Emotion classification

Last updated
Colored intaglio prints by Charles Le Brun and J. Pass depicting the facial expressions of sixteen emotions Sixteen faces expressing the human passions. Wellcome L0068375 (cropped).jpg
Colored intaglio prints by Charles Le Brun and J. Pass depicting the facial expressions of sixteen emotions

Emotion classification, the means by which one may distinguish or contrast one emotion from another, is a contested issue in emotion research and in affective science. Researchers have approached the classification of emotions from one of two fundamental viewpoints:[ citation needed ]

Contents

  1. that emotions are discrete and fundamentally different constructs
  2. that emotions can be characterized on a dimensional basis in groupings

Emotions as discrete categories

In discrete emotion theory, all humans are thought to have an innate set of basic emotions that are cross-culturally recognizable. These basic emotions are described as "discrete" because they are believed to be distinguishable by an individual's facial expression and biological processes. [1] Theorists have conducted studies to determine which emotions are basic. A popular example is Paul Ekman and his colleagues' cross-cultural study of 1992, in which they concluded that the six basic emotions are anger, disgust, fear, happiness, sadness, and surprise. [2] Ekman explains that there are particular characteristics attached to each of these emotions, allowing them to be expressed in varying degrees in a non-verbal manner. [3] [4] Each emotion acts as a discrete category rather than an individual emotional state. [5]

Simplicity debate

Humans' subjective experience is that emotions are clearly recognizable in ourselves and others. This apparent ease of recognition has led to the identification of a number of emotions that are said to be basic, and universal among all people. However, a debate among experts has questioned this understanding of what emotions are. There has been recent discussion of the progression on the different views of emotion over the years. [6]

On "basic emotion" accounts, activation of an emotion, such as anger, sadness, or fear, is "triggered" by the brain's appraisal of a stimulus or event with respect to the perceiver's goals or survival. In particular, the function, expression, and meaning of different emotions are hypothesized to be biologically distinct from one another. A theme common to many basic emotions theories is that there should be functional signatures that distinguish different emotions: we should be able to tell what emotion a person is feeling by looking at his or her brain activity and/or physiology. Furthermore, knowledge of what the person is seeing or the larger context of the eliciting event should not be necessary to deduce what the person is feeling from observing the biological signatures. [5]

On "constructionist" accounts, the emotion a person feels in response to a stimulus or event is "constructed" from more elemental biological and psychological ingredients. Two hypothesized ingredients are "core affect" (characterized by, e.g., hedonicvalence and physiological arousal) and conceptual knowledge (such as the semantic meaning of the emotion labels themselves, e.g., the word "anger"). A theme common to many constructionist theories is that different emotions do not have specific locations in the nervous system or distinct physiological signatures, and that context is central to the emotion a person feels because of the accessibility of different concepts afforded by different contexts. [7]

Dimensional models of emotion

For both theoretical and practical reasons, researchers define emotions according to one or more dimensions. In his philosophical treatise, The Passions of the Soul, Descartes defines and investigates the six primary passions (Wonder, love, hate, desire, joy, and sadness). In 1897, Wilhelm Max Wundt, the father of modern psychology, proposed that emotions can be described by three dimensions: "pleasurable versus unpleasurable", "arousing or subduing", and "strain or relaxation". [8] In 1954, Harold Schlosberg named three dimensions of emotion: "pleasantness–unpleasantness", "attention–rejection" and "level of activation". [9]

Dimensional models of emotion attempt to conceptualize human emotions by defining where they lie in two or three dimensions. Most dimensional models incorporate valence and arousal or intensity dimensions. Dimensional models of emotion suggest that a common and interconnected neurophysiological system is responsible for all affective states. [10] These models contrast theories of basic emotion, which propose that different emotions arise from separate neural systems. [10] Several dimensional models of emotion have been developed, though there are just a few that remain as the dominant models currently accepted by most. [11] The two-dimensional models that are most prominent are the circumplex model, the vector model, and the Positive Activation – Negative Activation (PANA) model. [11]

Circumplex model

Circumplex model of emotion has two axes: Valence and Arousal Circumplex model of emotion.svg
Circumplex model of emotion has two axes: Valence and Arousal

The circumplex model of emotion was developed by James Russell. [12] This model suggests that emotions are distributed in a two-dimensional circular space, containing arousal and valence dimensions. Arousal represents the vertical axis and valence represents the horizontal axis, while the center of the circle represents a neutral valence and a medium level of arousal. [11] In this model, emotional states can be represented at any level of valence and arousal, or at a neutral level of one or both of these factors. Circumplex models have been used most commonly to test stimuli of emotion words, emotional facial expressions, and affective states. [13]

Russell and Lisa Feldman Barrett describe their modified circumplex model as representative of core affect, or the most elementary feelings that are not necessarily directed toward anything. Different prototypical emotional episodes, or clear emotions that are evoked or directed by specific objects, can be plotted on the circumplex, according to their levels of arousal and pleasure. [14]

Vector model

The vector model of emotion suggests that emotions are structured in terms of arousal and valence. A positive valence represents appetitive motivation and negative valence represents defensive motivation. Vector model of emotion.svg
The vector model of emotion suggests that emotions are structured in terms of arousal and valence. A positive valence represents appetitive motivation and negative valence represents defensive motivation.

The vector model of emotion appeared in 1992. [16] This two-dimensional model consists of vectors that point in two directions, representing a "boomerang" shape. The model assumes that there is always an underlying arousal dimension, and that valence determines the direction in which a particular emotion lies. For example, a positive valence would shift the emotion up the top vector and a negative valence would shift the emotion down the bottom vector. [11] In this model, high arousal states are differentiated by their valence, whereas low arousal states are more neutral and are represented near the meeting point of the vectors. Vector models have been most widely used in the testing of word and picture stimuli. [13]

Positive activation – negative activation (P.A.N.A.) model

The positive activation – negative activation (P.A.N.A.) or "consensual" model of emotion, originally created by Watson and Tellegen in 1985, [17] suggests that positive affect and negative affect are two separate systems. Similar to the vector model, states of higher arousal tend to be defined by their valence, and states of lower arousal tend to be more neutral in terms of valence. [11] In the P.A.N.A. model, the vertical axis represents low to high positive affect and the horizontal axis represents low to high negative affect. The dimensions of valence and arousal lay at a 45-degree rotation over these axes. [17]

Plutchik's model

Robert Plutchik offers a three-dimensional model that is a hybrid of both basic-complex categories and dimensional theories. It arranges emotions in concentric circles where inner circles are more basic and outer circles more complex. Notably, outer circles are also formed by blending the inner circle emotions. Plutchik's model, as Russell's, emanates from a circumplex representation, where emotional words were plotted based on similarity. [18] There are numerous emotions, which appear in several intensities and can be combined in various ways to form emotional "dyads". [19] [20] [21] [22] [23]

PAD emotional state model

The PAD emotional state model is a psychological model developed by Albert Mehrabian and James A. Russell to describe and measure emotional states. PAD uses three numerical dimensions to represent all emotions. [24] [25] The PAD dimensions are Pleasure, Arousal and Dominance.

Criticisms

Cultural considerations

Ethnographic and cross-cultural studies of emotions have shown the variety of ways in which emotions differ with cultures. Because of these differences, many cross-cultural psychologists and anthropologists challenge the idea of universal classifications of emotions altogether. Cultural differences have been observed in the way in which emotions are valued, expressed, and regulated. The social norms for emotions, such as the frequency with or circumstances in which they are expressed, also vary drastically. [26] [27] For example, the demonstration of anger is encouraged by Kaluli people, but condemned by Utku Inuit. [28]

The largest piece of evidence that disputes the universality of emotions is language. Differences within languages directly correlate to differences in emotion taxonomy. Languages differ in that they categorize emotions based on different components. Some may categorize by event types, whereas others categorize by action readiness. Furthermore, emotion taxonomies vary due to the differing implications emotions have in different languages. [26] That being said, not all English words have equivalents in all other languages and vice versa, indicating that there are words for emotions present in some languages but not in others. [29] Emotions such as the schadenfreude in German and saudade in Portuguese are commonly expressed in emotions in their respective languages, but lack an English equivalent.

Some languages do not differentiate between emotions that are considered to be the basic emotions in English. For instance, certain African languages have one word for both anger and sadness, and others for shame and fear. There is ethnographic evidence that even challenges the universality of the category "emotions" because certain cultures lack a specific word relating to the English word "emotions". [27]

Lists of emotions

Emotions are categorized into various affects, which correspond to the current situation. [30] An affect is the range of feeling experienced. [31] Both positive and negative emotions are needed in our daily lives. [32] Many theories of emotion have been proposed, [33] with contrasting views. [34]

Basic emotions

Contrasting basic emotions

A 2009 review [44] of theories of emotion identifies and contrasts fundamental emotions according to three key criteria for mental experiences that:

  1. have a strongly motivating subjective quality like pleasure or pain;
  2. are a response to some event or object that is either real or imagined;
  3. motivate particular kinds of behavior.

The combination of these attributes distinguishes emotions from sensations, feelings and moods.

Kind of emotionPositive emotionsNegative emotions
Related to object propertiesInterest, curiosity, enthusiasm Indifference, habituation, boredom
Attraction , desire, admirationAversion, disgust, revulsion
Surprise, amusementAlarm, panic
Future appraisalHope, excitementFear, anxiety, dread
Event-relatedGratitude, thankfulnessAnger, rage
Joy, elation, triumph, jubilation Sorrow , grief
Patience Frustration , restlessness
ContentmentDiscontentment, disappointment
Self-appraisalHumility, modestyPride, arrogance
SocialCharityAvarice, greed, miserliness, envy, jealousy
Sympathy Cruelty
Cathected LoveHate

Emotion dynamics

Researchers distinguish several emotion dynamics, most commonly how intense (mean level), variable (fluctuations), inert (temporal dependency), instable (magnitude of moment-to-moment fluctuations), or differentiated someone's emotions are (the specificity of granularity of emotions), and whether and how an emotion augments or blunts other emotions. [45] Meta-analytic reviews show systematic developmental changes in emotion dynamics throughout childhood and adolescence and substantial between-person differences. [45]

HUMAINE's proposal for EARL

The emotion annotation and representation language (EARL) proposed by the Human-Machine Interaction Network on Emotion (HUMAINE) classifies 48 emotions. [46]

Parrott's emotions by groups

A tree-structured list of emotions was described in Shaver et al. (1987), [47] and also featured in Parrott (2001). [48]

Primary emotionSecondary emotionTertiary emotion
LoveAffectionAdoration · Fondness  · Liking  ·Attraction · Caring  · Tenderness  ·Compassion · Sentimentality
Lust/Sexual desire Desire · Passion  · Infatuation
Longing Longing
Joy Cheerfulness Amusement · Bliss  · Gaiety  · Glee  · Jolliness  · Joviality  ·Joy ·Delight · Enjoyment  · Gladness  ·Happiness · Jubilation  ·Elation · Satisfaction  · Ecstasy  · Euphoria
Zest Enthusiasm · Zeal  ·Excitement · Thrill  · Exhilaration
ContentmentPleasure
PrideTriumph
Optimism Eagerness  ·Hope
Enthrallment Enthrallment · Rapture
ReliefRelief
SurpriseSurprise Amazement  · Astonishment
AngerIrritability Aggravation  · Agitation  ·Annoyance · Grouchy  · Grumpy  · Crosspatch
Exasperation Frustration
RageAnger ·Outrage · Fury  · Wrath  · Hostility  · Ferocity  · Bitterness  · Hatred  · Scorn  · Spite  · Vengefulness  · Dislike  · Resentment
Disgust Revulsion  ·Contempt ·Loathing
EnvyJealousy
Torment Torment
Sadness Suffering Agony  · Anguish  ·Hurt
SadnessDepression ·Despair · Gloom  · Glumness  · Unhappiness  ·Grief ·Sorrow · Woe  · Misery  · Melancholy
Disappointment Dismay  · Displeasure
ShameGuilt · Regret  · Remorse
Neglect Alienation  · Defeatism  · Dejection  ·Embarrassment · Homesickness  · Humiliation  · Insecurity  · Insult  · Isolation  · Loneliness  · Rejection
Sympathy Pity  · Mono no aware  ·Sympathy
FearHorror Alarm  ·Shock ·Fear ·Fright ·Horror ·Terror ·Panic · Hysteria  · Mortification
Nervousness Anxiety  · Suspense  · Uneasiness  ·Apprehension (fear) ·Worry · Distress  · Dread

Plutchik's wheel of emotions

Plutchik-wheel.svg
Plutchik's original emotion wheel
Plutchik Dyads.png
A diagram depicting the primary, secondary, and tertiary dyads

In 1980, Robert Plutchik diagrammed a wheel of eight emotions: joy, trust, fear, surprise, sadness, disgust, anger and anticipation, inspired by his Ten Postulates. [49] [50] Plutchik also theorized twenty-four "Primary", "Secondary", and "Tertiary" dyads (feelings composed of two emotions). [51] [52] [53] [54] [55] [56] [57] The wheel emotions can be paired in four groups:

Primary dyad = one petal apart = Love = Joy + Trust
Secondary dyad = two petals apart = Envy = Sadness + Anger
Tertiary dyad = three petals apart = Shame = Fear + Disgust
Opposite emotions = four petals apart = AnticipationSurprise

There are also triads, emotions formed from 3 primary emotions, though Plutchik never describes in any detail what the triads might be. [58] This leads to a combination of 24 dyads and 32 triads, making 56 emotions at 1 intensity level. [59] Emotions can be mild or intense; [60] for example, distraction is a mild form of surprise, and rage is an intense form of anger. The kinds of relation between each pair of emotions are:

Emotions and opposites
Mild emotionMild oppositeBasic emotionBasic oppositeIntense emotionIntense opposite
Serenity Pensiveness , Gloominess Joy, CheerfulnessSadness, DejectionEcstasy, ElationGrief, Sorrow
Acceptance, ToleranceBoredom, DislikeTrustDisgust, AversionAdmiration, Adoration Loathing , Revulsion
Apprehension, DismayAnnoyance, IrritationFear, FrightAnger, Hostility Terror, PanicRage, Fury
Distraction, Uncertainty Interest, Attentiveness SurpriseAnticipation, Expectancy Amazement, Astonishment Vigilance
Dyads (Combinations)
Human feelingsEmotionsOpposite feelingsEmotions
Optimism, CourageAnticipation + Joy Disapproval , DisappointmentSurprise + Sadness
Hope, Fatalism Anticipation + Trust Unbelief , ShockSurprise + Disgust
Anxiety, DreadAnticipation + Fear Outrage , HateSurprise + Anger
Love, Friendliness Joy + TrustRemorse, MiserySadness + Disgust
Guilt, ExcitementJoy + FearEnvy, Sullenness Sadness + Anger
Delight, Doom Joy + Surprise Pessimism Sadness + Anticipation
Submission, Modesty Trust + FearContempt, ScornDisgust + Anger
CuriosityTrust + Surprise Cynicism Disgust + Anticipation
Sentimentality, Resignation Trust + Sadness Morbidness , Derisiveness Disgust + Joy
Awe, Alarm Fear + Surprise Aggressiveness , VengeanceAnger + Anticipation
DespairFear + SadnessPride, Victory Anger + Joy
Shame, Prudishness Fear + Disgust Dominance Anger + Trust
Opposite combinations [54]
Human feelingsEmotions
Bittersweetness Joy + Sadness
Ambivalence Trust + Disgust
Frozenness Fear + Anger
Confusion Surprise + Anticipation

Similar emotions in the wheel are adjacent to each other. [61] Anger, Anticipation, Joy, and Trust are positive in valence, while Fear, Surprise, Sadness, and Disgust are negative in valence. Anger is classified as a "positive" emotion because it involves "moving toward" a goal, [62] while surprise is negative because it is a violation of someone's territory. [63] The emotion dyads each have half-opposites and exact opposites: [64]

Anticipation, Joy, Surprise, Sadness
+SadnessJoy
AnticipationPessimismOptimism
SurpriseDisapprovalDelight
Joy, Trust, Sadness, Disgust
+DisgustTrust
JoyMorbidnessLove
SadnessRemorseSentimentality
Trust, Fear, Disgust, Anger
+FearAnger
TrustSubmissionDominance
DisgustShameContempt
Fear, Surprise, Anger, Anticipation
+SurpriseAnticipation
AngerOutrageAggressiveness
FearAweAnxiety
Trust, Surprise, Disgust, Anticipation
+SurpriseAnticipation
TrustCuriosityHope
DisgustUnbeliefCynicism
Joy, Fear, Sadness, Anger
+FearAnger
JoyGuiltPride
SadnessDespairEnvy

Six emotion axes

MIT researchers [65] published a paper titled "An Affective Model of Interplay Between Emotions and Learning: Reengineering Educational Pedagogy—Building a Learning Companion" that lists six axes of emotions with different opposite emotions, and different emotions coming from ranges. [65]

Emotional flow
Axis-1.0-0.500+0.5+1.0
Anxiety – Confidence AnxietyWorry Discomfort Comfort HopefulConfident
Boredom – Fascination Ennui Boredom Indifference InterestCuriosity Intrigue
Frustration – EuphoriaFrustration Puzzlement Confusion Insight Enlightenment Epiphany
DispiritedEncouraged DispiritedDisappointed Dissatisfied Satisfied Thrilled Enthusiastic
Terror – Enchantment TerrorDread Apprehension CalmAnticipatoryExcited
Humiliation – PrideHumiliatedEmbarrassed Self-conscious Pleased SatisfiedProud

They also made a model labeling phases of learning emotions. [65]

Negative AffectPositive Affect
Constructive LearningDisappointment, Puzzlement, ConfusionAwe, Satisfaction, Curiosity
Un-learningFrustration, Discard,

Misconceptions

Hopefulness, Fresh research

The Book of Human Emotions

Tiffany Watt Smith listed 154 different worldwide emotions and feelings. [66]

Mapping facial expressions

Scientists map twenty-one different facial emotions [68] [69] expanded from Paul Ekman's six basic emotions of anger, disgust, fear, happiness, sadness, and surprise:

FearfulAngrySurprisedDisgusted
HappyHappily SurprisedHappily Disgusted
SadSadly FearfulSadly AngrySadly SurprisedSadly Disgusted
AppalledFearfully AngryFearfully SurprisedFearfully Disgusted
AwedAngrily SurprisedAngrily Disgusted
HatredDisgustedly Surprised

See also

Bibliography

Notes and references

  1. Colombetti, Giovanna (August 2009). "From affect programs to dynamical discrete emotions". Philosophical Psychology. 22 (4): 407–425. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.728.9666 . doi:10.1080/09515080903153600. S2CID   40157414.
  2. Ekman, Paul (January 1992). "Facial Expressions of Emotion: New Findings, New Questions". Psychological Science. 3 (1): 34–38. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1992.tb00253.x. S2CID   9274447.
  3. Bąk, Halszka (2023-12-01). "Issues in the translation equivalence of basic emotion terms". Ampersand. 11: 100128. doi: 10.1016/j.amper.2023.100128 . ISSN   2215-0390.
  4. Elfenbein, Hillary Anger; Ambady, Nalini (2003-10-01). "Universals and Cultural Differences in Recognizing Emotions". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 12 (5): 159–164. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.01252. ISSN   0963-7214. S2CID   262438746.
  5. 1 2 Ekman, Paul (1992). "An Argument for Basic Emotions". Cognition and Emotion. 6 (3/4): 169–200. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.454.1984 . doi:10.1080/02699939208411068.
  6. Gendron, Maria; Barrett, Lisa Feldman (October 2009). "Reconstructing the Past: A Century of Ideas About Emotion in Psychology". Emotion Review. 1 (4): 316–339. doi:10.1177/1754073909338877. PMC   2835158 . PMID   20221412.
  7. Barrett, Lisa Feldman (2006). "Solving the Emotion Paradox: Categorization and the Experience of Emotion". Personality and Social Psychology Review. 10 (1): 20–46. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr1001_2. PMID   16430327. S2CID   7750265.
  8. W.M. Wundt, Outlines of Psychology. (1897). In: Classics in the history of psychology. http://psychclassics.asu.edu/index.htm Archived 2001-02-24 at the Wayback Machine , York University 2010, Toronto.
  9. Schlosberg, H. (1954). "Three dimensions of emotion". Psychological Review . 61 (2): 81–8. doi:10.1037/h0054570. PMID   13155714. S2CID   27914497.
  10. 1 2 Posner, Jonathan; Russell, J.A.; Peterson, B. S. (2005). "The circumplex model of affect: An integrative approach to affective neuroscience, cognitive development, and psychopathology". Development and Psychopathology. 17 (3): 715–734. doi:10.1017/s0954579405050340. PMC   2367156 . PMID   16262989.
  11. 1 2 3 4 5 Rubin, D. C.; Talerico, J.M. (2009). "A comparison of dimensional models of emotion". Memory. 17 (8): 802–808. doi:10.1080/09658210903130764. PMC   2784275 . PMID   19691001.
  12. Russell, James (1980). "A circumplex model of affect". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 39 (6): 1161–1178. doi:10.1037/h0077714. hdl: 10983/22919 .
  13. 1 2 Remington, N. A.; Fabrigar, L. R.; Visser, P. S. (2000). "Re-examining the circumplex model of affect". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 79 (2): 286–300. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.79.2.286. PMID   10948981.
  14. Russell, James; Feldman Barrett, Lisa (1999). "Core affect, prototypical emotional episodes, and other things called emotion: dissecting the elephant". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 76 (5): 805–819. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.76.5.805. PMID   10353204.
  15. Wilson, Graham; Dobrev, Dobromir; Brewster, Stephen A. (2016-05-07). "Hot Under the Collar: Mapping Thermal Feedback to Dimensional Models of Emotion" (PDF). Proceedings of the 2016 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems. pp. 4838–4849. doi:10.1145/2858036.2858205. ISBN   9781450333627. S2CID   13051231.
  16. Bradley, M. M.; Greenwald, M. K.; Petry, M.C.; Lang, P. J. (1992). "Remembering pictures: Pleasure and arousal in memory". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 18 (2): 379–390. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.18.2.379. PMID   1532823.
  17. 1 2 Watson, D.; Tellegen, A. (1985). "Toward a consensual structure of mood". Psychological Bulletin. 98 (2): 219–235. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.98.2.219. PMID   3901060.
  18. Plutchik, R. "The Nature of Emotions". American Scientist. Archived from the original on July 16, 2001. Retrieved 14 April 2011.{{cite news}}: CS1 maint: unfit URL (link)
  19. Plutchik, Robert (16 September 1991). The Emotions. University Press of America. p. 110. ISBN   9780819182869 . Retrieved 16 September 2017 via Google Books.
  20. Plutchik, R. "The Nature of Emotions". American Scientist. Archived from the original on July 16, 2001. Retrieved 14 April 2011.{{cite news}}: CS1 maint: unfit URL (link)
  21. "Robert Plutchik's Psychoevolutionary Theory of Basic Emotions" (PDF). Adliterate.com. Retrieved 2017-06-05.
  22. Jonathan Turner (1 June 2000). On the Origins of Human Emotions: A Sociological Inquiry Into the Evolution of Human Affect. Stanford University Press. p. 76. ISBN   978-0-8047-6436-0.
  23. Atifa Athar; M. Saleem Khan; Khalil Ahmed; Aiesha Ahmed; Nida Anwar (June 2011). "A Fuzzy Inference System for Synergy Estimation of Simultaneous Emotion Dynamics in Agents". International Journal of Scientific & Engineering Research. 2 (6).
  24. 1 2 3 4 Mehrabian, Albert (1980). Basic dimensions for a general psychological theory . Oelgeschlager, Gunn & Hain. pp.  39–53. ISBN   978-0-89946-004-8.
  25. Bales, Robert Freed (2001). Social interaction systems: theory and measurement. Transaction Publishers. pp. 139–140. ISBN   978-0-7658-0872-1.
  26. 1 2 Mesquita, Batja; Nico Frijda (September 1992). "Cultural variations in emotions: a review". Psychological Bulletin. 112 (2): 179–204. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.112.2.179. PMID   1454891.
  27. 1 2 Russell, James (1991). "Culture and Categorization of Emotions" (PDF). Psychological Bulletin. 110 (3): 426–450. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.110.3.426. PMID   1758918. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2016-03-04. Retrieved 15 December 2015.
  28. Eid, Michael; Ed Diener (November 2001). "Norms for experiencing emotions in different cultures: Inter- and intranational differences". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 81 (5): 869–885. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.5.869. PMID   11708563.(subscription required)
  29. Wierzbicka, Anna (September 1986). "Human Emotions: Universal or Culture-Specific?". American Anthropologist. 88 (3): 584–594. doi:10.1525/aa.1986.88.3.02a00030. JSTOR   679478.(subscription required)
  30. Lisa Feldman Barrett (2006). "Solving the Emotion Paradox : Categorization and the Experience of Emotion". Personality and Social Psychology Review. 10 (1): 20–46. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr1001_2. PMID   16430327. S2CID   7750265.
  31. "Emotions and Moods" (PDF). Catalogue.pearsoned.co.uk. Archived from the original (PDF) on 8 January 2020. Retrieved 20 October 2017.
  32. Parrott, W. Gerrod (27 January 2014). The Positive Side of Negative Emotions. Guilford Publications. ISBN   9781462513338 . Retrieved 19 December 2018 via Google Books.
  33. "Comparing The 5 Theories of Emotion – Brain Blogger". Mind. os–IX (34): 188–205. 1884. doi:10.1093/mind/os-IX.34.188 . Retrieved 23 November 2017.
  34. Candland, Douglas (23 November 2017). Emotion. iUniverse. ISBN   9780595270262 . Retrieved 23 November 2017 via Google Books.
  35. James, William (1 April 2007). The Principles of Psychology. Cosimo, Inc. ISBN   9781602063136 . Retrieved 20 October 2017 via Google Books.
  36. Handel, Steven (2011-05-24). "Classification of Emotions" . Retrieved 30 April 2012.
  37. "Are There Basic Emotions?" (PDF). Paulekam.com. Retrieved 20 October 2017.
  38. 1 2 Ekman, Paul (1999), "Basic Emotions", in Dalgleish, T; Power, M (eds.), Handbook of Cognition and Emotion (PDF), Sussex, UK: John Wiley & Sons
  39. Lazarus, Richard S.; Lazarus, Bernice N. (23 September 1996). Passion and Reason: Making Sense of Our Emotions. Oxford University Press. ISBN   9780195104615 . Retrieved 23 September 2017 via Google Books.
  40. "Emotional Competency – Recognize these emotions". Emotionalcompetency.com. Retrieved 23 September 2017.
  41. Cowen, Alan S.; Keltner, Dacher (2017). "Self-report captures 27 distinct categories of emotion bridged by continuous gradients". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 114 (38): E7900–E7909. Bibcode:2017PNAS..114E7900C. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1702247114 . PMC   5617253 . PMID   28874542.
  42. "Psychologists Identify Twenty Seven Distinct Categories of Emotion – Psychology". Sci-news.com. Retrieved 23 November 2017.
  43. "The Emotions Evoked by Video" . Retrieved 2017-09-11.
  44. Robinson, D. L. (2009). "Brain function, mental experience and personality" (PDF). The Netherlands Journal of Psychology. pp. 152–167.
  45. 1 2 Reitsema, A.M. (2022). "Emotion dynamics in children and adolescents: A meta-analytic and descriptive review" (PDF). Emotion. 22 (2): 374–396. doi:10.1037/emo0000970. PMID   34843305. S2CID   244748515.
  46. "HUMAINE Emotion Annotation and Representation Language". Emotion-research.net. Archived from the original on April 11, 2008. Retrieved June 30, 2006.
  47. Shaver, P.; Schwartz, J.; Kirson, D.; O'connor, C. (1987). "Emotion knowledge: further exploration of a prototype approach". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 52 (6): 1061–86. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.52.6.1061. PMID   3598857.
  48. Parrott, W. (2001). Emotions in Social Psychology. Key Readings in Social Psychology. Philadelphia: Psychology Press. ISBN   978-0863776830.
  49. "Basic Emotions—Plutchik". Personalityresearch.org. Retrieved 1 September 2017.
  50. Plutchik, R. "The Nature of Emotions". American Scientist. Archived from the original on July 16, 2001. Retrieved 14 April 2011.{{cite news}}: CS1 maint: unfit URL (link)
  51. "Robert Plutchik's Psychoevolutionary Theory of Basic Emotions" (PDF). Adliterate.com. Retrieved 2017-06-05.
  52. Jonathan Turner (1 June 2000). On the Origins of Human Emotions: A Sociological Inquiry Into the Evolution of Human Affect. Stanford University Press. p. 76. ISBN   978-0-8047-6436-0.
  53. Atifa Athar; M. Saleem Khan; Khalil Ahmed; Aiesha Ahmed; Nida Anwar (June 2011). "A Fuzzy Inference System for Synergy Estimation of Simultaneous Emotion Dynamics in Agents". International Journal of Scientific & Engineering Research. 2 (6).
  54. 1 2 TenHouten, Warren D. (1 December 2016). Alienation and Affect. Taylor & Francis. ISBN   9781317678533 . Retrieved 25 June 2019 via Google Books.
  55. Chorianopoulos, Konstantinos; Divitini, Monica; Hauge, Jannicke Baalsrud; Jaccheri, Letizia; Malaka, Rainer (24 September 2015). Entertainment Computing - ICEC 2015: 14th International Conference, ICEC 2015, Trondheim, Norway, September 29 - October 2, 2015, Proceedings. Springer. ISBN   9783319245898 . Retrieved 25 June 2019 via Google Books.
  56. Plutchik, Robert (25 June 1991). The Emotions. University Press of America. ISBN   9780819182869 . Retrieved 25 June 2019 via Google Books.
  57. O'Shaughnessy, John (4 December 2012). Consumer Behaviour: Perspectives, Findings and Explanations. Macmillan International Higher Education. ISBN   9781137003782 . Retrieved 25 June 2019 via Google Books.[ permanent dead link ]
  58. Plutchik, Robert (31 December 1991). The Emotions. University Press of America. ISBN   9780819182869 . Retrieved 31 December 2018 via Google Books.
  59. Izard, Carroll Ellis (31 December 1971). The face of emotion. Appleton-Century-Crofts. ISBN   9780390478313 . Retrieved 31 December 2018 via Google Books.
  60. "The Nature of Emotions" (PDF). Emotionalcompetency.com. Retrieved 2017-09-16.
  61. Plutchik, Robert (16 September 1991). The Emotions. University Press of America. p. 110. ISBN   9780819182869 . Retrieved 16 September 2017 via Google Books.
  62. TenHouten, Warren D. (23 June 2014). Emotion and Reason: Mind, Brain, and the Social Domains of Work and Love. Routledge. ISBN   9781317580614 . Retrieved 10 December 2019 via Google Books.
  63. TenHouten, Warren D. (22 November 2006). A General Theory of Emotions and Social Life. Routledge. ISBN   9781134229079 . Retrieved 10 December 2019 via Google Books.
  64. TenHouten, Warren D. (22 November 2006). A General Theory of Emotions and Social Life. Routledge. ISBN   9781134229086 . Retrieved 10 December 2019 via Google Books.
  65. 1 2 3 Kort, B.; Reilly, R.; Picard, R.W. (2001). "An affective model of interplay between emotions and learning: Reengineering educational pedagogy-building a learning companion". Proceedings IEEE International Conference on Advanced Learning Technologies. pp. 43–46. doi:10.1109/ICALT.2001.943850. ISBN   0-7695-1013-2. S2CID   9573470 via Academia.edu.
  66. Tiffany Watt Smith. "The Book of Human Emotions: An Encyclopedia of Feeling from Anger to Wanderlust" (PDF). Anarchiveforemotions.com. Archived from the original (PDF) on 18 April 2021. Retrieved 2017-05-28.
  67. "Invisibilia: A Man Finds An Explosive Emotion Locked In A Word". NPR.org. Retrieved 2017-12-29.
  68. "Happily disgusted? Scientists map facial expressions for 21 emotions". The Guardian. 31 March 2014.
  69. Jacque Wilson (2014-04-04). "Happily disgusted? 15 new emotions ID'd". KSL.com. Retrieved 2017-07-16.

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Emotion</span> Conscious subjective experience of humans

Emotions are physical and mental states brought on by neurophysiological changes, variously associated with thoughts, feelings, behavioral responses, and a degree of pleasure or displeasure. There is no scientific consensus on a definition. Emotions are often intertwined with mood, temperament, personality, disposition, or creativity.

Amusement is the state of experiencing humorous and entertaining events or situations while the person or animal actively maintains the experience, and is associated with enjoyment, happiness, laughter and pleasure. It is an emotion with positive valence and high physiological arousal.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Robert Plutchik</span> American psychologist (1927–2006)

Robert Plutchik was an American psychologist who was professor emeritus at the Albert Einstein College of Medicine and adjunct professor at the University of South Florida. He received his Ph.D. from Columbia University. He authored or coauthored more than 260 articles, 45 chapters and eight books and edited seven books. His research interests included the study of emotions, the study of suicide and violence, and the study of the psychotherapy process.

The two-factor theory of emotion posits when an emotion is felt, a physiological arousal occurs and the person uses the immediate environment to search for emotional cues to label the physiological arousal.

Affective neuroscience is the study of how the brain processes emotions. This field combines neuroscience with the psychological study of personality, emotion, and mood. The basis of emotions and what emotions are remains an issue of debate within the field of affective neuroscience.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Affect (psychology)</span> Experience of feeling or emotion

Affect, in psychology, is the underlying experience of feeling, emotion, attachment, or mood. It encompasses a wide range of emotional states and can be positive or negative. Affect is a fundamental aspect of human experience and plays a central role in many psychological theories and studies. It can be understood as a combination of three components: emotion, mood, and affectivity. In psychology, the term affect is often used interchangeably with several related terms and concepts, though each term may have slightly different nuances. These terms encompass: emotion, feeling, mood, emotional state, sentiment, affective state, emotional response, affective reactivity, disposition. Researchers and psychologists may employ specific terms based on their focus and the context of their work.

Surprise is a rapid, fleeting, mental and physiological state. It is related to the startle response experienced by animals and humans as the result of an unexpected event.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Emotionality</span>

Emotionality is the observable behavioral and physiological component of emotion. It is a measure of a person's emotional reactivity to a stimulus. Most of these responses can be observed by other people, while some emotional responses can only be observed by the person experiencing them. Observable responses to emotion do not have a single meaning. A smile can be used to express happiness or anxiety, while a frown can communicate sadness or anger. Emotionality is often used by experimental psychology researchers to operationalize emotion in research studies.

Neuroticism is a personality trait associated with negative emotions. It is one of the Big Five traits. Individuals with high scores on neuroticism are more likely than average to experience such feelings as anxiety, worry, fear, anger, frustration, envy, jealousy, pessimism, guilt, depressed mood, and loneliness. Such people are thought to respond worse to stressors and are more likely to interpret ordinary situations, such as minor frustrations, as appearing hopelessly difficult. Their behavioral responses may include procrastination, substance use, and other maladaptive behaviors, which may temporarily aid in relieving negative emotions and generating positive ones.

The theory of constructed emotion is a theory in affective science proposed by Lisa Feldman Barrett to explain the experience and perception of emotion. The theory posits that instances of emotion are constructed predictively by the brain in the moment as needed. It draws from social construction, psychological construction, and neuroconstruction.

Emotions are biocultural phenomena, meaning they are shaped by both evolution and culture. Emotions are "internal phenomena that can, but do not always, make themselves observable through expression and behavior". While some emotions are universal and are experienced in similar ways as a reaction to similar events across all cultures, other emotions show considerable cultural differences in their antecedent events, the way they are experienced, the reactions they provoke and the way they are perceived by the surrounding society. According to social constructionist theories, emotions are more deeply culturally influenced. The components of emotions are universal, but the patterns are social constructions. Some also theorize that culture is affected by the emotions of the people.

Discrete emotion theory is the claim that there is a small number of core emotions. For example, Silvan Tomkins concluded that there are nine basic affects which correspond with what we come to know as emotions: interest, enjoyment, surprise, distress, fear, anger, shame, dissmell and disgust. More recently, Carroll Izard at the University of Delaware factor analytically delineated 12 discrete emotions labeled: Interest, Joy, Surprise, Sadness, Anger, Disgust, Contempt, Self-Hostility, Fear, Shame, Shyness, and Guilt.

The broaden-and-build theory in positive psychology suggests that positive emotions broaden one's awareness and encourage novel, exploratory thoughts and actions. Over time, this broadened behavioral repertoire builds useful skills and psychological resources. The theory was developed by Barbara Fredrickson around 1998.

Emotion can have a powerful effect on humans and animals. Numerous studies have shown that the most vivid autobiographical memories tend to be of emotional events, which are likely to be recalled more often and with more clarity and detail than neutral events.

Affect measures are used in the study of human affect, and refer to measures obtained from self-report studies asking participants to quantify their current feelings or average feelings over a longer period of time. Even though some affect measures contain variations that allow assessment of basic predispositions to experience a certain emotion, tests for such stable traits are usually considered to be personality tests.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">PAD emotional state model</span> Psychological model based on Pleasure, Arousal and Dominance

The PAD emotional state model is a psychological model developed by Albert Mehrabian and James A. Russell to describe and measure emotional states. PAD uses three numerical dimensions, Pleasure, Arousal and Dominance to represent all emotions. Its initial use was in a theory of environmental psychology, the core idea being that physical environments influence people through their emotional impact. It was subsequently used by Peter Lang and colleagues to propose a physiological theory of emotion. It was also used by James A. Russell to develop a theory of emotional episodes. The PA part of PAD was developed into a circumplex model of emotion experience, and those two dimensions were termed "core affect". The D part of PAD was re-conceptualized as part of the appraisal process in an emotional episode. A more fully developed version of this approach is termed the psychological construction theory of emotion.

Emotional granularity is an individual's ability to differentiate between the specificity of their emotions. Similar to how an interior decorator is aware of fine gradations in shades of blue, where others might see a single color, an individual with high emotional granularity would be able to discriminate between their emotions that all fall within the same level of valence and arousal, labeling their experiences with discrete emotion words. Someone with low emotional granularity would report their emotions in global terms, usually of pleasure or displeasure. It is unknown whether these differences of granularity among individuals stem from an inability of some to verbally label the discrete emotions they feel inside, or whether some people are simply unaware of the distinctions between specific emotions.

Emotion perception refers to the capacities and abilities of recognizing and identifying emotions in others, in addition to biological and physiological processes involved. Emotions are typically viewed as having three components: subjective experience, physical changes, and cognitive appraisal; emotion perception is the ability to make accurate decisions about another's subjective experience by interpreting their physical changes through sensory systems responsible for converting these observed changes into mental representations. The ability to perceive emotion is believed to be both innate and subject to environmental influence and is also a critical component in social interactions. How emotion is experienced and interpreted depends on how it is perceived. Likewise, how emotion is perceived is dependent on past experiences and interpretations. Emotion can be accurately perceived in humans. Emotions can be perceived visually, audibly, through smell and also through bodily sensations and this process is believed to be different from the perception of non-emotional material.

A functional account of emotions posits that emotions facilitate adaptive responses to environmental challenges. In other words, emotions are systems that respond to environmental input, such as a social or physical challenge, and produce adaptive output, such as a particular behavior. Under such accounts, emotions can manifest in maladaptive feelings and behaviors, but they are largely beneficial insofar as they inform and prepare individuals to respond to environmental challenges, and play a crucial role in structuring social interactions and relationships.

In cognitive psychology, the affect-as-information hypothesis, or 'approach', is a model of evaluative processing, postulating that affective feelings provide a source of information about objects, tasks, and decision alternatives. A goal of this approach is to understand the extent of influence that affect has on cognitive functioning. It has been proposed that affect has two major dimensions, namely affective valence and affective arousal, and in this way is an embodied source of information. Affect is thought to impact three main cognitive functions: judgement, thought processing and memory. In a variety of scenarios, the influence of affect on these processes is thought to be mediated by its effects on attention. The approach is thought to account for a wide variety of behavioural phenomena in psychology.