "Neuropathic pain: redefinition and a grading system for clinical and research purposes". Neurology. 70 (18): 1630–1635. doi:10.1212/01.wnl.0000282763.29778.59. hdl:11573/97043. PMID   18003941. S2CID   30172528.
  83. Paice JA (2003). "Mechanisms and management of neuropathic pain in cancer" (PDF). The Journal of Supportive Oncology. 1 (2): 107–120. PMID   15352654. Archived from the original (PDF) on 7 January 2010. Retrieved 8 January 2010.
  84. Campbell JN, Meyer RA (October 2006). "Mechanisms of neuropathic pain". Neuron. 52 (1): 77–92. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2006.09.021. PMC   1810425 . PMID   17015228.
  85. 1 2 "Psychogenic Pain". Cleveland Clinic. Archived from the original on 14 July 2011. Retrieved 25 September 2017.
  86. Thienhaus O, Cole BE (2002). "The classification of pain". In Weiner RS (ed.). Pain management: A practical guide for clinicians. American Academy of Pain Management. p. 29. ISBN   978-0849322624.
  87. Brown AK, Christo PJ, Wu CL (December 2004). "Strategies for postoperative pain management". Best Practice & Research. Clinical Anaesthesiology. 18 (4): 703–717. doi:10.1016/j.bpa.2004.05.004. PMID   15460554.
  88. Cullen L, Greiner J, Titler MG (June 2001). "Pain management in the culture of critical care". Critical Care Nursing Clinics of North America. 13 (2): 151–166. doi:10.1016/S0899-5885(18)30046-7. PMID   11866399.
  89. Rupp T, Delaney KA (April 2004). "Inadequate analgesia in emergency medicine". Annals of Emergency Medicine. 43 (4): 494–503. doi:10.1016/j.annemergmed.2003.11.019. PMID   15039693.
  90. Smith GF, Toonen TR (April 2007). "Primary care of the patient with cancer". American Family Physician. 75 (8): 1207–1214. PMID   17477104.
  91. Jacobson PL, Mann JD (January 2003). "Evolving role of the neurologist in the diagnosis and treatment of chronic noncancer pain". Mayo Clinic Proceedings. 78 (1): 80–84. doi:10.4065/78.1.80. PMID   12528880.
  92. Deandrea S, Montanari M, Moja L, Apolone G (December 2008). "Prevalence of undertreatment in cancer pain. A review of published literature". Annals of Oncology. 19 (12): 1985–1991. doi:10.1093/annonc/mdn419. PMC   2733110 . PMID   18632721.
  93. Perron V, Schonwetter RS (2001). "Assessment and management of pain in palliative care patients". Cancer Control. 8 (1): 15–24. doi: 10.1177/107327480100800103 . PMID   11176032.
  94. Selbst SM, Fein JA (2006). "Sedation and analgesia". In Henretig FM, Fleisher GR, Ludwig S (eds.). Textbook of pediatric emergency medicine. Hagerstwon, MD: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. ISBN   978-1605471594. Archived from the original on 11 June 2016. Retrieved 3 February 2016.
  95. Cleeland CS (June 1998). "Undertreatment of cancer pain in elderly patients". JAMA. 279 (23): 1914–1915. doi:10.1001/jama.279.23.1914. PMID   9634265.
  96. Bonham VL (2001). "Race, ethnicity, and pain treatment: striving to understand the causes and solutions to the disparities in pain treatment" (PDF). The Journal of Law, Medicine & Ethics. 29 (1): 52–68. doi:10.1111/j.1748-720X.2001.tb00039.x. PMID   11521272. S2CID   18257031. Archived from the original (PDF) on 19 July 2011. Retrieved 7 March 2022.
  97. Green CR, Anderson KO, Baker TA, Campbell LC, Decker S, Fillingim RB, Kalauokalani DA, Kaloukalani DA, Lasch KE, Myers C, Tait RC, Todd KH, Vallerand AH (September 2003). "The unequal burden of pain: confronting racial and ethnic disparities in pain" (PDF). Pain Medicine. 4 (3): 277–294. doi: 10.1046/j.1526-4637.2003.03034.x . hdl:2027.42/73822. PMID   12974827. Archived from the original on 30 March 2021. Retrieved 2 September 2019.
  98. Hoffmann DE, Tarzian AJ (2001). "The girl who cried pain: a bias against women in the treatment of pain". The Journal of Law, Medicine & Ethics. 29 (1): 13–27. doi:10.1111/j.1748-720X.2001.tb00037.x. PMID   11521267. S2CID   219952180. Archived from the original on 1 November 2019. Retrieved 11 July 2019.
  99. Delegates to the International Pain Summit of the International Association for the Study of Pain (2010). "Declaration of Montreal". Archived from the original on 13 May 2011. Retrieved 7 March 2022.
  100. Horlocker TT, Cousins MJ, Bridenbaugh PO, Carr DL (2008). Cousins and Bridenbaugh's Neural Blockade in Clinical Anesthesia and Pain Medicine. Hagerstwon, MD: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. ISBN   978-0781773881.
  101. "Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation". Archived from the original on 16 May 2008. Retrieved 7 March 2022.
  102. Human Rights Watch (2011). "Tens of Millions Face Death in Agony". Archived from the original on 1 September 2013. Retrieved 26 August 2013.
  103. Mallinson TE (2017). "A review of ketorolac as a prehospital analgesic". Journal of Paramedic Practice. 9 (12): 522–526. doi: 10.12968/jpar.2017.9.12.522 . Archived from the original on 5 June 2018. Retrieved 2 June 2018.
  104. Derry CJ, Derry S, Moore RA (December 2014). "Caffeine as an analgesic adjuvant for acute pain in adults". The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. 12 (12): CD009281. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD009281.pub3. PMC   6485702 . PMID   25502052.
  105. Derry S, Wiffen PJ, Moore RA (July 2015). "Single dose oral ibuprofen plus caffeine for acute postoperative pain in adults". The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. 2019 (7): CD011509. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD011509.pub2. PMC   6481458 . PMID   26171993.
  106. Karlow N, Schlaepfer CH, Stoll CR, Doering M, Carpenter CR, Colditz GA, et al. (October 2018). "A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis of Ketamine as an Alternative to Opioids for Acute Pain in the Emergency Department". Academic Emergency Medicine. 25 (10): 1086–1097. doi: 10.1111/acem.13502 . PMID   30019434.
  107. Higgins C, Smith BH, Matthews K (June 2019). "Evidence of opioid-induced hyperalgesia in clinical populations after chronic opioid exposure: a systematic review and meta-analysis". British Journal of Anaesthesia. 122 (6): e114–e126. doi: 10.1016/j.bja.2018.09.019 . PMID   30915985.
  108. Fishbain DA, Pulikal A (November 2019). "Does Opioid Tapering in Chronic Pain Patients Result in Improved Pain or Same Pain vs Increased Pain at Taper Completion? A Structured Evidence-Based Systematic Review". Pain Medicine. 20 (11): 2179–2197. doi:10.1093/pm/pny231. PMID   30597076. Archived from the original on 18 January 2021. Retrieved 19 February 2021.
  109. Stevens B, Yamada J, Ohlsson A, Haliburton S, Shorkey A (July 2016). "Sucrose for analgesia in newborn infants undergoing painful procedures". The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. 7 (2): CD001069. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD001069.pub5. PMC   6457867 . PMID   27420164.
  110. Lasky RE, van Drongelen W (October 2010). "Is sucrose an effective analgesic for newborn babies?". Lancet. 376 (9748): 1201–1203. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(10)61358-X. PMID   20817245. S2CID   18724497.
  111. Harrison D, Stevens B, Bueno M, Yamada J, Adams-Webber T, Beyene J, Ohlsson A (June 2010). "Efficacy of sweet solutions for analgesia in infants between 1 and 12 months of age: a systematic review". Archives of Disease in Childhood. 95 (6): 406–413. doi: 10.1136/adc.2009.174227 . PMID   20463370.
  112. Wark DM (July 2008). "What we can do with hypnosis: a brief note". The American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis. 51 (1): 29–36. doi:10.1080/00029157.2008.10401640. PMID   18714889. S2CID   12240662.
  113. Elkins G, Jensen MP, Patterson DR (July 2007). "Hypnotherapy for the management of chronic pain". The International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis. 55 (3): 275–287. doi:10.1080/00207140701338621. PMC   2752362 . PMID   17558718.
  114. Madsen MV, Gøtzsche PC, Hróbjartsson A (January 2009). "Acupuncture treatment for pain: systematic review of randomised clinical trials with acupuncture, placebo acupuncture, and no acupuncture groups". BMJ. 338: a3115. doi:10.1136/bmj.a3115. PMC   2769056 . PMID   19174438.
  115. Chiu HY, Hsieh YJ, Tsai PS (March 2017). "Systematic review and meta-analysis of acupuncture to reduce cancer-related pain". European Journal of Cancer Care. 26 (2): e12457. doi: 10.1111/ecc.12457 . PMID   26853524. S2CID   20096639.
  116. Chang SC, Hsu CH, Hsu CK, Yang SS, Chang SJ (February 2017). "The efficacy of acupuncture in managing patients with chronic prostatitis/chronic pelvic pain syndrome: A systemic review and meta-analysis". Neurourology and Urodynamics. 36 (2): 474–481. doi:10.1002/nau.22958. PMID   26741647. S2CID   46827576.
  117. Ji M, Wang X, Chen M, Shen Y, Zhang X, Yang J (2015). "The Efficacy of Acupuncture for the Treatment of Sciatica: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis". Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine. 2015: 192808. doi: 10.1155/2015/192808 . PMC   4575738 . PMID   26425130.
  118. Gagnier JJ, Oltean H, van Tulder MW, Berman BM, Bombardier C, Robbins CB (January 2016). "Herbal Medicine for Low Back Pain: A Cochrane Review". Spine. 41 (2): 116–133. doi:10.1097/BRS.0000000000001310. PMID   26630428.
  119. 1 2 Rubinstein SM, Terwee CB, Assendelft WJ, de Boer MR, van Tulder MW (September 2012). "Spinal manipulative therapy for acute low-back pain". The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. 2012 (9): CD008880. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD008880.pub2. PMC   6885055 . PMID   22972127.
  120. Straube S, Andrew Moore R, Derry S, McQuay HJ (January 2009). "Vitamin D and chronic pain". Pain. 141 (1–2): 10–13. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2008.11.010. PMID   19084336. S2CID   17244398.
  121. "IASP Position Statement on the Use of Cannabinoids to Treat Pain". iasp-pain. Retrieved 10 May 2024.
  122. Cordell WH, Keene KK, Giles BK, Jones JB, Jones JH, Brizendine EJ (May 2002). "The high prevalence of pain in emergency medical care". The American Journal of Emergency Medicine. 20 (3): 165–169. doi:10.1053/ajem.2002.32643. PMID   11992334.
  123. Hasselström J, Liu-Palmgren J, Rasjö-Wrååk G (2002). "Prevalence of pain in general practice". European Journal of Pain. 6 (5): 375–385. doi:10.1016/S1090-3801(02)00025-3. PMID   12160512. S2CID   798849.
  124. Abu-Saad Huijer H (2010). "Chronic pain: a review". Le Journal Medical Libanais. The Lebanese Medical Journal. 58 (1): 21–27. PMID   20358856.
  125. Smith AK, Cenzer IS, Knight SJ, Puntillo KA, Widera E, Williams BA, Boscardin WJ, Covinsky KE (November 2010). "The epidemiology of pain during the last 2 years of life". Annals of Internal Medicine. 153 (9): 563–569. doi:10.7326/0003-4819-153-9-201011020-00005. PMC   3150170 . PMID   21041575.
  126. Perquin CW, Hazebroek-Kampschreur AA, Hunfeld JA, Bohnen AM, van Suijlekom-Smit LW, Passchier J, van der Wouden JC (July 2000). "Pain in children and adolescents: a common experience". Pain. 87 (1): 51–58. doi:10.1016/S0304-3959(00)00269-4. PMID   10863045. S2CID   9813003.
  127. Foucault M (2007). Security, Territory, Population: Lectures at the College de France, 1977–78. Palgrave Macmillan. p. 1.
  128. 1 2 3 Einolf C (2007). "The Fall and Rise of Torture: A Comparative and Historical Analysis". Social Theory. 25 (2): 101–121. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9558.2007.00300.x. JSTOR   20453071. S2CID   53345959.
  129. Morinis A (1985). "The ritual experience: pain and the transformation of consciousness in ordeals of initiation". Ethos. 13 (2): 150–174. doi: 10.1525/eth.1985.13.2.02a00040 . JSTOR   639985.
  130. Atkinson M, Young K (2001). "Flesh journeys: neo primitives and the contemporary rediscovery of radical body modification". Deviant Behavior. 22 (2): 117–146. doi:10.1080/016396201750065018. S2CID   146525156.
  131. Loland S, Skirstad B, Waddington I, eds. (2006). Pain and injury in sport: Social and ethical analysis. London and New York: Routledge. pp. 17–20. ISBN   978-0415357043.
  132. Narayan MC (April 2010). "Culture's effects on pain assessment and management". The American Journal of Nursing. 110 (4): 38–47. doi: 10.1097/01.NAJ.0000370157.33223.6d . PMID   20335689.
  133. Working party of the Nuffield Council on Bioethics (2005). "The ethics of research involving animals. London: Nuffield Council on Bioethics." ISBN   978-1904384106. Archived from the original on 25 June 2008. Retrieved 12 January 2010.
  134. Rollin BE (June 2007). "Animal research: a moral science. Talking Point on the use of animals in scientific research". EMBO Reports. 8 (6): 521–525. doi:10.1038/sj.embor.7400996. PMC   2002540 . PMID   17545990.
  135. Rollin B (1989). The Unheeded Cry: Animal Consciousness, Animal Pain, and Science. New York: Oxford University Press. pp. 117–118. cited in Carbone L (2004). What animals want: expertise and advocacy in laboratory animal welfare policy. US: Oxford University Press. p. 150.
  136. Griffin DR, Speck GB (January 2004). "New evidence of animal consciousness". Animal Cognition. 7 (1): 5–18. doi:10.1007/s10071-003-0203-x. PMID   14658059. S2CID   8650837.
  137. 1 2 Sherwin CM (February 2001). "Can invertebrates suffer? Or, how robust is argument-by-analogy?". Animal Welfare. 10 (1): 103–118. doi:10.1017/S0962728600023551. S2CID   54126137. Archived from the original on 7 March 2022. Retrieved 22 December 2021.
  138. Lockwood JA (1987). "The Moral Standing of Insects and the Ethics of Extinction". The Florida Entomologist. 70 (1): 70–89. doi:10.2307/3495093. JSTOR   3495093.
  139. DeGrazia D, Rowan A (September 1991). "Pain, suffering, and anxiety in animals and humans". Theoretical Medicine. 12 (3): 193–211. doi:10.1007/BF00489606. PMID   1754965. S2CID   34920699.
  140. "Do Invertebrates Feel Pain?". The Senate Standing Committee on Legal and Constitutional Affairs. The Parliament of Canada. Archived from the original on 6 January 2010. Retrieved 11 June 2008.
  141. Smith JA (1991). "A Question of Pain in Invertebrates". Institute for Laboratory Animal Research Journal. 33: 1–2. Archived from the original on 8 October 2011.
  142. Abbott FV, Franklin KB, Westbrook FR (January 1995). "The formalin test: scoring properties of the first and second phases of the pain response in rats". Pain. 60 (1): 91–102. doi:10.1016/0304-3959(94)00095-V. PMID   7715946. S2CID   35448280.
  143. Jones JM, Foster W, Twomey CR, Burdge J, Ahmed OM, Pereira TD, et al. (August 2020). "A machine-vision approach for automated pain measurement at millisecond timescales". eLife. 9: e57258. doi: 10.7554/eLife.57258 . PMC   7434442 . PMID   32758355.
  144. 1 2 3 Petruzzello, Melissa (2016). "Do Plants Feel Pain?". Encyclopedia Britannica. Retrieved 8 January 2023. Given that plants do not have pain receptors, nerves, or a brain, they do not feel pain as we members of the animal kingdom understand it. Uprooting a carrot or trimming a hedge is not a form of botanical torture, and you can bite into that apple without worry.
  145. Draguhn, Andreas; Mallatt, Jon M.; Robinson, David G. (2021). "Anesthetics and plants: no pain, no brain, and therefore no consciousness". Protoplasma. 258 (2). Springer: 239–248. doi:10.1007/s00709-020-01550-9. PMC   7907021 . PMID   32880005. 32880005.