Arbore language

Last updated
Arbore
Arbora
Native to Ethiopia
Regionextreme southwest, Omo region
Ethnicity Arbore
Native speakers
7,200 (2007) [1]
Language codes
ISO 639-3 arv
Glottolog arbo1245
ELP Arbore

Arbore is an Afro-Asiatic language spoken by the Arbore people in southern Ethiopia in a few settlements of Hamer woreda near Lake Chew Bahir. [2] [3]

Contents

That the Arbore language belongs within a "Macro-Somali" (now called Omo-Tana) group was first recognized by Sasse (1974). Other members of this group are Daasanach, Bayso, Rendille, Boni and the various Somali dialects. Omo-Tana itself is a major division of Lowland East Cushitic. Arbore's nearest relatives (jointly classified as Western Omo-Tana) are Daasanach and especially the probably extinct Kenyan language of the Elmolo fishermen of Lake Turkana. The sub-grouping is justified [4] in terms of uniquely shared lexicon and certain common grammatical innovations, amongst which the generalizations of the absolute forms of the 1st person singular and 2nd person singular personal pronouns to subject function, thereby replacing the earlier Proto-Lowland East Cushitic forms, e.g. 2nd personal pronouns, e.g., 2nd person singular tai/u 'thou': ki/u 'thee', but Arbore ke 'thou' and 'thee'.

Phonology

The phonology of Arbore was described by Hayward (1984). He lists the following phonemes: [5]

Consonants

Labial Alveolar Palatal Velar Glottal
Nasal m n ɲ ŋ
Plosive Voiceless p [lower-alpha 1] t c k ʔ
Voiced b d ɟ g
Ejective [lower-alpha 1]
Implosive ɓ ɗ
Fricative f s ʃ [lower-alpha 2] h
Liquid Lateral l
Trill r
Glide w j
  1. 1 2 The two phonemes /p/ and /tʼ/ are described as rare and mostly occurring in loan words. [6]
  2. The phoneme /ʃ/ is also rare, and there may be a free variation with /s/. [7]

All consonant phonemes except /h, ʃ, ŋ, p, ʔ/ can be geminated. [8]

Vowels

Front Back
Close i u
Close-mid e o
Open a

Syllable structure

In Arbore, no word begins or ends with more than one consonantal segment. Moreover, no word contains a sequence of more than two consonantal segments internally. [9]

Changes of sounds in certain phonological and morphological environments

There are instances in which sounds differ from their usual pronunciation. The differences follow certain patterns and these patterns are: [10]

Grammar

Arbore well exemplifies a number of typical Lowland East Cushitic features such as: a three-term number system (basic unit: singulative: plural) in nouns, within which "polarity" figures, i.e., gender alternations across the various number forms of a lexeme; a morphosyntax thoroughly deployed in distinguishing topic and contrastive focus; great morphophonological complexity in its verbal derivation and inflection. [11] Of historical interest is the language's preservation of at least a dozen verbs of the archaic "Prefix Conjugation", often attributed to Proto-Afroasiatic itself.

Morphology

Pluralizing nouns

There are several patterns for the way the plural of nouns can be formed. Which pattern is used depends on morphological, lexical and phonological categories. For consonant-final, masculine nouns, -mé is suffixed. Half of these forms are sr forms ending in -n, - č or -t. However, simple ur forms and feminine forms also possible in this pattern. To masculine, consonant-final nouns, -a?ame is added as a suffix in the plural. Additionally, there is a fairly large group of mostly feminine nouns with an h-tone in the end, to which -n is suffixed in the plural. To other feminine nouns with a final consonant, -ó as added a suffix. Plural nouns with a final vowel have -má as a suffix to indicate the plural. There are further patterns of the pluralization of nouns, but these other patterns are only minor. [12]

Adjectives

Adjectives in Arbore have either a predicative or an attributive function and their form depends on this function. In the predicative function, adjectives only have one form which does not change depending on the gender of the subject noun. In the attributive function they act as a modifier in a noun phrase. In this case, the adjective has two forms: in the non-plural form, -a is added as a suffix, and in the plural form, -o is suffixed. Adjectives also have forms with reduplicated stems, which can be substituted for the single-stem forms when the noun is semantically plural. Otherwise, these adjectives work the same way as single-stem adjectives. [13]

Pronouns

There are simple forms and suffixed forms of independent subject pronouns, which are summarized in the table below. [14] The simple forms are used in sentences with either one other focussed constituent or in neutral sentences without a focussed constituent. [15] For the -he form, it is important to note that it cannot take the function of an object. The -t form can replace the simple form and can function as subject and object in sentences without focussed constituents. All other forms can stand as nominal predicates. [16]

Simple form-he form-t form-s(s)e form-ta formø/-he/-t + -lo form
1sY[éhe]y[éhe]lo
2sK[éhe]k[éhe]lo
1p?onó?onos(s)e?onolo
2p?ín?inse?inlo
Abs. nom.
3ms?us(s)ú ús(s)u?us(s)út ?ús(s)ut?us(s)uta?us(s)u[l:]o
3fs?es(s)ét ?és(s)et?es(s)eta?es(s)u[l:]o
3p?os(s)ót ?ós(s)ot?os(s)ota?os(s)u[l:]o

Inflection of verbs based on tense

There are different patterns of verb conjugation. These can be divided into two classes: the prefix conjugations and the suffix conjugations.

There are two patterns of prefix conjugation, which is rarer than the suffix conjugation, only applying to 12 verbs in total. [17]

  1. Pattern 1: the stem of the verb has two different alternants; one having a stem-vocalism in l, and the other having a stem-vocalism in e. The first alternant occurs in all 1s and all affirmative imperative forms. The second one occurs in all other verb forms.
  2. Pattern 2: this pattern has two alternants as well. The first one has a stem-vocalism in a mid-vowel (e or o) and occurs in all negative jussive and imperative forms and the 3p jussive affirmative verb forms. The second one has a stem-vocalism in a and occurs in all other verb forms.

There are the following patterns of suffix conjugation: [18]

  1. Subject marker alternants: 2s, 2p, 3fs suffixed subject markers have two alternants each: -t, and -ø. -ø is used for causative 1 extended stem verbs and -t- is used elsewhere.
  2. Stem-alternants – causative 1: this pattern involves a threefold alternation in the form of radical extension. There is -i- used for 1p members of perfect affirmative, imperfect affirmative, imperfect negative and jussive affirmative. Additionally, there is -s-, which is used for 1s, 3ms, and 3p members of perfect affirmative, pluperfect affirmative, imperfect affirmative, imperfect negative, and jussive affirmative; all members of the perfect negative, jussive negative and imperative negative, the plural affirmative imperative. Lastly, -is- is used for all other verb forms.
  3. Stem alternants – Middle 1 and Middle 2: this pattern exhibits a threefold alternation in the form of radical extension. Due to parallels in shape and distribution, it is treated in sets. The first alternant exhibits a final d and occurs in1s members of perfect affirmative, pluperfect affirmative, imperfect affirmative, imperfect negative and jussive affirmative paradigms, and plural imperative affirmative. The second alternant occurs in 3ms and 3p members of perfect affirmative, pluperfect affirmative and jussive affirmative paradigms; all members of the perfect and jussive negative paradigms except 1s, and in singular and plural imperative negative. This alternant exhibits a final t. The last alternant ends in an -a and is used in all other verb forms.
  4. Stem alternants – Middle 3: this pattern has a fourfold alternation in the form of the radical extension. -a- occurs in singular imperative affirmative forms. -d-, however, is used in the 1s members of perfect affirmative, pluperfect affirmative, imperfect affirmative, imperfect negative and jussive affirmative forms, and in plural imperative affirmative forms. -t- occurs in 3ms and 3p members of perfect affirmative, pluperfect affirmative, imperfect affirmative, imperfect negative and jussive affirmative, all the members of the perfect and jussive negative paradigms except 1s, and in singular and plural imperative negative forms. Lastly, -aha- is used elsewhere.
  5. Stem alternants – Middle 4: here, two alternants occur. The first one exhibits the suffix -add- which is used for 1s members of perfect affirmative, pluperfect affirmative, imperfect affirmative, imperfect negative and jussive affirmative; plural imperative affirmative. The second one, -at-, is used elsewhere.
  6. Stem alternants – Inchoative: this pattern has two alternants as well. There is - ø- which is used for 2s, 2p, 3fs and 1p members of perfect affirmative, pluperfect affirmative, imperfect affirmative and negative; and the 3fs and 1p members of the jussive affirmative forms. For the other forms, -w- is used.
  7. Stem alternants – Plural imperative marker alternants: in this pattern, the terminal vowel which marks the plural in the imperative affirmative has an optional alternant, i.e. -e-/-a-, and -a-. The former one is used in all causative and middle extended-stem verbs. The latter one occurs in the other verb forms.

Prepositions

In Arbore, there are two types of prepositions. The first type has a nominal origin and is homophonous with nouns denoting either locations or body parts. Focussed noun phrases take the phrase-final clitic subject pronoun. Location words, however, appear to be more postpositional in that they occur as heads of genitival constructions, and the clitic-pronoun is attached to them. The second type does not have a nominal counterpart and does not appear in any other syntactic position. Some of these prepositions are bound to a particular verb lexically. ?ár, for example, generally indicates movement towards the speaker when the verb is an intransitive verb of motion, or activity an activity with motion. Additionally, there is ?ug, which generally indicates movement away from the speaker. Ka is the commonest preposition, which can have several meanings depending on the context. It can have an ablative sense with some verbs, and with other verbs a dative or beneficiary meaning. But it can also carry a sense of location in place or time. Lastly, there is kaŋ, which always relates to instrumentality or path. [19]

Numerals

Numerals in Arbore share many grammatical properties of nouns. They have an inherent gender, even though the phonological shape or tone do not always correlate with their counterparts in nouns. Additionally, semantic plurality is grammatically more important than gender. The numbers 1-10 and one hundred are listed with their gender in the table below. [20]

EnglishArbore
onetakká (f), tokkó (m)
twolaamá (p)
threeseezzé (p)
fourʔafúr (f)
fivečénn (f)
sixǧih (f)
seventuzba (p)
eightsuyé (f)/(p)
ninesaagald (f)
tentommon~d (f)
one hundreddibbá (m)

Syntax

Word order

In neutral sentences, the word order is the following: subject (S); preverbal selections (PVS); complements (if necessary); verb (V). Complement phrases can precede the PVS, but a subject noun phrase (NP) can never be used after the PVC. Also in all other types of sentences the verb generally comes last. [21]

In questions, the word order is: S – question word – verb. Arbore has three interrogative pronouns which can be translated with "which" and depend on the gender of the subject: búko for masculine subjects; bítoko for feminines; and toko for plurals. Furthermore, there is the question pronoun bíteh meaning "whose". When asking for an amount, kaakó is used. [22]

Possessive clauses

In Arbore, the pronominal possessor is attached to the possessed as a suffix. These suffixes have different forms depending on the gender of the noun: -h- is used for masculine and plural nouns and -t- with feminine nouns. The gender suffixes can not stand on their own. The gender suffix -h- occurs with vowel-final nouns and is assimilated in consonant-final nouns. The masculine and plural marker is independent of the spelling of the head noun. Additionally, there are seven possessive definitives that are attached to the head noun as a suffix following the gender marker:

  • 1s: -áw
  • 2s: -áako/ -áakko
  • 3ms: -ásut/ -ássut
  • 3fs: -áset/ -ásset
  • 1p: -ánno
  • 2p: -áň
  • 3p: -áso/ -ásso [23]

The possessive definitives are attached to the head noun of a sentence following a suffix with the owner being suffixed to the thing that is being owned| [24]

Relative clauses

Relative clauses always follow their heads, meaning that the suffixes are gender-sensitive and are always attached to the head noun. The suffixes are: -h- for masculine nouns, and -t- for feminine nouns. Exceptions to this are nouns whose head takes the deictic definitive -lo-. In relative clauses, a preverbal selector as well as a predicate identifying element can never occur together with the relative clause verb. A bound subject pronoun can occur, however. [25]

Notes

  1. Arbore at Ethnologue (25th ed., 2022) Closed Access logo transparent.svg
  2. Klaus Wedekind, "Sociolinguistic Survey Report of the Languages of the Gawwada, Tsamay and Diraasha Areas with Excursions to Birayle (Ongota) and Arbore (Irbore) Part II" SIL Electronic Survey Reports SILESR 2002-066. Includes a word-list of Arbore with 320 entries.
  3. "2007 Census". Archived from the original on 2012-11-13. Retrieved 2010-12-16.
  4. Hayward, Richard (1984). The Arbore Language: a First Investigation. Including a Vocabulary. Hamburg: Kuschitische Sprachstudien 2.
  5. Hayward 1984, p. 51, the phoneme characters are altered to reflect the sound values according to the IPA
  6. Hayward 1984, p. 51
  7. Hayward 1984, p. 52
  8. Hayward 1984, p. 54
  9. Hayward (1984). The Arbore Language: A First Investigation; Including a Vocabulary. Kuschitische Sprachstudien 2: Burke. p. 58.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: location (link)
  10. Hayward, Dick (1984). The Arbore Language: A First Investigation; Including a Vocabulary (Kuschitische Sprachstudien 2 ed.). Burke. pp. 62–88.
  11. Hayward, Richard (2003). "Arbore language". Encyclopedia Aethiopica. Wiesbaden: Harrassowitz Verlag.
  12. Hayward, Dick (1984). The Arbore Language: A First Investigation; Including a Vocabulary (Kuschitische Sprachstudien 2 ed.). pp. 166–178.
  13. Hayward, Dick (1984). The Arbore Language: A First Investigation; Including a Vocabulary (Kuschitische Sprachstudien 2. ed.). Buske. pp. 200–201.
  14. Hayward, Dick (1984). The Arbore Language: A First Investigation; Including a Vocabulary (Kuschitische Sprachstudien 2 ed.). p. 215.
  15. (p. 216)
  16. Hayward, Dick (1984). The Arbore Language: A First Investigation; Including a Vocabulary (Kuschitische Sprachstudien 2 ed.). pp. 315–318.
  17. Hayward, Dick (1984). The Arbore Language: A First Investigation; Including a Vocabulary (Kuschitische Sprachstudien 2 ed.). Buske. pp. 261–262.
  18. Hayward, Dick (1984). The Arbore Language: A First Investigation; Including a Vocabulary (Kuschitische Sprachstudien 2 ed.). Buske. pp. 271–274.
  19. Hayward, Dick (1984). The Arbore Language: A First Investigation; Including a Vocabulary (Kuschitische Sprachstudien 2. ed.). pp. 308–313.
  20. Hayward, Dick (1984). The Arbore Language: A First Investigation; Including a Vocabulary. Kuschitische Sprachstudien 2. pp. 208–209.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: location (link) CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link)
  21. Hayward, Dick (1984). The Arbore Language: A First Investigation; Including a Vocabulary. Kuschitische Sprachstudien 2. pp. 110–111.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: location (link) CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link)
  22. Hayward, Dick (1984). The Arbore Language: A First Investigation; Including a Vocabulary (Kuschitische Sprachstudien 2 ed.). pp. 199–200.
  23. Hayward, Dick (1984). The Arbore Language: A First Investigation; Including a Vocabulary. Kuschitische Sprachstudien 2. pp. 184–185.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: location (link) CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link)
  24. Hayward, Dick (1984). The Arbore Language: A First Investigation; Including a Vocabulary (Kuschitische Sprachstudien 2 ed.). Burke.
  25. Hayward, Dick (1984). The Arbore Language: A First Investigation; Including a Vocabulary. Kuschitische Sprachstudien 2: Burke. pp. 314–315.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: location (link)

Further reading

Related Research Articles

The Finnish language is spoken by the majority of the population in Finland and by ethnic Finns elsewhere. Unlike the languages spoken in neighbouring countries, such as Swedish and Norwegian, which are North Germanic languages, or Russian, which is a Slavic language, Finnish is a Uralic language of the Finnic languages group. Typologically, Finnish is agglutinative. As in some other Uralic languages, Finnish has vowel harmony, and like other Finnic languages, it has consonant gradation.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Oromo language</span> Cushitic language of Ethiopia and Kenya

Oromo, historically also called Galla, is an Afroasiatic language that belongs to the Cushitic branch. It is native to the Ethiopian state of Oromia and Northern Kenya and is spoken predominantly by the Oromo people and neighboring ethnic groups in the Horn of Africa. It is used as a lingua franca particularly in the Oromia Region and northeastern Kenya.

Beja is an Afroasiatic language of the Cushitic branch spoken on the western coast of the Red Sea by the Beja people. Its speakers inhabit parts of Egypt, Sudan and Eritrea. In 2022 there were 2,550,000 Beja speakers in Sudan, and 121,000 Beja speakers in Eritrea according to Ethnologue. As of 2023 there are an estimated 88,000 Beja speakers in Egypt. The total number of speakers in all three countries is 2,759,000.

Neo-Mandaic, also known as Modern Mandaic, sometimes called the "ratna", is the modern reflex of the Mandaic language, the liturgical language of the Mandaean religious community of Iraq and Iran. Although severely endangered, it survives today as the first language of a small number of Mandaeans in Iran and in the Mandaean diaspora. All Neo-Mandaic speakers are multilingual in the languages of their neighbors, Arabic and Persian, and the influence of these languages upon the grammar of Neo-Mandaic is considerable, particularly in the lexicon and the morphology of the noun. Nevertheless, Neo-Mandaic is more conservative even in these regards than most other Neo-Aramaic languages.

Argobba is an Ethiopian Semitic language spoken in several districts of Afar, Amhara, and Oromia regions of Ethiopia by the Argobba people. It belongs to the South Ethiopic languages subgroup, and is closely related to Amharic.

Wintu is a Wintu language which was spoken by the Wintu people of Northern California. It was the northernmost member of the Wintun family of languages. The Wintun family of languages was spoken in the Shasta County, Trinity County, Sacramento River Valley and in adjacent areas up to the Carquinez Strait of San Francisco Bay. Wintun is a branch of the hypothetical Penutian language phylum or stock of languages of western North America, more closely related to four other families of Penutian languages spoken in California: Maiduan, Miwokan, Yokuts, and Costanoan.

The grammar of Classical Nahuatl is agglutinative, head-marking, and makes extensive use of compounding, noun incorporation and derivation. That is, it can add many different prefixes and suffixes to a root until very long words are formed. Very long verbal forms or nouns created by incorporation, and accumulation of prefixes are common in literary works. New words can thus be easily created.

In Hebrew, verbs, which take the form of derived stems, are conjugated to reflect their tense and mood, as well as to agree with their subjects in gender, number, and person. Each verb has an inherent voice, though a verb in one voice typically has counterparts in other voices. This article deals mostly with Modern Hebrew, but to some extent, the information shown here applies to Biblical Hebrew as well.

Unless otherwise indicated, Tigrinya verbs in this article are given in the usual citation form, the third person singular masculine perfect.

The roots of the reconstructed Proto-Indo-European language (PIE) are basic parts of words to carry a lexical meaning, so-called morphemes. PIE roots usually have verbal meaning like "to eat" or "to run". Roots never occurred alone in the language. Complete inflected verbs, nouns, and adjectives were formed by adding further morphemes to a root and potentially changing the root's vowel in a process called ablaut.

Southern Oromo, or Borana, is a variety of Oromo spoken in southern Ethiopia and northern Kenya by the Borana people. Günther Schlee also notes that it is the native language of a number of related peoples, such as the Sakuye.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Nukak language</span> Endangered indigenous language of Colombia

The Nukak language is a language of uncertain classification, perhaps part of the macrofamily Puinave-Maku. It is very closely related to Kakwa.

Dirasha is a member of the Cushitic branch of the Afro-Asiatic family. It is spoken in the Omo region of Ethiopia, in the hills west of Lake Chamo, around the town of Gidole.

Baiso or Bayso is an Afro-Asiatic, more specifically a Lowland East Cushitic language belonging to the Omo-Tana subgroup, and is spoken in Ethiopia, in the region around Lake Abaya.

This article deals with the grammar of the Udmurt language.

Konda-Dora, also known simply as Konda or Kubi, is a Dravidian language spoken in India. It is spoken by the scheduled tribe of the Konda-Dora, who mostly live in the districts of Vizianagaram, Srikakulam, and East Godavari in Andhra Pradesh, and the Koraput district in Odisha.

Grass Koiari (Koiali) is a Papuan language of Papua New Guinea spoken in the inland Port Moresby area. It is not very close to the other language which shares its name, Mountain Koiali. It is considered a threatened language.

Arabic verbs, like the verbs in other Semitic languages, and the entire vocabulary in those languages, are based on a set of two to five consonants called a root. The root communicates the basic meaning of the verb, e.g. ك-ت-ب k-t-b 'write', ق-ر-ء q-r-ʾ 'read', ء-ك-ل ʾ-k-l 'eat'. Changes to the vowels in between the consonants, along with prefixes or suffixes, specify grammatical functions such as person, gender, number, tense, mood, and voice.

Tamashek or Tamasheq is a variety of Tuareg, a Berber macro-language widely spoken by nomadic tribes across North Africa in Algeria, Mali, Niger, and Burkina Faso. Tamasheq is one of the three main varieties of Tuareg, the others being Tamajaq and Tamahaq.