Imitation

Last updated

A toddler imitates his father. Walter and Seth Singleton.jpg
A toddler imitates his father.

Imitation (from Latin imitatio, "a copying, imitation" [1] ) is a behavior whereby an individual observes and replicates another's behavior. Imitation is also a form of that leads to the "development of traditions, and ultimately our culture. It allows for the transfer of information (behaviors, customs, etc.) between individuals and down generations without the need for genetic inheritance." [2] The word imitation can be applied in many contexts, ranging from animal training to politics. [3] The term generally refers to conscious behavior; subconscious imitation is termed mirroring. [4]

Contents

Anthropology and social sciences

In anthropology, some theories hold that all cultures imitate ideas from one of a few original cultures or several cultures whose influence overlaps geographically. Evolutionary diffusion theory holds that cultures influence one another, but that similar ideas can be developed in isolation.

Scholars [5] as well as popular authors [6] [7] have argued that the role of imitation in humans is unique among animals. However, this claim has been recently challenged by scientific research which observed social learning and imitative abilities in animals.

Psychologist Kenneth Kaye showed [8] [9] that the ability of infants to match the sounds or gestures of an adult depends on an interactive process of turn-taking over many successive trials, in which adults' instinctive behavior plays as great a role as that of the infant. These writers assume that evolution would have selected imitative abilities as fit because those who were good at it had a wider arsenal of learned behavior at their disposal, including tool-making and language.

However, research also suggests that imitative behaviors and other social learning processes are only selected for when outnumbered or accompanied by asocial learning processes: an over-saturation of imitation and imitating individuals leads humans to collectively copy inefficient strategies and evolutionarily maladaptive behaviors, thereby reducing flexibility to new environmental contexts that require adaptation. [10] Research suggests imitative social learning hinders the acquisition of knowledge in novel environments and in situations where asocial learning is faster and more advantageous. [11] [12]

In the mid-20th century, social scientists began to study how and why people imitate ideas. Everett Rogers pioneered innovation diffusion studies, identifying factors in adoption and profiles of adopters of ideas. [13] Imitation mechanisms play a central role in both analytical and empirical models of collective human behavior. [14]

Neuroscience

Humans are capable of imitating movements, actions, skills, behaviors, gestures, pantomimes, mimics, vocalizations, sounds, speech, etc. and that we have particular "imitation systems" in the brain is old neurological knowledge dating back to Hugo Karl Liepmann. Liepmann's model 1908 "Das hierarchische Modell der Handlungsplanung" (the hierarchical model of action planning) is still valid. On studying the cerebral localization of function, Liepmann postulated that planned or commanded actions were prepared in the parietal lobe of the brain's dominant hemisphere, and also frontally. His most important pioneering work is when extensively studying patients with lesions in these brain areas, he discovered that the patients lost (among other things) the ability to imitate. He was the one who coined the term "apraxia" and differentiated between ideational and ideomotor apraxia. It is in this basic and wider frame of classical neurological knowledge that the discovery of the mirror neuron has to be seen. Though mirror neurons were first discovered in macaques, their discovery also relates to humans. [15]

Human brain studies using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) revealed a network of regions in the inferior frontal cortex and inferior parietal cortex which are typically activated during imitation tasks. [16] It has been suggested that these regions contain mirror neurons similar to the mirror neurons recorded in the macaque monkey. [17] However, it is not clear if macaques spontaneously imitate each other in the wild.

Neurologist V. S. Ramachandran argues that the evolution of mirror neurons were important in the human acquisition of complex skills such as language and believes the discovery of mirror neurons to be a most important advance in neuroscience. [18] However, little evidence directly supports the theory that mirror neuron activity is involved in cognitive functions such as empathy or learning by imitation. [19]

Evidence is accumulating that bottlenose dolphins employ imitation to learn hunting and other skills from other dolphins. [20] [21]

Japanese monkeys have been seen to spontaneously begin washing potatoes after seeing humans washing them. [22]

Mirror neuron system

Research has been conducted to locate where in the brain specific parts and neurological systems are activated when humans imitate behaviors and actions of others, discovering a mirror neuron system. This neuron system allows a person to observe and then recreate the actions of others. Mirror neurons are premotor and parietal cells in the macaque brain that fire when the animal performs a goal directed action and when it sees others performing the same action." [23] Evidence suggests that the mirror neuron system also allows people to comprehend and understand the intentions and emotions of others. [24] Problems of the mirror neuron system may be correlated with the social inadequacies of autism. There have been many studies done showing that children with autism, compared with typically-developing children, demonstrate reduced activity in the frontal mirror neuron system area when observing or imitating facial emotional expressions. Of course, the higher the severity of the disease, the lower the activity in the mirror neuron system is. [23]

Animal behavior

Scientists debate whether animals can consciously imitate the unconscious incitement from sentinel animals, whether imitation is uniquely human, or whether humans do a complex version of what other animals do. [25] [26] The current controversy is partly definitional. Thorndike uses "learning to do an act from seeing it done." [27] It has two major shortcomings: first, by using "seeing" it restricts imitation to the visual domain and excludes, e.g., vocal imitation and, second, it would also include mechanisms such as priming, contagious behavior and social facilitation, [28] which most scientist distinguish as separate forms of observational learning. Thorpe suggested defining imitation as "the copying of a novel or otherwise improbable act or utterance, or some act for which there is clearly no instinctive tendency." [29] This definition is favored by many scholars, though questions have been raised how strictly the term "novel" has to be interpreted and how exactly a performed act has to match the demonstration to count as a copy.

Hayes and Hayes (1952) used the "do-as-I-do" procedure to demonstrate the imitative abilities of their trained chimpanzee "Viki." [30] Their study was repeatedly criticized for its subjective interpretations of their subjects' responses. Replications of this study [31] found much lower matching degrees between subjects and models. However, imitation research focusing on the copying fidelity got new momentum from a study by Voelkl and Huber. [32] They analyzed the motion trajectories of both model and observer monkeys and found a high matching degree in their movement patterns.

Paralleling these studies, comparative psychologists provided tools or apparatuses that could be handled in different ways. Heyes [33] [34] and co-workers reported evidence for imitation in rats that pushed a lever in the same direction as their models, though later on they withdrew their claims due to methodological problems in their original setup. [35] By trying to design a testing paradigm that is less arbitrary than pushing a lever to the left or to the right, Custance and co-workers [36] introduced the "artificial fruit" paradigm, where a small object could be opened in different ways to retrieve food placed inside—not unlike a hard-shelled fruit. Using this paradigm, scientists reported evidence for imitation in monkeys [37] [38] and apes. [39] [40] [41] There remains a problem with such tool (or apparatus) use studies: what animals might learn in such studies need not be the actual behavior patterns (i.e., the actions) that were observed. Instead they might learn about some effects in the environment (i.e., how the tool moves, or how the apparatus works). [42] This type of observational learning, which focuses on results, not actions, has been dubbed emulation (see Emulation (observational learning)).

In an article written by Carl Zimmer, he looked into a study being done by Derek Lyons, focusing on human evolution, in which he studied a chimpanzee. He first started with showing the chimpanzee how to retrieve food from a box. The chimpanzee soon caught on and did exactly what the scientist just did. They wanted to see if the chimpanzee's brain functioned just like a human brain, so they replicated the experiment using 16 children, following the same procedure; once the children saw how it was done, they followed the same exact steps. [43]

Imitation in animals

Blackbird imitating the vehicle motion alarm of a local garbage truck in Brastad, Sweden.

Imitation in animals is a study in the field of social learning where learning behavior is observed in animals specifically how animals learn and adapt through imitation. Ethologists can classify imitation in animals by the learning of certain behaviors from conspecifics. [44] More specifically, these behaviors are usually unique to the species and can be complex in nature and can benefit the individual's survival. [44]

Some scientists believe true imitation is only produced by humans, arguing that simple learning though sight is not enough to sustain as a being who can truly imitate. [45] Thorpe defines true imitation as "the copying of a novel or otherwise improbable act or utterance, or some act for which there is clearly no instinctive tendency," which is highly debated for its portrayal of imitation as a mindless repeating act. [45] True imitation is produced when behavioral, visual and vocal imitation is achieved, not just the simple reproduction of exclusive behaviors. [45] Imitation is not a simple reproduction of what one sees; rather it incorporates intention and purpose. [45] Animal imitation can range from survival purpose; imitating as a function of surviving or adapting, to unknown possible curiosity, which vary between different animals and produce different results depending on the measured intelligence of the animal. [45]

There is considerable evidence to support true imitation in animals. [46] Experiments performed on apes, birds and more specifically the Japanese quail have provided positive results to imitating behavior, demonstrating imitation of opaque behavior. [46] However the problem that lies is in the discrepancies between what is considered true imitation in behavior. [46] Birds have demonstrated visual imitation, where the animal simply does as it sees. [46] Studies on apes however have proven more advanced results in imitation, being able to remember and learn from what they imitate. [46] Studies have demonstrated far more positive results with behavioral imitation in primates and birds than any other type of animal. [46] Imitation in non-primate mammals and other animals have been proven difficult to conclude solid positive results for and poses a difficult question to scientists on why that is so. [46]

Theories

There are two types of theories of imitation, transformational and associative. Transformational theories suggest that the information that is required to display certain behavior is created internally through cognitive processes and observing these behaviors provides incentive to duplicate them. [47] Meaning we already have the codes to recreate any behavior and observing it results in its replication. Albert Bandura's "social cognitive theory" is one example of a transformational theory. [48] Associative, or sometimes referred to as "contiguity", [49] theories suggest that the information required to display certain behaviors does not come from within ourselves but solely from our surroundings and experiences. [47] These theories have not yet provided testable predictions in the field of social learning in animals and have yet to conclude strong results. [47]

New developments

There have been three major developments in the field of animal imitation. The first, behavioral ecologists and experimental psychologists found there to be adaptive patterns in behaviors in different vertebrate species in biologically important situations. [50] The second, primatologists and comparative psychologists have found imperative evidence that suggest true learning through imitation in animals. [50] The third, population biologists and behavioral ecologists created experiments that demand animals to depend on social learning in certain manipulated environments. [50]

Child development

Developmental psychologist Jean Piaget noted that children in a developmental phase he called the sensorimotor stage (a period which lasts up to the first two years of a child) begin to imitate observed actions. [51] This is an important stage in the development of a child because the child is beginning to think symbolically, associating behaviors with actions, thus setting the child up for the development of further symbolic thinking. Imitative learning also plays a crucial role in the development of cognitive and social communication behaviors, such as language, play, and joint attention. Imitation serves as both a learning and a social function because new skills and knowledge are acquired, and communication skills are improved by interacting in social and emotional exchanges. It is shown, however, that "children with autism exhibit significant deficits in imitation that are associated with impairments in other social communication skills." [52] To help children with autism, reciprocal imitation training (RIT) is used. It is a naturalistic imitation intervention that helps teach the social benefits of imitation during play by increasing child responsiveness and by increasing imitative language. [52]

Reinforcement learning, both positive and negative, and punishment, are used by people that children imitate to either promote or discontinue behavior. If a child imitates a certain type of behavior or action and the consequences are rewarding, the child is very likely to continue performing the same behavior or action. The behavior "has been reinforced (i.e. strengthened)". [53] [ self-published source? ] However, if the imitation is not accepted and approved by others, then the behavior will be weakened.

Naturally, children are surrounded by many different types of people that influence their actions and behaviors, including parents, family members, teachers, peers, and even characters on television programs. These different types of individuals that are observed are called models. According to Saul McLeod, "these models provide examples of masculine and feminine behavior to observe and imitate." [53] Children imitate the behavior they have observed from others, regardless of the gender of the person and whether or not the behavior is gender appropriate. However, it has been proven that children will reproduce the behavior that "its society deems appropriate for its sex." [53]

Infants

Infants have the ability to reveal an understanding of certain outcomes before they occur, therefore in this sense they can somewhat imitate what they have perceived. Andrew N. Meltzoff, ran a series of tasks involving 14-month-old infants to imitate actions they perceived from adults. In this gathering he had concluded that the infants, before trying to reproduce the actions they wish to imitate, somehow revealed an understanding of the intended goal even though they failed to replicate the result wished to be imitated. These task implicated that the infants knew the goal intended. [54] Gergely, Bekkering, and Király (2002) figured that infants not only understand the intended goal but also the intentions of the person they were trying to imitate engaging in "rational imitation", as described by Tomasello, Carpenter and others [54]

It has long been claimed that newborn humans imitate bodily gestures and facial expressions as soon as their first few days of life. [55] [56] For example, in a study conducted at the Mailman Centre for Child Development at the University of Miami Medical School, 74 newborn babies (with a mean age of 36 hours) were tested to see if they were able to imitate a smile, a frown and a pout, and a wide-open mouth and eyes. An observer stood behind the experimenter (so he/she couldn't see what facial expressions were being made by the experimenter) and watched only the babies' facial expressions, recording their results. Just by looking only at the babies' faces, the observer was more often able to correctly guess what facial expression was being presented to the child by the experimenter. [57] After the results were calculated, "the researchers concluded that...babies have an innate ability to compare an expression they see with their own sense of muscular feedback from making the movements to match that expression." [57]

However, the idea that imitation is an inborn ability has been recently challenged. A research group from the University of Queensland in Australia carried out the largest-ever longitudinal study of neonatal imitation in humans. One hundred and nine newborns were shown a variety of gestures including tongue protrusion, mouth opening, happy and sad facial expressions, at four time points between one week and 9 weeks of age. The results failed to reveal compelling evidence that newborns imitate: Infants were just as likely to produce matching and non-matching gestures in response to what they saw. [58]

At around eight months, infants will start to copy their child care providers' movements when playing pat-a-cake and peek-a-boo, as well as imitating familiar gestures, such as clapping hands together or patting a doll's back. At around 18 months, infants will then begin to imitate simple actions they observe adults doing, such as taking a toy phone out of a purse and saying "hello", pretending to sweep with a child-sized broom, as well as imitating using a toy hammer.[ citation needed ]

Toddlers

At around 30–36 months, toddlers will start to imitate their parents by pretending to get ready for work and school and saying the last word(s) of what an adult just said. For example, toddlers may say "bowl" or "a bowl" after they hear someone say, "That's a bowl." They may also imitate the way family members communicate by using the same gestures and words. For example, a toddler will say, "Mommy bye-bye" after the father says, "Mommy went bye-bye." [59]

Toddlers love to imitate their parents and help when they can; imitation helps toddlers learn, and through their experiences lasting impressions are made. 12- to 36-month-olds learn by doing, not by watching, and so it is often recommended to be a good role model and caretaker by showing them simple tasks like putting on socks or holding a spoon. [60]

Duke developmental psychologist Carol Eckerman did a study on toddlers imitating toddlers and found that at the age of 2 children involve themselves in imitation play to communicate with one another. This can be seen within a culture or across different cultures. 3 common imitative patterns Eckerman found were reciprocal imitation, follow-the-leader, and lead-follow. [61]

Kenneth Kaye's "apprenticeship" theory of imitation rejected assumptions that other authors had made about its development. His research showed that there is no one simple imitation skill with its own course of development. What changes is the type of behavior imitated. [62]

An important agenda for infancy is the progressive imitation of higher levels of use of signs, until the ultimate achievement of symbols. The principal role played by parents in this process is their provision of salient models within the facilitating frames that channel the infant's attention and organize his imitative efforts.

Gender and age differences

A small boy of Matera, Italy, unconsciously repeats the gesture of his grandmother's hands, ca. 1948 - ca. 1955 Matera, Italy. Still unaware of despair, a small boy of Matera, Italy, unconsciously repeats the gesture of his... - NARA - 541739.tif
A small boy of Matera, Italy, unconsciously repeats the gesture of his grandmother's hands, ca. 1948 – ca. 1955

Imitation and imitative behaviors do not manifest ubiquitously and evenly in all human individuals; some individuals rely more on imitated information than others. [63] Although imitation is very useful when it comes to cognitive learning with toddlers, research has shown that there are some gender and age differences when it comes to imitation. Research done to judge imitation in toddlers 2–3 years old shows that when faced with certain conditions "2-year-olds displayed more motor imitation than 3-year-olds, and 3-year-olds displayed more verbal-reality imitation than 2-year-olds. Boys displayed more motor imitation than girls." [64]

No other research is more controversial pertaining gender differences in toddler imitation than psychologist, Bandura's, bobo doll experiments. [65] The goal of the experiment was to see what happens to toddlers when exposed to aggressive and non-aggressive adults, would the toddlers imitate the behavior of the adults and if so, which gender is more likely to imitate the aggressive adult. In the beginning of the experiment Bandura had several predictions that actually came true. Children exposed to violent adults will imitate the actions of that adult when the adult is not present, boys who had observed an adult of the opposite sex act aggressively are less likely to act violently than those who witnessed a male adult act violently. In fact "boys who observed an adult male behaving violently were more influenced than those who had observed a female model behavior aggressively". One observation was that while boys are likely to imitate physical acts of violence, girls are likely to imitate verbal acts of violence.

Negative imitation

Imitation plays a major role on how a toddler interprets the world. Much of a child's understanding is derived from imitation, due to a lack of verbal skill imitation in toddlers for communication.[ citation needed ] It is what connects them to the communicating world, as they continue to grow they begin to learn more. This may mean that it is crucial for parents to be cautious as to how they act and behave around their toddlers. Imitation is the toddlers way of confirming and dis-conforming socially acceptable actions in society. Actions like washing dishes, cleaning up the house and doing chores are actions you want your toddlers to imitate. Imitating negative things is something that is never beyond young toddlers. If they are exposed to cursing and violence, it is going to be what the child views as the norm of their world, since imitation is the "mental activity that helps to formulate the conceptions of the world for toddlers". [66] So it is important for parents to be careful what they say or do in front of their children.[ citation needed ]

Autism

Children with autism exhibit significant impairment in imitation skills. [52] Imitation deficits have been reported on a variety of tasks including symbolic and non-symbolic body movements, symbolic and functional object use, vocalizations, and facial expressions. [52] In contrast, typically-developing children can copy a broad range of novel (as well as familiar) rules from a very early age. [67] Problems with imitation discriminate children with autism from those with other developmental disorders as early as age 2 and continue into adulthood. [68]

Children with autism exhibit significant deficits in imitation that are associated with impairments in other social communication skills. It is unclear whether imitation is mediating these relationships directly, or whether they are due to some other developmental variable that is also reflected in the measurement of imitation skills. [52]

On the contrary, research from the early 21st century suggests that people affected with forms of high-functioning autism easily interact with one another by using a more analytically-centered communication approach rather than an imitative cue-based approach, [69] suggesting that reduced imitative capabilities do not affect abilities for expressive social behavior but only the understanding of said social behavior. Social communication is not negatively affected when said communication involves less or no imitation. Children with autism may have significant problems understanding typical social communication not because of inherent social deficits, but because of differences in communication style which affect reciprocal understanding. [70] [71]

Autistic individuals are also shown to possess increased analytical, cognitive, and visual processing, [72] [73] [74] suggesting that they have no true impairments in observing the actions of others but may decide not to imitate them because they do not analytically understand them. [75] A 2016 study has shown that involuntary, spontaneous facial mimicry – which supposedly depends on the mirror neuron system – is intact in individuals with autism, contrasting with previous studies and suggesting that the mirror neuron system is not inherently broken in autistic individuals. [76]

Automatic imitation

The automatic imitation comes very fast when a stimulus is given to replicate. The imitation can match the commands with the visual stimulus (compatible) or it cannot match the commands with the visual stimulus (incompatible). For example: 'Simon Says', a game played with children where they are told to follow the commands given by the adult. In this game, the adult gives the commands and shows the actions; the commands given can either match the action to be done or it will not match the action. The children who imitate the adult who has given the command with the correct action will stay in the game. The children who imitate the command with the wrong action will go out of the game, and this is where the child's automatic imitation comes into play. Psychologically, the visual stimulus being looked upon by the child is being imitated faster than the imitation of the command. In addition, the response times were faster in compatible scenarios than in incompatible scenarios. [77]

Children are surrounded by many different people, day by day. Their parents make a big impact on them, and usually what the children do is what they have seen their parent do. In this article they found that a child, simply watching its mother sweep the floor, right after soon picks up on it and starts to imitate the mother by sweeping the floor. By the children imitating, they are really teaching themselves how to do things without instruction from the parent or guardian. Toddlers love to play the game of house. They picked up on this game of house by television, school or at home; they play the game how they see it. The kids imitate their parents or anybody in their family. In the article it says it is so easy for them to pick up on the things they see on an everyday basis.[ citation needed ]

Over-imitation

Over-imitation is "the tendency of young children to copy all of an adult model's actions, even components that are irrelevant for the task at hand." [78] According to this human and cross-cultural phenomenon, a child has a strong tendency to automatically encode the deliberate action of an adult as causally meaningful even when the child observes evidence that proves that its performance is unnecessary. It is suggested that over-imitation "may be critical to the transmission of human culture." Experiments done by Lyons et al. (2007) has shown that when there are obvious pedagogical cues, children tend to imitate step by step, including many unnecessary steps; without pedagogical cues, children will simply skip those useless steps. [79]

However, another study suggests that children do not just "blindly follow the crowd" since they can also be just as discriminating as adults in choosing whether an unnecessary action should be copied or not. [80] They may imitate additional but unnecessary steps to a novel process if the adult demonstrations are all the same. However, in cases where one out of four adults showed a better technique, only 40% actually copied the extra step, as described by Evans, Carpenter and others. [81] Children's imitation is selective, also known as "selective imitation". Studies have shown that children tend to imitate older, competitive, and trustworthy individuals. [82]

Deferred imitation

Piaget coined the term deferred imitation and suggested that it arises out of the child's increasing ability to "form mental representations of behavior performed by others." [51] Deferred imitation is also "the ability to reproduce a previously witnessed action or sequence of actions in the absence of current perceptual support for the action." [2] Instead of copying what is currently occurring, individuals repeat the action or behavior later on. It appears that infants show an improving ability for deferred imitation as they get older, especially by 24 months. By 24 months, infants are able to imitate action sequences after a delay of up to three months, meaning that "they're able to generalize knowledge they have gained from one test environment to another and from one test object to another." [2]

A child's deferred imitation ability "to form mental representations of actions occurring in everyday life and their knowledge of communicative gestures" has also been linked to earlier productive language development. [83] Between 9 (preverbal period) and 16 months (verbal period), deferred imitation performance on a standard actions-on-objects task was consistent in one longitudinal study testing participants' ability to complete a target action, with high achievers at 9 months remaining so at 16 months. Gestural development at 9 months was also linked to productive language at 16 months. Researchers now believe that early deferred imitation ability is indicative of early declarative memory, also considered a predictor of productive language development.

See also

Related Research Articles

Observational learning is learning that occurs through observing the behavior of others. It is a form of social learning which takes various forms, based on various processes. In humans, this form of learning seems to not need reinforcement to occur, but instead, requires a social model such as a parent, sibling, friend, or teacher with surroundings. Particularly in childhood, a model is someone of authority or higher status in an environment. In animals, observational learning is often based on classical conditioning, in which an instinctive behavior is elicited by observing the behavior of another, but other processes may be involved as well.

Empathy is generally described as the ability to take on other's perspective, to understand, feel, and possibly share and respond to their experience. There are more definitions of empathy that include but are not limited to social, cognitive, and emotional processes primarily concerned with understanding others. Often times, empathy is considered to be a broad term, and broken down into more specific concepts and types that include cognitive empathy, emotional empathy, somatic empathy, and spiritual empathy.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Self-awareness</span> Capacity for introspection and individuation as a subject

In philosophy of self, self-awareness is the experience of one's own personality or individuality. It is not to be confused with consciousness in the sense of qualia. While consciousness is being aware of one's body and environment, self-awareness is the recognition of that consciousness. Self-awareness is how an individual experiences and understands their own character, feelings, motives, and desires.

In psychology, theory of mind refers to the capacity to understand other people by ascribing mental states to them. A theory of mind includes the understanding that others' beliefs, desires, intentions, emotions, and thoughts may be different from one's own. Possessing a functional theory of mind is crucial for success in everyday human social interactions. People utilize a theory of mind when analyzing, judging, and inferring others' behaviors. The discovery and development of theory of mind primarily came from studies done with animals and infants. Factors including drug and alcohol consumption, language development, cognitive delays, age, and culture can affect a person's capacity to display theory of mind. Having a theory of mind is similar to but not identical with having the capacity for empathy or sympathy.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Bobo doll experiment</span> Psychology experiment

The Bobo doll experiment is the collective name for a series of experiments performed by psychologist Albert Bandura to test his social learning theory. Between 1961 and 1963, he studied children's behaviour after watching an adult model act aggressively towards a Bobo doll. The most notable variation of the experiment measured the children's behavior after seeing the adult model rewarded, punished, or experience no consequence for physically abusing the Bobo doll.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Echolalia</span> Speech disorder

Echolalia is the unsolicited repetition of vocalizations made by another person; when repeated by the same person, it is called palilalia. In its profound form it is automatic and effortless. It is one of the echophenomena, closely related to echopraxia, the automatic repetition of movements made by another person; both are "subsets of imitative behavior" whereby sounds or actions are imitated "without explicit awareness". Echolalia may be an immediate reaction to a stimulus or may be delayed.

A mirror neuron is a neuron that fires both when an animal acts and when the animal observes the same action performed by another. Thus, the neuron "mirrors" the behavior of the other, as though the observer were itself acting. Mirror neurons are not always physiologically distinct from other types of neurons in the brain; their main differentiating factor is their response patterns. By this definition, such neurons have been directly observed in humans and primate species, and in birds.

Echopraxia is the involuntary repetition or imitation of another person's actions. Similar to echolalia, the involuntary repetition of sounds and language, it is one of the echophenomena. It has long been recognized as a core feature of Tourette syndrome, and is considered a complex tic, but it also occurs in autism spectrum disorders, schizophrenia and catatonia, aphasia, and disorders involving the startle reflex such as latah. Echopraxia has also been observed in individuals with epilepsy, dementia and autoimmune disorders; the causes of and the link between echopraxia and these disorders is undetermined.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Mu wave</span> Electrical activity in the part of the brain controlling voluntary movement

The sensorimotor mu rhythm, also known as mu wave, comb or wicket rhythms or arciform rhythms, are synchronized patterns of electrical activity involving large numbers of neurons, probably of the pyramidal type, in the part of the brain that controls voluntary movement. These patterns as measured by electroencephalography (EEG), magnetoencephalography (MEG), or electrocorticography (ECoG), repeat at a frequency of 7.5–12.5 Hz, and are most prominent when the body is physically at rest. Unlike the alpha wave, which occurs at a similar frequency over the resting visual cortex at the back of the scalp, the mu rhythm is found over the motor cortex, in a band approximately from ear to ear. People suppress mu rhythms when they perform motor actions or, with practice, when they visualize performing motor actions. This suppression is called desynchronization of the wave because EEG wave forms are caused by large numbers of neurons firing in synchrony. The mu rhythm is even suppressed when one observes another person performing a motor action or an abstract motion with biological characteristics. Researchers such as V. S. Ramachandran and colleagues have suggested that this is a sign that the mirror neuron system is involved in mu rhythm suppression, although others disagree.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Mirroring</span> Subconscious imitative behaviour

Mirroring is the behavior in which one person subconsciously imitates the gesture, speech pattern, or attitude of another. Mirroring often occurs in social situations, particularly in the company of close friends or family, often going unnoticed by both parties. The concept often affects other individuals' notions about the individual that is exhibiting mirroring behaviors, which can lead to the individual building rapport with others.

Typically researched in infants, intermodal mapping refers to the ability to gather information about a particular stimulus by integrating multiple senses. Researched by American psychologists Andrew N. Meltzoff and M. Keith Moore, this capability plays an underlying part in neonatal imitation.

Modeling is:

  1. a method used in certain cognitive-behavioral techniques of psychotherapy whereby the client learns by imitation alone, copying a human model without any specific verbal direction by the therapist, and
  2. a general process in which persons serve as models for others, exhibiting the behavior to be imitated by others. This process is most commonly discussed for children.
<span class="mw-page-title-main">Joint attention</span> When two people focus on something at once

Joint attention or shared attention is the shared focus of two individuals on an object. It is achieved when one individual alerts another to an object by means of eye-gazing, pointing or other verbal or non-verbal indications. An individual gazes at another individual, points to an object and then returns their gaze to the individual. Scaife and Bruner were the first researchers to present a cross-sectional description of children's ability to follow eye gaze in 1975. They found that most eight- to ten-month-old children followed a line of regard, and that all 11- to 14-month-old children did so. This early research showed it was possible for an adult to bring certain objects in the environment to an infant's attention using eye gaze.

Associative sequence learning (ASL) is a neuroscientific theory that attempts to explain how mirror neurons are able to match observed and performed actions, and how individuals are able to imitate body movements. The theory was proposed by Cecilia Heyes in 2000.. A conceptually similar model proposed by Christian Keysers and David Perrett, based on what we know about the neural properties of mirror neurons and spike-timing-dependent plasticity is the Hebbian learning account of mirror neurons.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Speech repetition</span> Repeating something someone else said

Speech repetition occurs when individuals speak the sounds that they have heard another person pronounce or say. In other words, it is the saying by one individual of the spoken vocalizations made by another individual. Speech repetition requires the person repeating the utterance to have the ability to map the sounds that they hear from the other person's oral pronunciation to similar places and manners of articulation in their own vocal tract.

The study of memory incorporates research methodologies from neuropsychology, human development and animal testing using a wide range of species. The complex phenomenon of memory is explored by combining evidence from many areas of research. New technologies, experimental methods and animal experimentation have led to an increased understanding of the workings of memory.

Cultural group selection is an explanatory model within cultural evolution of how cultural traits evolve according to the competitive advantage they bestow upon a group. This multidisciplinary approach to the question of human culture engages research from the fields of anthropology, behavioural economics, evolutionary biology, evolutionary game theory, sociology, and psychology.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Cecilia Heyes</span> British psychologist (born 1960)

Cecilia Heyes is a British psychologist who studies the evolution of the human mind. She is a Senior Research Fellow in Theoretical Life Sciences at All Souls College, and a Professor of Psychology at the University of Oxford. She is also a Fellow of the British Academy, and President of the Experimental Psychology Society.

Imitative learning is a type of social learning whereby new behaviors are acquired via imitation. Imitation aids in communication, social interaction, and the ability to modulate one's emotions to account for the emotions of others, and is "essential for healthy sensorimotor development and social functioning". The ability to match one's actions to those observed in others occurs in humans and animals; imitative learning plays an important role in humans in cultural development. Imitative learning is different from observational learning in that it requires a duplication of the behaviour exhibited by the model, whereas observational learning can occur when the learner observes an unwanted behaviour and its subsequent consequences and as a result learns to avoid that behaviour.

Social learning refers to learning that is facilitated by observation of, or interaction with, another animal or its products. Social learning has been observed in a variety of animal taxa, such as insects, fish, birds, reptiles, amphibians and mammals.

References

  1. imitation, Online etymology dictionary
  2. 1 2 3 Hopper, Lydia M. (2010). "Deferred imitation in children and apes". Psychologist. 23 (4): 294–7. Archived from the original on August 3, 2014. Retrieved March 8, 2021.
  3. Moss, Ellen; Strayer, F. F. (1988). "Imitation is the Greatest form of Flattery". PsycCRITIQUES. 33 (11): 970–971. doi:10.1037/026218.
  4. Chartrand, T.; Bargh, J. (1999). "The Chameleon Effect: The Perception-Behavior Link and Social Interaction" (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 76 (6). New York University: 893–910. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.76.6.893. PMID   10402679. S2CID   11818459 . Retrieved September 28, 2014.
  5. George Herbert Mead, Mind, Self, and Society; Kenneth Kaye, The Mental and Social Life of Babies , U. Chicago Press 1982[ page needed ]
  6. Susan Blackmore, The Meme Machine, Oxford U. Press 2007[ page needed ]
  7. Jared Diamond, The Third Chimpanzee , HarperCollins 1992.[ page needed ]
  8. In M. Bullowa, ed. Before Speech: The beginning of interpersonal communication, Cambridge U. Press 1979[ page needed ]
  9. Kaye, Kenneth; Marcus, Janet (1981). "Infant imitation: The sensory-motor agenda". Developmental Psychology. 17 (3): 258. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.17.3.258.
  10. Rendell, Luke; Fogarty, Laurel; Laland, Kevin N. (2010). "Rogers' Paradox Recast and Resolved: Population Structure and the Evolution of Social Learning Strategies". Evolution. 64 (2): 534–548. doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2009.00817.x. ISSN   1558-5646. PMID   19674093. S2CID   2898779.
  11. Laland, Kevin N.; Williams, Kerry (1998). "Social transmission of maladaptive information in the guppy". Behavioral Ecology. 9 (5): 493–499. doi: 10.1093/beheco/9.5.493 . ISSN   1045-2249.
  12. Johnstone, R. A.; Dall, S. R. X.; Giraldeau, Luc–Alain; Valone, Thomas J.; Templeton, Jennifer J. (November 29, 2002). "Potential disadvantages of using socially acquired information". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences. 357 (1427): 1559–1566. doi:10.1098/rstb.2002.1065. PMC   1693065 . PMID   12495513.
  13. Rogers, EM (2010). "Diffusion of innovations". Simon and Schuster.
  14. Castellano, Claudio; Fortunato, Santo; Loreto, Vittorio (May 11, 2009). "Statistical physics of social dynamics". Reviews of Modern Physics. 81 (2): 591–646. arXiv: 0710.3256 . Bibcode:2009RvMP...81..591C. doi:10.1103/RevModPhys.81.591. S2CID   118376889.
  15. Bonini, Luca; Ferrari, Pier Francesco (2011). "Evolution of mirror systems: a simple mechanism for complex cognitive functions". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1225 (1): 166–175. Bibcode:2011NYASA1225..166B. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06002.x. PMC   3113469 . PMID   21535003.
  16. Iacoboni, M.; Woods, RP; Brass, M; Bekkering, H; Mazziotta, JC; Rizzolatti, G (1999). "Cortical Mechanisms of Human Imitation". Science. 286 (5449): 2526–8. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.555.8075 . doi:10.1126/science.286.5449.2526. PMID   10617472.
  17. Rizzolatti, Giacomo; Craighero, Laila (2004). "The Mirror-Neuron System". Annual Review of Neuroscience. 27: 169–92. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230. PMID   15217330. S2CID   1729870.
  18. Ramachandran, V.S. "Mirror Neurons and imitation learning as the driving force behind 'the great leap forward' in human evolution". Edge Foundation.
  19. Dinstein, Ilan; Thomas, Cibu; Behrmann, Marlene; Heeger, David J. (2008). "A mirror up to nature". Current Biology. 18 (1): R13–8. Bibcode:2008CBio...18..R13D. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2007.11.004. PMC   2517574 . PMID   18177704.
  20. Krützen, Michael; Mann, Janet; Heithaus, Michael R.; Connor, Richard C.; Bejder, Lars; Sherwin, William B. (2005). "Cultural transmission of tool use in bottlenose dolphins". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 102 (25): 8939–43. Bibcode:2005PNAS..102.8939K. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0500232102 . PMC   1157020 . PMID   15947077.
  21. "Wild dolphins teaches others tail walking tricks". WDCS, the Whale and Dolphin Conservation Society. Archived from the original on January 14, 2009.
  22. Boesch, Christophe; Tomasello, Michael (December 1998). "Chimpanzee and Human Cultures" (PDF). Current Anthropology. 39 (5): 591–614. doi:10.1086/204785. S2CID   55562574. Archived from the original (PDF) on February 26, 2019.
  23. 1 2 Iacoboni, Marco; Dapretto, Mirella (2006). "The mirror neuron system and the consequences of its dysfunction". Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 7 (12): 942–51. doi:10.1038/nrn2024. PMID   17115076. S2CID   9463011.
  24. Geiger, Alexander; Bente, Gary; Lammers, Sebastian; Tepest, Ralf; Roth, Daniel; Bzdok, Danilo; Vogeley, Kai (November 15, 2019). "Distinct functional roles of the mirror neuron system and the mentalizing system". NeuroImage. 202: 116102. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116102. ISSN   1053-8119. PMID   31446128. S2CID   201125892.
  25. Zentall, Thomas R. (2006). "Imitation: Definitions, evidence, and mechanisms". Animal Cognition. 9 (4): 335–53. doi:10.1007/s10071-006-0039-2. PMID   17024510. S2CID   16183221.
  26. Mead, George Herbert (2015) [1934]. "PART II: MIND 8. Imitation and the Origin of Language". In Morris, Charles W.; Huebner, Daniel R.; Joas, Hans (eds.). Mind, Self & Society. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. pp. 58–59. doi:10.7208/chicago/9780226112879.001.0001. ISBN   978-0-226-11273-2. [T]he 'herding' instinct, if reduced down to something concrete in the action of the form itself...this may lead to a stampede in the herd. Something of that sort is involved in the so-called 'sentinel.' One animal, a little more sensitive than the others, lifts his head and starts to run away, and the other animals do tend to move with the sentinel form...[Y]ou [humans] unconsciously imitate [dialects]. The same thing is also true of various other [primate] mannerisms...That is what we call 'imitation,' and what is curious is that there is practically no indication of such behavior on the part of lower [life]forms. You can teach a sparrow to sing as a canary but you have to keep that sparrow constantly listening to a canary. It does not take place readily...in general the taking over of the processes of others is not natural to lower forms. Imitation seems to belong to the human form, where it has reached some sort of independent conscious existence.
  27. Thorndike, Edward L. (1898). "Animal intelligence: An experimental study of the associative processes in animals". The Psychological Review: Monograph Supplements. 2 (4): i–109. doi:10.1037/h0092987. hdl:2027/njp.32101045694773. S2CID   143644299.
  28. Heyes, Cecilia M.; Galef, Bennett G., eds. (1996). Social Learning in Animals: The Roots of Culture. San Diego: Academic Press. ISBN   978-0-08-054131-0.[ page needed ]
  29. Thorpe, William Homan (1963). Learning and Instinct in Animals . London: Methuen. OCLC   225309.[ page needed ]
  30. Hayes, Keith J.; Hayes, Catherine (1952). "Imitation in a home-raised chimpanzee". Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 45 (5): 450–9. doi:10.1037/h0053609. PMID   13000013.
  31. Custance, Deborah M.; Whiten, Andrew; Bard, Kim A. (1995). "Can Young Chimpanzees (Pan Troglodytes) Imitate Arbitrary Actions? Hayes & Hayes (1952) Revisited". Behaviour. 132 (11): 837–59. doi:10.1163/156853995X00036. JSTOR   4535304.
  32. Voelkl, Bernhard; Huber, Ludwig (2007). Ferrari, Pier (ed.). "Imitation as Faithful Copying of a Novel Technique in Marmoset Monkeys". PLOS ONE. 2 (7): e611. Bibcode:2007PLoSO...2..611V. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0000611 . PMC   1905941 . PMID   17622356. Open Access logo PLoS transparent.svg
  33. Heyes, C. M.; Dawson, G. R.; Nokes, T. (1992). "Imitation in rats: Initial responding and transfer evidence". The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 45 (3): 229–40. doi:10.1080/14640749208401018 (inactive November 1, 2024).{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: DOI inactive as of November 2024 (link)
  34. Heyes, C. M.; Dawson, G. R. (1990). "A demonstration of observational learning in rats using a bidirectional control". The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 42 (1): 59–71. doi:10.1080/14640749008401871 (inactive November 1, 2024). PMID   2326494.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: DOI inactive as of November 2024 (link)
  35. Heyes, C.M.; Ray, E.D.; Mitchell, C.J.; Nokes, T. (2000). "Stimulus Enhancement: Controls for Social Facilitation and Local Enhancement". Learning and Motivation. 31 (2): 83–98. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.520.2135 . doi:10.1006/lmot.1999.1041.
  36. Custance, Deborah; Whiten, Andrew; Fredman, Tamar (1999). "Social learning of an artificial fruit task in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella)". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 113: 13–23. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.113.1.13.
  37. Bugnyar, Thomas; Huber, Ludwig (1997). "Push or pull: An experimental study on imitation in marmosets". Animal Behaviour. 54 (4): 817–31. doi:10.1006/anbe.1996.0497. PMID   9344436. S2CID   40005662.
  38. Voelkl, Bernhard; Huber, Ludwig (2000). "True imitation in marmosets". Animal Behaviour. 60 (2): 195–202. doi:10.1006/anbe.2000.1457. PMID   10973721. S2CID   31623649.
  39. Whiten, Andrew; Custance, Deborah M.; Gomez, Juan-Carlos; Teixidor, Patricia; Bard, Kim A. (1996). "Imitative learning of artificial fruit processing in children (Homo sapiens) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 110 (1): 3–14. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.110.1.3. PMID   8851548.
  40. Stoinski, Tara S.; Wrate, Joanna L.; Ure, Nicky; Whiten, Andrew (2001). "Imitative learning by captive western lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) in a simulated food-processing task". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 115 (3): 272–81. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.115.3.272. PMID   11594496.
  41. Whiten, Andrew; Horner, Victoria; Litchfield, Carla A.; Marshall-Pescini, Sarah (2004). "How do apes ape?". Animal Learning & Behavior. 32 (1): 36–52. doi: 10.3758/BF03196005 . PMID   15161139.
  42. Tennie, Claudio; Call, Josep; Tomasello, Michael (2006). "Push or Pull: Imitation vs. Emulation in Great Apes and Human Children". Ethology. 112 (12): 1159–69. Bibcode:2006Ethol.112.1159T. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.2006.01269.x. S2CID   56228726.
  43. Zimmer, Carl (December 13, 2005). "Children Learn by Monkey See, Monkey Do. Chimps Don't". The New York Times.
  44. 1 2 Howard, Maureen; Keenen, Michael (Spring 1993). "Outline for a Functional Analysis of Imitation in Animals". The Physiological Record. 43 (2): 185 via Business Source Complete.
  45. 1 2 3 4 5 Zentall, Thomas R. (2001). "Imitation in Animals: Evidence, Function, and Mechanisms". Cybernetics and Systems. 32 (1–2): 53–96. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.459.375 . doi:10.1080/019697201300001812. S2CID   30219876.
  46. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Zentall, Thomas R. (2003). "Imitation by animals: How do they do it?". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 12 (3): 91–5. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.01237. JSTOR   20182847. S2CID   145596513.
  47. 1 2 3 Heyes, Cecilia M.; Ray, Elizabeth D. (2000). "What is the Significance of Imitation in Animals?". In Slater, Peter J.B.; Rosenblatt, Jay S.; Snowdon, Charles T.; Roper, Timothy J. (eds.). Advances in the Study of Behavior. Vol. 29. pp. 215–45. doi:10.1016/S0065-3454(08)60106-0. ISBN   978-0-12-004529-7.
  48. Bandura, A. Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Prentice-Hall, 1986.
  49. Heyes, C. "Transformational and Associative Theories of Imitation". MIT Press, 2002, p. 504-505.
  50. 1 2 3 Galef, Bennett G. (1998). "Recent progress in studies of imitation and social learning in animals". In Sabourin, Michel; Craik, Fergus; Robert, Michèle (eds.). Advances in psychological science, Vol. 2: Biological and cognitive aspects. Hove, England: Psychology Press. pp. 275–99. ISBN   978-0-86377-471-3.
  51. 1 2 Carlson, Neil R.; Buskist, William; Enzle, Michael E.; Heth, C Donald (2005). Psychology: The Science of Behaviour (3rd ed.). Pearson Education Canada. p. 384. ISBN   978-0-205-40386-8.
  52. 1 2 3 4 5 Ingersoll, Brooke (2008). "The Social Role of Imitation in Autism". Infants & Young Children. 21 (2): 107–19. doi: 10.1097/01.IYC.0000314482.24087.14 . S2CID   10001799.
  53. 1 2 3 McLeod, S. A. (2011). "Bandura: Social learning theory". Simply Psychology. Retrieved November 10, 2012.
  54. 1 2 Tomasello, Michael (2006). "Rational Imitation in 12-Month-Old Infants Christiane Schwier, Catharine van Maanen, Malinda Carpenter". Infancy. 10 (3): 303–11. doi:10.1207/s15327078in1003_6. S2CID   41793867.
  55. Meltzoff, A. N. & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198(4312), 75-78. DOI: 10.1126/science.198.4312.75
  56. Field, T.; Woodson, R; Greenberg, R; Cohen, D (October 8, 1982). "Discrimination and imitation of facial expression by neonates". Science. 218 (4568): 179–181. Bibcode:1982Sci...218..179F. doi:10.1126/science.7123230. ISSN   0036-8075. PMID   7123230.
  57. 1 2 "Newborns found able to imitate facial expressions". The New York Times. October 12, 1982. Retrieved November 14, 2012.
  58. Oostenbroek, Janine; Suddendorf, Thomas; Nielsen, Mark; Redshaw, Jonathan; Kennedy-Costantini, Siobhan; Davis, Jacqueline; Clark, Sally; Slaughter, Virginia (May 2016). "Comprehensive Longitudinal Study Challenges the Existence of Neonatal Imitation in Humans". Current Biology. 26 (10): 1334–1338. Bibcode:2016CBio...26.1334O. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.03.047 . PMID   27161497. S2CID   3918288.
  59. "Foundation: Imitation: California infant/toddler learning & development foundations". California Department of Education. 2012. Retrieved November 10, 2012.
  60. Stiefel, Chana (2012). "What Your Child Learns By Imitating You". Parents Magazine.
  61. Malcom, Kelly (June 30, 2000). "Studies shed light on toddler development".
  62. Kaye, Kenneth (1982). "Imitation". The Mental and Social Life of Babies. University of Chicago Press. pp.  155–88. ISBN   978-0-226-42848-2.
  63. Toyokawa, Wataru; Saito, Yoshimatsu; Kameda, Tatsuya (May 1, 2017). "Individual differences in learning behaviours in humans: Asocial exploration tendency does not predict reliance on social learning". Evolution and Human Behavior. 38 (3): 325–333. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.11.001. ISSN   1090-5138.
  64. Patrick, Karen; Richman, Charles L. (1985). "Imitation in Toddlers as a Function of Motor and Verbal Aspects of Modeling". The Journal of Genetic Psychology. 146 (4): 507–18. doi:10.1080/00221325.1985.10532470. PMID   3835229.
  65. Cherry, Kendra. "Bobo Doll Experiment". About.com. Archived from the original on March 5, 2016. Retrieved November 20, 2012.
  66. Hay, Dennis C.; Young, Andrew W.; Ellis, Andrew W. (November 1991). "Routes through the Face Recognition System". The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A. 43 (4): 761–791. doi:10.1080/14640749108400957. ISSN   0272-4987. PMID   1775665. S2CID   36149305.
  67. Subiaul, Francys; Lurie, Herbert; Romansky, Kathryn; Klein, Tovah; Holmes, David; Terrace, Herbert (2007). "Cognitive imitation in typically-developing 3- and 4-year olds and individuals with autism". Cognitive Development. 22 (2): 230. Bibcode:1996CogD...11..491S. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2006.10.003. PMC   2818295 . PMID   20148122.
  68. Rogers, Sally J.; Hepburn, Susan L.; Stackhouse, Tracy; Wehner, Elizabeth (2003). "Imitation performance in toddlers with autism and those with other developmental disorders". Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 44 (5): 763–81. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00162. PMID   12831120. S2CID   17936792.
  69. Crompton, Catherine J.; Sharp, Martha; Axbey, Harriet; Fletcher-Watson, Sue; Flynn, Emma G.; Ropar, Danielle (2020). "Neurotype-Matching, but Not Being Autistic, Influences Self and Observer Ratings of Interpersonal Rapport". Frontiers in Psychology. 11: 2961. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.586171 . ISSN   1664-1078. PMC   7645034 . PMID   33192918.
  70. Milton, Damian E.M. (October 1, 2012). "On the ontological status of autism: the 'double empathy problem'". Disability & Society. 27 (6): 883–887. doi:10.1080/09687599.2012.710008. ISSN   0968-7599. S2CID   54047060.
  71. DeThorne, Laura S. (March 1, 2020). "Revealing the Double Empathy Problem". The ASHA Leader. 25 (3): 58–65. doi: 10.1044/leader.ftr2.25042020.58 . S2CID   216359201.
  72. Soulières, Isabelle; Dawson, Michelle; Samson, Fabienne; Barbeau, Elise B.; Sahyoun, Chérif P.; Strangman, Gary E.; Zeffiro, Thomas A.; Mottron, Laurent (2009). "Enhanced visual processing contributes to matrix reasoning in autism". Human Brain Mapping. 30 (12): 4082–4107. doi:10.1002/hbm.20831. PMC   2787806 . PMID   19530215.
  73. Plaisted, Kate; O'Riordan, Michelle; Baron-Cohen, Simon (July 1998). "Enhanced Discrimination of Novel, Highly Similar Stimuli by Adults with Autism During a Perceptual Learning Task". Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 39 (5): 765–775. doi:10.1017/s0021963098002601 (inactive November 3, 2024). ISSN   0021-9630. PMID   9690939.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: DOI inactive as of November 2024 (link)
  74. O'Riordan, Michelle; Plaisted, Kate (November 1, 2001). "Enhanced discrimination in autism". The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A. 54 (4): 961–979. doi:10.1080/713756000. ISSN   0272-4987. PMID   11765744. S2CID   144120552.
  75. Baron-Cohen, Simon; Belmonte, Matthew K. (July 21, 2005). "AUTISM: A Window Onto the Development of the Social and the Analytic Brain". Annual Review of Neuroscience. 28 (1): 109–126. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144137. ISSN   0147-006X. PMID   16033325.
  76. Schulte-Rüther, Martin; Otte, Ellen; Adigüzel, Kübra; Firk, Christine; Herpertz-Dahlmann, Beate; Koch, Iring; Konrad, Kerstin (June 28, 2016). "Intact mirror mechanisms for automatic facial emotions in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder: Intact mirror mechanisms in Austism". Autism Research. 10 (2): 298–310. doi:10.1002/aur.1654. PMID   27349835. S2CID   4924514.
  77. Heyes, Cecilia (2011). "Automatic imitation". Psychological Bulletin. 137 (3): 463–83. doi:10.1037/a0022288. PMID   21280938. S2CID   6975248.
  78. Lyons, Derek E.; Keil, Frank C. (April 23, 2013). Navigating the Social World. Oxford University Press. pp. 145–149. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199890712.003.0026. ISBN   9780199890712.
  79. Lyons, Derek E.; Young, Andrew G.; Keil, Frank C. (December 11, 2007). "The hidden structure of overimitation". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 104 (50): 19751–19756. Bibcode:2007PNAS..10419751L. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0704452104 . ISSN   0027-8424. PMC   2148370 . PMID   18056814.
  80. Bower, Bruce (May 15, 2018). "Kids are selective imitators, not extreme copycats". Science News. Retrieved May 18, 2018.
  81. Evans, Cara L.; Laland, Kevin N.; Carpenter, Malinda; Kendal, Rachel L. (December 17, 2017). "Selective copying of the majority suggests children are broadly "optimal-" rather than "over-" imitators" (PDF). Developmental Science. 6 (1): e12637. doi:10.1111/desc.12637. hdl:10023/16702. ISSN   1363-755X. PMID   29250871.
  82. Koenig, M. A.; Clement, F.; Harris, P. L. (October 1, 2004). "Trust in Testimony: Children's Use of True and False Statements". Psychological Science. 15 (10): 694–698. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00742.x. ISSN   0956-7976. PMID   15447641. S2CID   2116025.
  83. Sundqvist, Annette; Nordqvist, Emelie; Koch, Felix-Sebastian; Heimann, Mikael (November 2016). "Early declarative memory predicts productive language: A longitudinal study of deferred imitation and communication at 9 and 16months". Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 151: 109–119. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2016.01.015 . PMID   26925719.

Further reading