Pronunciation of English ⟨th⟩

Last updated • 24 min readFrom Wikipedia, The Free Encyclopedia

In English, the digraph th usually represents either the voiced dental fricative phoneme /ð/ (as in this) or the voiceless dental fricative phoneme /θ/ (as in thing). Occasionally, it stands for /t/ (as in Thailand, or Thomas). In the word eighth, it is often pronounced /tθ/. In compound words, th may be a consonant sequence rather than a digraph (as in the /t.h/ of lighthouse).

Contents

General description

In standard English, the phonetic realization of the two dental fricative phonemes shows less variation than many other English consonants. Both are pronounced either interdentally, with the blade of the tongue resting against the lower part of the back of the upper teeth and the tip protruding slightly, or with the tip of the tongue against the back of the upper teeth. For some speakers these two positions are in free variation, while for other speakers they are in complementary distribution, the position behind the teeth being used when the dental fricative stands in proximity to an alveolar fricative /s/ or /z/, as in myths (/θs/) or clothes (/ðz/). Lip configuration may vary depending on phonetic context. The vocal folds are abducted. The velopharyngeal port is closed. Air forced between tongue surface and cutting edge of the upper teeth (interdental) or inside surface of the teeth (dental) creates audible frictional turbulence.

The difference between /θ/ and /ð/ is normally described as a voiceless–voiced contrast, the distinction that native speakers are most aware of. They are also distinguished by other phonetic markers: the fortis /θ/ is pronounced with more muscular tension than the lenis /ð/; and /θ/ is more strongly aspirated than /ð/, as can be demonstrated by holding a hand in front of the mouth as they are spoken.

Phonology and distribution

In modern English, /θ/ and /ð/ are distinct phonemes, not merely allophones, as demonstrated by minimal pairs such as thigh:thy, ether:either, teeth:teethe. They are distinguished from the neighbouring labiodental fricatives, sibilants and alveolar stops by such minimal pairs as thought:fought/sought/taught and then:Venn/Zen/den.

The vast majority of words in English spelled with th have /θ/, and almost all newly created words do. However, the high frequency of the function words, particularly the, means that /ð/ is more frequent in actual use.

As a general rule, in initial position, /θ/ is used except in certain function words; in medial position, /ð/ is used except for certain foreign loan words; and in final position, /θ/ is used except in certain verbs. A more detailed explanation follows.

Initial position

Medial position

Final position

Plurals

Grammatical alternation

In pairs of related words, an alternation between /θ/ and /ð/ is possible, which may be thought of as a kind of consonant mutation. Typically /θ/ appears in the singular of a noun, /ð/ in the plural and in the related verb: cloth/θ/, clothes/ð/, to clothe/ð/. This is directly comparable to the /s/-/z/ or /f/-/v/ alternation in house, houses or wolf, wolves. It goes back to the allophonic variation in Old English (see below), where it was possible for þ to be in final position and thus voiceless in the basic form of a word, but in medial position and voiced in a related form. The loss of inflections then brought the voiced medial consonant to the end of the word. Often a remnant of the old inflection can be seen in the spelling in the form of a silent e, which may be thought of synchronically as a marker of the voicing.

Regional differences in distribution

The above discussion follows Daniel Jones' English Pronouncing Dictionary, an authority on standard British English, and Webster's New World College Dictionary, an authority on American English. Usage appears much the same between the two. Regional variation within standard English includes the following:

History of the English phonemes

Germanic origins

Proto-Indo-European (PIE) had no dental fricatives, but these evolved in the earliest stages of the Germanic languages. In Proto-Germanic, /ð/ and /θ/ were separate phonemes, usually represented in Germanic studies by the symbols *đ and *þ.

In West Germanic, the Proto-Germanic *đ shifted further to *d, leaving only one dental fricative phoneme. However, a new [ð] appeared as an allophone of /θ/ in medial positions by assimilation of the voicing of the surrounding vowels. [θ] remained in initial and presumably in final positions (though later terminal devoicing would in any case have eliminated the evidence of final [ð]). This West Germanic phoneme, complete with its distribution of allophones, survived into Old English. In German and Dutch, it shifted to a /d/, the allophonic distinction simply being lost. In German, West Germanic *d shifted to /t/ in what may be thought of as a chain shift, but in Dutch, *þ, *đ and *d merged into a single /d/.

The whole complex of Germanic dentals, and the place of the fricatives within it, can be summed up in this table:

PIEProto-GermanicWest GermanicOld EnglishGermanDutchNotes
*t*[þ][θ]/d//d/Original *t in initial position, or in final position after a stressed vowel
*[đ][ð]Original *t in medial position after a stressed vowel
*d/d//t/Original *t after an unstressed vowel
*dʰOriginal *dʰ in all positions
*d*t*t/t//s/ or /ts//t/Original *d in all positions

Note that this table shows only the basic rules. The actual developments in all of the mentioned languages are more complicated (due to dialectal variation, peculiar developments in consonant clusters, etc.). For more on these phonemes from a comparative perspective, see Grammatischer Wechsel. For the developments in German and Dutch see High German consonant shift.

Old English

Thus English inherited a phoneme /θ/ in positions where other West Germanic languages have /d/ and most other Indo-European languages have /t/: English three, German drei, Latin tres.

In Old English, the phoneme /θ/, like all fricative phonemes in the language, had two allophones, one voiced and one voiceless, which were distributed regularly according to phonetic environment.

Although Old English had two graphemes to represent these sounds, þ (thorn) and ð (eth), it used them interchangeably, unlike Old Icelandic, which used þ for /θ/ and ð for /ð/.

Development up to Modern English

The most important development on the way to modern English was the investing of the existing distinction between [ð] and [θ] with phonemic value. Minimal pairs, and hence the phonological independence of the two phones, developed as a result of three main processes.

  1. In early Middle English times, a group of very common function words beginning with /θ/ (the, they, there, etc.) came to be pronounced with /ð/ instead of /θ/. Possibly this was a sandhi development; as these words are frequently found in unstressed positions, they can sometimes appear to run on from the preceding word, which may have resulted in the dental fricative being treated as though it were word-internal. This allowed a word-initial minimal pair like thigh:thy.
  2. English has borrowed many words from Greek, including a vast number of scientific terms. Where the original Greek had the letter θ (theta), English usually retained the Late Greek pronunciation regardless of phonetic environment, resulting in the presence of /θ/ in medial position (anthem, methyl, etc.). This allowed a medial minimal pair like ether:either.
  3. English has lost its original verb inflections. When the stem of a verb ends with a dental fricative, this was usually followed by a vowel in Old English, and was therefore voiced. It is still voiced in modern English, even though the verb inflection has disappeared leaving the /ð/ at the end of the word. Examples are to bathe, to mouth, to breathe. Sometimes a remnant of the original vowel remained in the spelling (see: Silent e), but this was inconsistent. This allowed a minimal pair in final position like loath:loathe.

Other changes that affected these phonemes included a shift /d//ð/ when followed by unstressed suffix -er. Thus Old English fæder became modern English father; likewise mother, gather, hither, together, weather (from mōdor, gaderian, hider, tōgædere, weder). In a reverse process, Old English byrþen and morþor or myrþra become burden and murder (compare the obsolete variants burthen and murther).

Dialectally, the alternation between /d/ and /ð/ sometimes extends to other words, as bladder, ladder, solder with /ð/ (possibly being restricted elsewhere by the former two clashing with blather and lather). On the other hand, some dialects retain original d, and extend it to other words, as brother, further, rather. The Welsh name Llewelyn appears in older English texts as Thlewelyn (Rolls of Parliament (Rotuli parliamentorum) I. 463/1, King Edward I or II), and Fluellen (Shakespeare, Henry V). Th also occurs dialectally for wh, as in thirl, thortleberry, thorl, for whirl, whortleberry, whorl. Conversely, Scots has whaing, whang, white, whittle, for thwaing, thwang, thwite, thwittle.

The old verb inflection -eth (Old English -eþ) was replaced by -s (he singeth → he sings), not a sound shift but a completely new inflection.

Dialectal realizations

In some dialects the "th"-sound phonemes /θ/ and /ð/ are pronounced differently from the dental fricatives [θ] and [ð]. Most common are: substitution with labiodental fricatives [f] and [v] (fronting), substitution with alveolar stops [t] and [d] (stopping), and substitution with alveolar fricatives [s] and [z] (alveolarization). Fronting and stopping are more common among speakers of English dialects, whereas alveolarization is more common among language learners whose first languages are French, German, or Mandarin. To speakers of varieties in which /θ/ and /ð/ are pronounced [θ] and [ð], fronting and stopping are generally considered to have less of a marked contrast with the standard pronunciation than alveolarization, which is often more stigmatized.

A fourth, less common substitution is [h] for /θ/ word-initially or intervocalically. This is called debuccalization, and somewhat prevalent in Scottish English.

th-fronting

In some areas, such as London, and certain dialects, including African American Vernacular English and less commonly New Zealand, many people realize the phonemes /θ/ and /ð/ as [f] and [v], respectively. Although traditionally stigmatized as typical of a Cockney accent, this pronunciation is fairly widespread, especially when immediately surrounded by other fricatives for ease of pronunciation, and has, in the early 20th century, become an increasingly noticeable feature of the Estuary English accent of South East England. It has in at least one case been transferred into standard English as a neologism: a bovver boy is a thug, a "boy" who likes "bother" (fights). Joe Brown and his Bruvvers was a Pop group of the 1960s. The song "Fings ain't wot they used t'be" was the title song of a 1959 Cockney comedy. Similarly, a New Zealander from the northernmost parts of the country might state that he or she is from "Norfland".

Note that, at least in Cockney, a word beginning with /ð/ (as opposed to its voiceless counterpart /θ/) can never be labiodental. Instead, it is realized as any of [ð, ð̞, d, l, ʔ], or is dropped altogether. [3] [4]

th-stopping

Many speakers of African American Vernacular English, Caribbean English, Liberian English, Nigerian English, Philadelphia English, and Philippine English (along with other Asian English varieties) pronounce the fricatives /θ,ð/ as alveolar stops [t,d]. Similarly, but still distinctly, many speakers of New York City English, Chicago English, Boston English, Indian English, Newfoundland English, and Hiberno-English use the dental stops [t̪,d̪] (typically distinct from alveolar [t,d]) instead of, or in free variation with, [θ,ð]. Native speakers of most Indo-Aryan languages often substitute the dental fricatives [θ,ð] with the voiceless aspirated and voiced dental stops [t̪ʰ,d̪], respectively.

In Cockney, the th-stopping may occur when a word begins with /ð/ (but not its voiceless counterpart /θ/). [3] [4] This is also associated with the accent of the English city of Sheffield (such as the nickname dee-dahs for residents) but such pronunciations are now confined to the very oldest residents of Sheffield. [5]

th-alveolarization

Th-alveolarization is a process that occurs in some African varieties of English where the dental fricatives /θ,ð/ merge with the alveolar fricatives /s/ and /z/. It is an example of assibilation.

In rarer or older varieties of African American Vernacular English, /θ/ may be pronounced [s] after a vowel and before another consonant, as in bathroom[ˈbæsɹum]. [6]

Th-alveolarization is often parodied as typical of French-, Japanese-, and German-speaking learners of English, but it is widespread among many other foreign learners because the dental fricative "th" sounds are not very common among the world's languages. Due to the said ridicule, learners who are unable to realize these sounds sometimes opt for the less marked th-fronting or th-stopping instead of alveolarization.[ citation needed ]

Homophonous pairs
/s,z//θ,ð/IPANotes
aceeighthˈeɪseighth more often merges with eights (see below)
bassbathˈbæsbass, the fish; but distinct in dialects with broad A
Bess Beth ˈbɛs
breezebreatheˈbɹiːz
closeclotheˈkloʊz
closeclothesˈkloʊz
eightseighthˈeɪts
Erse earthˈɜː(r)s
facefaithˈfeɪs
forceforthˈfoə(r)s
forcefourthˈfoə(r)s
frostfrothedˈfrɒst,ˈfrɔːst
grossgrowthˈɡroʊs
kisskithˈkɪs
layslatheˈleɪz
lazelatheˈleɪz
lieslitheˈlaɪz
louse Louth ˈlaʊs
lyselitheˈlaɪz
massmathˈmæs
messmethˈmɛs
missmythˈmɪs
monthsmonthˈmʌns
mossmothˈmɒs,ˈmɔːs
mousemouthˈmaʊs
passpathˈpæs, ˈpɑːs
pisspithˈpɪs
purse Perth ˈpɜː(r)s
racewraithˈreɪs
risewritheˈraɪz
Ross Roth ˈrɒs,ˈrɔːs
ryeswritheˈraɪz
saithighˈsaɪ
sanethaneˈseɪn
sanethegnˈseɪn
sankthankˈsæŋk
sawthawˈsɔː
saw Thor ˈsɔːIn most Non-rhotic accents; specifically those without the Cot-caught merger.
seamthemeˈsiːm
seasseetheˈsiːz
seemthemeˈsiːm
seesseetheˈsiːz
seizeseetheˈsiːz
sickthickˈsɪk
sighthighˈsaɪ
sinthinˈsɪn
singthingˈsɪŋ
sinkthinkˈsɪŋk
sixsixthˈsɪks
sizescytheˈsaɪz
soarthawˈsɔː Non-rhotic acents with horse-hoarse merger.
soar Thor ˈsɔː(r)With horse-hoarse merger.
soaredthawedˈsɔːd Non-rhotic accents with horse-hoarse merger.
somethumbˈsʌm
songthongˈsɒŋ,ˈsɔːŋ
sorethawˈsɔː Non-rhotic accents with horse-hoarse merger.
sore Thor ˈsɔː(r)With horse-hoarse merger.
soredthawedˈsɔːd Non-rhotic accents with horse-hoarse merger.
sortthoughtˈsɔːt Non-rhotic accents.
soughtthoughtˈsɔːt
sudsthudsˈsʌdz
sumthumbˈsʌm
sumpthumpˈsʌmp
sunderthunderˈsʌndə(r)
sunkthunkˈsʌŋk
swartthwartˈswɔː(r)t
swordthawedˈsɔːd Non-rhotic accents with horse-hoarse merger.
tensetenthˈtɛns
tentstenthˈtɛn(t)s
trucetruthˈtruːs
use (n)youthˈjuːs
whizwithˈwɪzWith wine-whine merger.
wizardwitheredˈwɪzə(r)d
worseworthˈwə(r)s
wrassewrathˈræs
wreath Reece ˈriːs
wreath Rhys ˈriːs
Z; zeetheˈziːThe before vowels and silent H; but distinct in dialects where Z is [zɛd]
Z; zeetheeˈziːbut distinct in dialects where Z is [zɛd]
Zs; zeestheseˈziːz
zenthenˈzɛn

th-debuccalization

In many varieties of Scottish English, /θ/ becomes [h] word initially and intervocalically. [7]

Th-debuccalization occurs mainly in Glasgow and across the Central Belt. A common example is [hɪŋk] for think. This feature is becoming more common in these places over time, but is still variable.[ citation needed ] In word final position, [θ] is used, as in standard English.

The existence of local [h] for /θ/ in Glasgow complicates the process of th-fronting there, a process which gives [f] for historical /θ/. Unlike in the other dialects with th-fronting, where [f] solely varies with [θ], in Glasgow, the introduction of th-fronting there creates a three-way variant system of [h], [f] and [θ].

Use of [θ] marks the local educated norms (the regional standard), while use of [h] and [f] instead mark the local non-standard norms. [h] is well known in Glasgow as a vernacular variant of /θ/ when it occurs at the start of a word and intervocalically, while [f] has only recently risen above the level of social consciousness.

Given that th-fronting is a relatively recent innovation in Glasgow, it was expected that linguists might find evidence for lexical diffusion for [f] and the results found from Glaswegian speakers confirm this.[ citation needed ] The existing and particular lexical distribution of th-debuccalization imposes special constraints on the progress of th-fronting in Glasgow.

In accents with th-debuccalization, the cluster /θr/ becomes [hr],[ citation needed ] giving these dialects a consonant cluster that does not occur in other dialects. The replacement of /θr/ with [hr] leads to pronunciations like:

Assimilation

As with many English consonants, a process of assimilation can result in the substitution of other speech sounds in certain phonetic environments. Native speakers do this subconsciously.

At word boundaries, alveolar stops next to dental fricatives assimilate very regularly, especially in rapid colloquial speech, involving both the place of articulation and the manner of articulation: the alveolar stops become dental , while the dental fricatives become stops . [8] [9] [10] [11] The resulting consonant is usually long (geminated) which may be the only audible cue for the speaker to distinguish particular words (for example, the definite and indefinite articles, compare "run the mile" [ˈɹʌn̪n̪əˈmaɪl] and "run a mile" [ˈɹʌnəˈmaɪl]). [11]

in the: /ɪnðə/[ɪn̪n̪ə]
join the army: /ˈdʒɔɪnðiˈɑːmi/[ˈdʒɔɪn̪n̪iˈɑːmi]
read these: /riːdðiːz/[ɹiːd̪d̪iːz]
right there: /raɪtˈðɛə/[ɹaɪt̪ˈt̪ɛə] (more commonly: [ɹaɪʔˈðɛə], with a glottal stop)
fail the test: /ˈfeɪlðəˈtɛst/[ˈfeɪl̪l̪əˈtɛst]

The alveolar fricatives may become dental as well: [11]

this thing: /ðɪsθɪŋ/[ðɪs̪θɪŋ] or [ðɪs̪s̪ɪŋ]
takes them: /teɪksðəm/[teɪks̪ðm̩] or [teɪks̪s̪m̩]
was this: /wɒzðɪs/[wɒz̪ðɪs] or [wɒz̪z̪ɪs]

/θ/ and /ð/ can also be lost through elision: [12] [13] months[mʌns], clothes[kloʊz]. In rapid speech, sixth(s) may be pronounced like six. [14] Them may be contracted to 'em, and in this case the contraction is often indicated in writing. Some linguists see 'em as originally a separate word, a remnant of Old English hem, but as the apostrophe shows, it is perceived in modern English as a contraction of them. [15]

Acquisition problems

Children generally learn the less marked phonemes of the language before the more marked ones. In the case of English-speaking children, /θ/ and /ð/ are often among the last phonemes to be learnt, frequently not being mastered before the age of five. Prior to this age, many children substitute the sounds [f] and [v] respectively. For small children, fought and thought are therefore homophones. As British and American children begin school at age four and five respectively, this means that many are learning to read and write before they have sorted out these sounds, and the infantile pronunciation is frequently reflected in their spelling errors: ve fing for the thing.

Children with a lisp, however, have trouble distinguishing /θ/ and /ð/ from /s/ and /z/ respectively in speech, using a single /θ/ or /ð/ pronunciation for both, and may never master the correct sounds without speech therapy. The lisp is a common speech impediment in English.

Foreign learners may have parallel problems. Learners from very many cultural backgrounds have difficulties with English dental fricatives, usually caused by interference with either sibilants or stops. Words with a dental fricative adjacent to an alveolar fricative, such as clothes( /kloʊðz/ or /kloʊz/ ), truths /tɹuθs/ , fifths( /fɪfθs/ or /fɪθs/ ), sixths( /sɪksθs/ ), anesthetic( /ˌænəsˈθɛtɪk/ ), etc., are commonly very difficult for foreign learners to pronounce. Some of these words containing consonant clusters can also be difficult for native speakers, including those using the standard /θ/ and /ð/ pronunciations generally, allowing such accepted informal pronunciations of clothes as /kloʊz/ (a homophone of the verb close) and fifth(s) as /fɪθ(s)/.

History of the digraph

th for /θ/ and /ð/

Though English speakers take it for granted, the digraph th is in fact not an obvious combination for a dental fricative. The origins of this have to do with developments in Greek.

Proto-Indo-European had an aspirated /dʱ/ that came into Greek as /tʰ/, spelled with the letter theta. In the Greek of Homer and Plato, this was still pronounced /tʰ/, and therefore when Greek words were borrowed into Latin, theta was transcribed with th. Since /tʰ/ sounds like /t/ with a following puff of air, th was the logical spelling in the Latin alphabet.

By the time of New Testament Greek (koiné), however, the aspirated stop had shifted to a fricative: /tʰ/→/θ/. Thus theta came to have the sound that it still has in Modern Greek, and which it represents in the IPA. From a Latin perspective, the established digraph th now represented the voiceless fricative /θ/, and was used thus for English by French-speaking scribes after the Norman Conquest, since they were unfamiliar with the Germanic graphemes ð (eth) and þ (thorn). Likewise, the spelling th was used for /θ/ in Old High German prior to the completion of the High German consonant shift, again by analogy with the way Latin represented the Greek sound. It also appeared in early modern Swedish before a final shift to /d/.

The history of the digraphs ph for /f/ and ch for Scots, Welsh or German /x/ is parallel.

th for /t/

Since neither /tʰ/ nor /θ/ was a native sound in Latin, the tendency emerged at the latest in medieval Latin, to substitute /t/. Thus, in many modern languages, including French and German, the th digraph is used in Greek loan-words to represent an original /θ/, but is now pronounced /t/: examples are French théâtre, German Theater. In some cases, this etymological th, which has no remaining significance for pronunciation, has been transferred to words in which there is no etymological justification for it. For example, German Tal ('valley', cognate with English dale) appears in many place-names with an archaic spelling Thal (contrast Neandertal and Neanderthal). The German spelling reform of 1901 largely reversed these, but they remain in some proper nouns. The name Rothschild is an example of this, being a compound of rot[h] ("red") and Schild ("shield").

Examples of this are also to be found in English, perhaps influenced immediately by French. In some Middle English manuscripts, th appears for t or d: tho 'to' or 'do', thyll till, whythe white, thede deed. In Modern English we see it in Esther, Thomas, Thames, thyme, Witham (the town in Essex, not the river in Lincolnshire which is pronounced with /ð/) and the old spelling of Satan as Sathan. More recently, the name of the capital of Nepal was often written Katmandu down to the late 20th century, but is now usually spelt Kathmandu.

In a small number of cases, this spelling later influenced the pronunciation: amaranth, amianthus and author have spelling pronunciations with /θ/, and some English speakers use /θ/ in Neanderthal.

th for /th/

A few English compound words, such as lightheaded or hothouse, have the letter combination th split between the parts, though this is not a digraph. Here, the t and h are pronounced separately (light-headed) as a cluster of two consonants. Other examples are anthill, goatherd, lighthouse, outhouse, pothead; also in words formed with the suffix -hood: knighthood, and the similarly formed Afrikaans loanword apartheid . In a few place names ending in t+ham, the t-h boundary has been lost and become a spelling pronunciation, for example Grantham.

See also

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">H</span> 8th letter of the Latin alphabet

H, or h, is the eighth letter of the Latin alphabet, used in the modern English alphabet, including the alphabets of other western European languages and others worldwide. Its name in English is aitch, or regionally haitch, plural haitches.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Non-native pronunciations of English</span> Overview of English-learners pronunciation

Non-native pronunciations of English result from the common linguistic phenomenon in which non-native speakers of any language tend to transfer the intonation, phonological processes and pronunciation rules of their first language into their English speech. They may also create innovative pronunciations not found in the speaker's native language.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Ll</span> Digraph

Ll/ll is a digraph that occurs in several languages.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Digraph (orthography)</span> Pair of characters used to write one phoneme

A digraph or digram is a pair of characters used in the orthography of a language to write either a single phoneme, or a sequence of phonemes that does not correspond to the normal values of the two characters combined.

The voiceless dental non-sibilant fricative is a type of consonantal sound used in some spoken languages. It is familiar to most English speakers as the 'th' in think. Though rather rare as a phoneme among the world's languages, it is encountered in some of the most widespread and influential ones. The symbol in the International Phonetic Alphabet that represents this sound is ⟨θ⟩, and the equivalent X-SAMPA symbol is T. The IPA symbol is the lowercase Greek letter theta, which is used for this sound in post-classical Greek, and the sound is thus often referred to as "theta".

Dutch orthography uses the Latin alphabet. The spelling system is issued by government decree and is compulsory for all government documentation and educational establishments.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Ch (digraph)</span> Latin-script digraph

Ch is a digraph in the Latin script. It is treated as a letter of its own in the Chamorro, Old Spanish, Czech, Slovak, Igbo, Uzbek, Quechua, Ladino, Guarani, Welsh, Cornish, Breton, Ukrainian, Japanese, Latynka, and Belarusian Łacinka alphabets. Formerly ch was also considered a separate letter for collation purposes in Modern Spanish, Vietnamese, and sometimes in Polish; now the digraph ch in these languages continues to be used, but it is considered as a sequence of letters and sorted as such.

In linguistics, assibilation is a sound change resulting in a sibilant consonant. It is a form of spirantization and is commonly the final phase of palatalization.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Th (digraph)</span> Latin-script digraph

Th is a digraph in the Latin script. It was originally introduced into Latin to transliterate Greek loan words. In modern languages that use the Latin alphabet, it represents a number of different sounds. It is the most common digraph in order of frequency in the English language.

Ancient Greek phonology is the reconstructed phonology or pronunciation of Ancient Greek. This article mostly deals with the pronunciation of the standard Attic dialect of the fifth century BC, used by Plato and other Classical Greek writers, and touches on other dialects spoken at the same time or earlier. The pronunciation of Ancient Greek is not known from direct observation, but determined from other types of evidence. Some details regarding the pronunciation of Attic Greek and other Ancient Greek dialects are unknown, but it is generally agreed that Attic Greek had certain features not present in English or Modern Greek, such as a three-way distinction between voiced, voiceless, and aspirated stops ; a distinction between single and double consonants and short and long vowels in most positions in a word; and a word accent that involved pitch.

Bhutia is a language of the Tibeto-Burman languages spoken by the Bhutia people in Sikkim in northeast India, parts of Koshi province in eastern Nepal, and Bhutan. It is one of the official languages of Sikkim.

This article describes those aspects of the phonological history of English which concern consonants.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Portuguese orthography</span> Alphabet and spelling

Portuguese orthography is based on the Latin alphabet and makes use of the acute accent, the circumflex accent, the grave accent, the tilde, and the cedilla to denote stress, vowel height, nasalization, and other sound changes. The diaeresis was abolished by the last Orthography Agreement. Accented letters and digraphs are not counted as separate characters for collation purposes.

This article discusses the phonological system of the Czech language.

Unlike many languages, Icelandic has only very minor dialectal differences in sounds. The language has both monophthongs and diphthongs, and many consonants can be voiced or unvoiced.

In phonology, voicing is a sound change where a voiceless consonant becomes voiced due to the influence of its phonological environment; shift in the opposite direction is referred to as devoicing or surdization. Most commonly, the change is a result of sound assimilation with an adjacent sound of opposite voicing, but it can also occur word-finally or in contact with a specific vowel.

The phonology of Burmese is fairly typical of a Southeast Asian language, involving phonemic tone or register, a contrast between major and minor syllables, and strict limitations on consonant clusters.

The phonology of Old Saxon mirrors that of the other ancient Germanic languages, and also, to a lesser extent, that of modern West Germanic languages such as English, Dutch, Frisian, German, and Low German.

The central northeastern dialect of Brazilian Portuguese is a dialect spoken in the central part of the Northeast Region, Brazil, in all the states of Rio Grande do Norte, Paraíba, Alagoas and Sergipe, much of the state of Pernambuco, north of Bahia, south of Ceará, southeastern of Piauí and a few regions of Maranhão. It has roughly about 53,000,000 native speakers and varies within the region. This dialect shares similarities between north coast, Baiano and Recifense dialects.

References

Citations

  1. The American Heritage Dictionary, 1969.
  2. Kenyon, John S.; Knott, Thomas A. (1953) [1944]. A Pronouncing Dictionary of American English. Springfield, Mass.: Merriam-Webster. p.  87. ISBN   0-87779-047-7.
  3. 1 2 Wright (1981 :137)
  4. 1 2 Wells (1982 :329)
  5. Stoddart, Upton and Widdowson in Urban Voices, Arnold, London, 1999, p. 79
  6. Phonological Features of African American Vernacular English
  7. Weber, Tobias (February 2014). Principles in the Emergence and Evolution of Linguistic Features in World Englishes. ISBN   9783954891917.
  8. Roach, Peter (2009). English Phonetics and Phonology. Cambridge University Press. p. 112. ISBN   978-0-521-71740-3.
  9. Gramley, Stephan; Pátzold, Michael (2004). A Survey of Modern English. Routledge. p. 76. ISBN   978-1-134-42046-9.
  10. Collins, Beverley; Mees, Inger M. (2013). Practical Phonetics and Phonology: A Resource Book for Students. Routledge. p. 122. ISBN   978-0-415-50649-6.
  11. 1 2 3 Shockey, Linda (2003). "2.5.1 ð-reduction". Sound Patterns of Spoken English. pp. 43–44. ISBN   9780470758243.
  12. Bauer, Laurie; Lieber, Rochelle; Plag, Ingo (2015). The Oxford Reference Guide to English Morphology. Oxford University Press. p. 131. ISBN   978-0-19-874706-2.
  13. Collins, Beverley; Mees, Inger M. (2013). Practical Phonetics and Phonology: A Resource Book for Students. Routledge. p. 127. ISBN   978-0-415-50649-6.
  14. Jones, Daniel (2006). Roach, Peter; Hartman, James; Setter, Jane (eds.). "Introduction". Cambridge English Pronouncing Dictionary (17th ed.). Cambridge University Press. ISBN   0-521-68086-7.
  15. Online Etymology Dictionary. 'em. Retrieved 18 September 2006.

Sources