Tifal language

Last updated
Tifal
Tifalmin
Native to Papua New Guinea
RegionSandaun Province, Telefomin District
Ethnicityincl. Urapmin
Native speakers
4,000 (2003) [1]
Dialects
  • Tifal–Urap
  • Atbal
Language codes
ISO 639-3 tif – inclusive code
Individual code:
urm   Urap
Glottolog tifa1245   Tifal
urap1239   Urapmin
ELP
This article contains IPA phonetic symbols. Without proper rendering support, you may see question marks, boxes, or other symbols instead of Unicode characters. For an introductory guide on IPA symbols, see Help:IPA.

Tifal is an Ok language spoken in Papua New Guinea. Dialects are (1) Tifal (Tifalmin) and Urap (Urapmin) and (2) Atbal (Atbalmin).

Contents

Geography

The Tifal language is bounded by Papuan and Irian Jaya speakers to the south and west, the Telefomin valley in the east, and the Sepik river to the north. [2]

Orthography

Phonemicɑɑːbdfiklmnŋostuwj
Lowercaseaaab, pdefiiiklmnngooostuuuwy
UppercaseAAaBDEFIIiKLMNOOoSTUUuWY

Phonology

Consonants

Consonants
Labial Alveolar Palatal Velar
Plosive b t d k
Nasal m n ŋ
Fricative f s
Semivowel w j
Liquid l
/b/ is realized as [ ] word finally, as [ p ] in syllable-coda position before a consonant, and [ b ] elsewhere.
/t/ is realized as [ t ] in syllable coda before a consonant and [ ] elsewhere.
/d/ is realized as [ ɾ ] intervocalically, e.g. /didab/: [dɪˈɾʌpʰ] 'water container'.
/k/ is [ ɣ ] intervocalically, [ k ] in syllable coda before consonants, and [ ] elsewhere.
/s/ is realized as [ ʂ ] before /u/.
/l/ is alveolar adjacent to back vowels and alveodental elsewhere. [3] One dialect realizes /l/ as [ r ] intervocalically.

Vowels

Vowels
Front Central Back
Close i u
Mid o
Open ɑ ɑː

/o/ and /oː/ rarely contrast. [4]

Vowel allophones [5]
PhonemeConditionAllophoneRealization
elsewhere
/i/word-initially and finally[ i ][ ɪ ]
/a/[ a ][ ʌ ]
/u/[ u ][ ʊ ]
/eː/in open syllables, before /m/, and between /j/ and /p/[ ][ ɛː ]
/o/before /n/ or /ŋ/; between /t/ and /k/[ ɔ ][ o ]

Phonotactics

Syllable structure is (C)V(ː)(C). The expression kwiin takan 'oh my!' may be an exception.

/d/ only occurs word-initially. [6] /f/ only occurs syllable-initially. [7] /ŋ/ is always syllable-final. [8]

Initial /l/ only occurs in some dialects. Initial /kw/ occurs in two dialects, and may usually be interpreted as C+V.

/w/ and /j/ occur syllable-initially. [9] Only one dialect allows syllable-coda /j/. [10]

Stress

In inflected words stress lies on the last syllable of the verb stem. Otherwise, if there are long vowels stress falls on the first in the word. If all vowels are short, stress falls on the last syllable if it is closed and the first syllable otherwise.

Grammar

Nouns

Nouns are not inflected but may mark possession. Body parts and kinship terms are obligatorily possessed, and some kinship terms require affixing. On other nouns possession is optional, except for proper names which are never possessed. [11]

Pronouns

Pronoun stems [12]
PersonBasicEmphatic
1singularni-/na-nala-/nalal-/-nila
pluralnuu-/no-nuulu-/nulul-
2singularmkab-kaltab-/kalab-
fkub-kultub-/kulub-
pluralkib-kiltib-
3singularma-ala-/alal-/al-
fu-ulu-/ulul-/ul-
pluralib-/i-iltib-/ilib-/ilal-/il-
Non-emphatic pronouns [13]
Suffix meaning: Poss. Subj. Definitive Inst. Firstwith, and, also
Suffix:-mi~ni-i~-di-yota-siik/-siinsoo/soono
Emphatic pronouns [14]
Suffix meaning: Poss. Inst. 'only''like, simile'
Suffix:-mi~nita-kaltab

Verbs

Tifal has a rich aspectual system. [15] Verbs may be separated into four groups based on how they transform from continuative to punctiliar aspect. Some only have vowel and/or simple stem changes, some have suppletive stems, some change compound-final stems, and some which have allomorphs which add -(a)laa-min (or rarely -daa-laa-min) to the stem. [16]

Verbs also can be divided based on transitivity. Some require direct objects, some with optional objects, some with optional locational objects, and a few intransitive verbs. [17]

Order of verb suffixes [18]
verb ben. ben.-laatensepersonmoodstatement-final marker

Tense and aspect

Most final verbs mark tense, mood, and person, but most verbs can mark aspect and not tense and still be a final verb. [19]

Tense-aspect suffixes [20]
ContinuativePunctiliar
pres-b/m1-d
yesterday past1-m-som/-a-som-b
dist. past-a-s
very remote past-bis-s
abilitative1-m-am1-d-am
near future 1-m-okom1-d-okom
dist. future1-m-okob1-d-okob
  1. "initial consonant of the customary or class changing marker is retained"

Tifal sentences are contain inflected verb-root-chains, often with a final fully conjugated verb. One must inflect for the amount of time between one verb in the chain and the next. [21]

Deixis

Marking spatial relation between verbs and their objects is obligatory. "up" must be clarified as either "upslope" or "upstream", "down" as "downslope" or "downstream", and "across" as "across land" or "across a river". [21]

Kinship

Tifal has dyadic kinship terms (terms referring to the relationship two or more people have to each other), which are present in less than 10 languages and not prevalent in Papua New Guinea. However, they are a salient feature of the Ok languages. Related terms are found in Oksapmin, Mian, and Telefol. [22]

See also

Related Research Articles

In phonetics, rhotic consonants, or "R-like" sounds, are liquid consonants that are traditionally represented orthographically by symbols derived from the Greek letter rho, including ⟨R⟩, ⟨r⟩ in the Latin script and ⟨Р⟩, ⟨p⟩ in the Cyrillic script. They are transcribed in the International Phonetic Alphabet by upper- or lower-case variants of Roman ⟨R⟩, ⟨r⟩:, ,, ,, ,, and.

The phonology of Portuguese varies among dialects, in extreme cases leading to some difficulties in intelligibility. This article focuses on the pronunciations that are generally regarded as standard. Since Portuguese is a pluricentric language, and differences between European Portuguese (EP), Brazilian Portuguese (BP) and Angolan Portuguese (AP) can be considerable, varieties are distinguished whenever necessary.

Northern Sami Most widely spoken of all Sami languages

Northern or North Sami is the most widely spoken of all Sami languages. The area where Northern Sami is spoken covers the northern parts of Norway, Sweden and Finland. The number of Northern Sami speakers is estimated to be somewhere between 15,000 and 25,000. About 2,000 of these live in Finland and between 5,000 and 6,000 in Sweden.

Keres language Language isolate of New Mexico, United States

Keresan, also Keres, is a Native American language, spoken by the Keres Pueblo people in New Mexico. Depending on the analysis, Keresan is considered a small language family or a language isolate with several dialects. The varieties of each of the seven Keres pueblos are mutually intelligible with its closest neighbors. There are significant differences between the Western and Eastern groups, which are sometimes counted as separate languages.

The phonology of Italian describes the sound system—the phonology and phonetics—of Standard Italian and its geographical variants.

Carrier language Athabaskan language spoken in British Columbia

The Dakelh (ᑕᗸᒡ) or Carrier language is a Northern Athabaskan language. It is named after the Dakelh people, a First Nations people of the Central Interior of British Columbia, Canada, for whom Carrier has been a common English name derived from French explorers naming of the people. Dakelh people speak two related languages. One, Babine-Witsuwit'en is sometimes referred to as Northern Carrier. The other includes what are sometimes referred to as Central Carrier and Southern Carrier.

The Persian language has between six and eight vowel phonemes and twenty-six consonant phonemes. It features contrastive stress and syllable-final consonant clusters.

Tsou language

Tsou is a divergent Austronesian language spoken by the Tsou people of Taiwan. Tsou is a threatened language; however, this status is uncertain. Its speakers are located in the west-central mountains southeast of the Chiayi/Alishan area in Taiwan.

Gujarati is an Indo-Aryan language native to the Indian state of Gujarat. Much of its phonology is derived from Sanskrit.

Kara is an Austronesian language spoken by about 5,000 people in 1998 in the Kavieng District of New Ireland Province, Papua New Guinea.

Taos is a Tanoan language spoken by several hundred people in New Mexico, in the United States. The main description of its phonology was contributed by George L. Trager in a (pre-generative) structuralist framework. Earlier considerations of the phonetics-phonology were by John P. Harrington and Jaime de Angulo. Trager's first account was in Trager (1946) based on fieldwork 1935-1937, which was then substantially revised in Trager (1948). The description below takes Trager (1946) as the main point of departure and notes where this differs from the analysis of Trager (1948). Harrington's description is more similar to Trager (1946). Certain comments from a generative perspective are noted in a comparative work Hale (1967).

This article is about the sound system of the Navajo language. The phonology of Navajo is intimately connected to its morphology. For example, the entire range of contrastive consonants is found only at the beginning of word stems. In stem-final position and in prefixes, the number of contrasts is drastically reduced. Similarly, vowel contrasts found outside of the stem are significantly neutralized. For details about the morphology of Navajo, see Navajo grammar.

Nepali is the national language of Nepal. Besides being spoken as a mother tongue by more than 48% of the population of Nepal, it is also spoken in Bhutan and India. The language is recognized in the Nepali constitution as an official language of Nepal.

Telefol is a language spoken by the Telefol people in Papua New Guinea, notable for possessing a base-27 numeral system.

Tundra Nenets is a Uralic language spoken in European Russia and North-Western Siberia. It is the largest and best-preserved language in the Samoyedic group.

Afitti is a language spoken on the eastern side of Jebel el-Dair, a solitary rock formation in the North Kordofan province of Sudan. Although the term ‘Dinik’ can be used to designate the language regardless of cultural affiliation, people in the villages of the region readily recognize the terms ‘Ditti’ and ‘Afitti.’ There are approximately 4000 speakers of the Afitti language and its closest linguistic neighbor is the Nyimang language, spoken west of Jebel el-Dair in the Nuba Mountains of the South Kordofan province of Sudan.

Qaqet, or Baining, is a Papuan language spoken in East New Britain Province on the island of New Britain, Papua New Guinea.

Hachijō language Japonic language spoken in Hachijō

The small group of Hachijō dialects, natively called shima kotoba, depending on classification, are either the most divergent form of Japanese, or comprise a branch of Japonic. Hachijō is currently spoken on two of the Izu Islands south of Tokyo as well as on the Daitō Islands of Okinawa Prefecture, which were settled from Hachijō-jima in the Meiji period. It was also previously spoken on the island of Hachijō-kojima, which is now abandoned. Based on the criterion of mutual intelligibility, Hachijō may be considered a distinct Japonic language.

Wamesa language Austronesian language of West Papua, Indonesia

Wamesa is an Austronesian language of Indonesian New Guinea, spoken across the neck of the Doberai Peninsula or Bird's Head. There are currently 5,000–8,000 speakers. While it was historically used as a lingua franca, it is currently considered to under-documented, endangered language. This means that fewer and fewer children have an active command of Wamesa. Instead, Papuan Malay has become increasingly dominant in the area.

Situ is a Rgyalrong language spoken in Sichuan, China. The name "Situ", literally "four Tusi", comes from a historical name of the Ma'erkang region.

References

  1. Tifal at Ethnologue (18th ed., 2015) (subscription required)
    Urap at Ethnologue (18th ed., 2015) (subscription required)
  2. Boush 1975, 0. Introduction.
  3. Boush 1974, pp. 8-9.
  4. Boush 1974, p. 22.
  5. Boush 1974, pp. 17-19.
  6. Boush 1974, p. 3.
  7. Boush 1974, p. 7.
  8. Boush 1974, p. 8.
  9. Boush 1974, p. 9.
  10. Tifal Organised Phonology Data, p. 3.
  11. Boush 1975, pp. 3-5.
  12. Boush 1975, p. 6.
  13. Boush 1975, p. 7.
  14. Boush 1975, p. 8.
  15. "Aspectual stem distinctions in the Mian verb" (PDF).
  16. Boush 1975, pp. 10-12.
  17. Boush 1975, pp. 13-16.
  18. Boush 1975, p. 16.
  19. Boush 1979, p. 1.
  20. Boush 1975, pp. 22-23.
  21. 1 2 "On Learning a New Guinea Language". Language-Learning-Advisor.com.
  22. The Oksapmin Kinship System Archived 2009-09-20 at the Wayback Machine , retrieved May 21, 2009.

Bibliography

Further reading