Affixes |
---|
See also: |
In linguistics, reduplication is a morphological process in which the root or stem of a word, part of that or the whole word is repeated exactly or with a slight change.
The classic observation on the semantics of reduplication is Edward Sapir's: "Generally employed, with self-evident symbolism, to indicate such concepts as distribution, plurality, repetition, customary activity, increase of size, added intensity, continuance." [1] It is used in inflections to convey a grammatical function, such as plurality or intensification, and in lexical derivation to create new words. It is often used when a speaker adopts a tone more expressive or figurative than ordinary speech and is also often, but not exclusively, iconic in meaning. It is found in a wide range of languages and language groups, though its level of linguistic productivity varies. Examples can be found in language as old as Sumerian, where it was used in forming some color terms, e.g. babbar "white", kukku "black". [2]
Reduplication is the standard term for this phenomenon in the linguistics literature. Other occasional terms include cloning, doubling, duplication, repetition, and tautonym (when it is used in biological taxonomies, such as Bison bison ).
Reduplication is often described phonologically in one of two ways: either (1) as reduplicated segments (sequences of consonants/vowels) or (2) as reduplicated prosodic units (syllables or moras). In addition to phonological description, reduplication often needs to be described morphologically as a reduplication of linguistic constituents (i.e. words, stems, roots). As a result, reduplication is interesting theoretically as it involves the interface between phonology and morphology.
The base is the word (or part of the word) that is to be copied. The reduplicated element is called the reduplicant, often abbreviated as RED or sometimes just R.
In reduplication, the reduplicant is most often repeated only once. In some languages, it can occur more than once, resulting in a tripled form, and not a duple as in most reduplication. Triplication is the term for this phenomenon of copying two times. [3] Pingelapese has both forms. [4] In this article, English translations of words are shown in apostrophes:
Basic verb | Reduplication | Triplication |
---|---|---|
kɔul 'to sing' | kɔukɔul 'singing' | kɔukɔukɔul 'still singing' |
mejr 'to sleep' | mejmejr 'sleeping' | mejmejmejr 'still sleeping' |
Triplication occurs in other languages, e.g. Ewe, Shipibo, Twi, Mokilese, Min Nan (Hokkien), Stau. [3]
Sometimes gemination (i.e. the doubling of consonants or vowels) is considered to be a form of reduplication. The term dupleme has been used (after morpheme) to refer to different types of reduplication that have the same meaning.
Full reduplication involves a reduplication of the entire word. For example, Kham derives reciprocal forms from reflexive forms by total reduplication:
[ɡin] | 'ourselves' | → | [ɡinɡin] | 'we (to) us' | (ɡin-ɡin) | |||
[jaː] | 'themselves' | → | [jaːjaː] | 'they (to) them' | (jaː-jaː) | (Watters 2002) |
Another example is from Musqueam Halkomelem "dispositional" aspect formation:
[kʼʷə́ɬ] | 'to capsize' | → | [kʼʷə́ɬkʼʷəɬ] | 'likely to capsize' | (kʼʷə́ɬ-kʼʷəɬ) | |||
[qʷél] | 'to speak' | → | [qʷélqʷel] | 'talkative' | (qʷél-qʷel) | (Shaw 2004) |
Partial reduplication involves a reduplication of only part of the word. For example, Marshallese forms words meaning 'to wear X' by reduplicating the last consonant-vowel-consonant (CVC) sequence of a base, i.e. base+CVC:
kagir | 'belt' | → | kagirgir | 'to wear a belt' | (kagir-gir) | |||
takin | 'sock' | → | takinkin | 'to wear socks' | (takin-kin) | (Moravsik 1978) |
Many languages often use both full and partial reduplication, as in the Motu example below:
Base verb | Full reduplication | Partial reduplication |
---|---|---|
mahuta 'to sleep' | mahutamahuta 'to sleep constantly' | mamahuta 'to sleep (plural)' |
(mahuta-mahuta) | (ma-mahuta) |
Reduplication may be initial (i.e. prefixal), final (i.e. suffixal), or internal (i.e. infixal), e.g.
Initial reduplication in Agta (CV- prefix):
[ɸuɾab] | 'afternoon' | → | [ɸuɸuɾab] | 'late afternoon' | (ɸu-ɸuɾab) | |||
[ŋaŋaj] | 'a long time' | → | [ŋaŋaŋaj] | 'a long time (in years)' | (ŋa-ŋaŋaj) | (Healey 1960) |
Final reduplication in Dakota (-CCV suffix):
[hãska] | 'tall (singular)' | → | [hãskaska] | 'tall (plural)' | (hãska-ska) | |||
[waʃte] | 'good (singular)' | → | [waʃteʃte] | 'good (plural)' | (waʃte-ʃte) | (Shaw 1980, Marantz 1982, Albright 2002) |
Internal reduplication in Samoan (-CV- infix):
savali | 'he/she walks' (singular) | → | savavali | 'they walk' (plural) | (sa-va-vali) | |||
alofa | 'he/she loves' (singular) | → | alolofa | 'they love' (plural) | (a-lo-lofa) | (Moravcsik 1978, Broselow and McCarthy 1984) | ||
le tamaloa | 'the man' (singular) [5] | → | tamaloloa | 'men' (plural) | (tama-lo-loa) |
Internal reduplication is much less common than the initial and final types.
A reduplicant can copy from either the left edge of a word (left-to-right copying) or from the right edge (right-to-left copying). There is a tendency for prefixing reduplicants to copy left-to-right and for suffixing reduplicants to copy right-to-left:
Initial L → R copying in Oykangand Kunjen (a Pama–Nyungan language of Australia):
[eder] | → | [ededer] | 'rain' | (ed-eder) | ||
[alɡal] | → | [alɡalɡal] | 'straight' | (alg-algal) |
Final R → L copying in Sirionó:
achisia | → | achisiasia | 'I cut' | (achisia-sia) | |||
ñimbuchao | → | ñimbuchaochao | 'to come apart' | (ñimbuchao-chao) | (McCarthy and Prince 1996) |
Copying from the other direction is possible although less common:
Initial R → L copying in Tillamook:
[ɡaɬ] | 'eye' | → | [ɬɡaɬ] | 'eyes' | (ɬ-ɡaɬ) | |||
[təq] | 'break' | → | [qtəq] | 'they break' | (q-təq) | (Reichard 1959) |
Final L → R copying in Chukchi:
nute- | 'ground' | → | nutenut | 'ground (abs. sg.)' | (nute-nut) | |||
jilʔe- | 'gopher' | → | jilʔejil | 'gopher (abs. sg.)' | (jilʔe-jil) | (Marantz 1982) |
Internal reduplication can also involve copying the beginning or end of the base. In Quileute, the first consonant of the base is copied and inserted after the first vowel of the base.
Internal L → R copying in Quileute:
[tsiko] | 'he put it on' | → | [tsitsko] | 'he put it on (frequentative)' | (tsi-ts-ko) | |||
[tukoːjoʔ] | 'snow' | → | [tutkoːjoʔ] | 'snow here and there' | (tu-t-ko:jo’) | (Broselow and McCarthy 1984) |
In Temiar, the last consonant of the root is copied and inserted before the medial consonant of the root.
Internal R → L copying in Temiar (an Austroasiatic language of Malaysia):
[sluh] | 'to shoot (perfective)' | → | [shluh] | 'to shoot (continuative)' | (s-h-luh) | |||
[slɔɡ] | 'to marry (perfective)' | → | [sɡlɔɡ] | 'to marry (continuative)' | (s-ɡ-lɔɡ) | (Broselow and McCarthy 1984, Walther 2000) |
A rare type of reduplication is found in Semai (an Austroasiatic language of Malaysia). "Expressive minor reduplication" is formed with an initial reduplicant that copies the first and last segment of the base:
[kʉːʔ] | → | [kʔkʉːʔ] | 'to vomit' | (kʔ-kʉːʔ) | |||
[dŋɔh] | → | [dhdŋɔh] | 'appearance of nodding constantly' | (dh-dŋɔh) | |||
[cruhaːw] | → | [cwcruhaːw] | 'monsoon rain' | (cw-cruhaːw) | Diffloth 1973 |
All the examples above consist of only reduplication, which also often occurs with other phonological and morphological processes, such as vowel alternation, [6] deletion and affixation of non-reduplicating material.
For instance, in Tz'utujil a new '-ish' adjective form is derived from other words by suffixing the reduplicated first consonant of the base followed by the segment [oχ]. This can be written succinctly as -Coχ. Below are some examples:
Somali has a similar suffix that is used in forming the plural of some nouns: -aC (where C is the last consonant of the base):
This combination of reduplication and affixation is commonly referred to as fixed-segment reduplication.
In Tohono O'odham initial reduplication also involves gemination of the first consonant in the distributive plural and in repetitive verbs:
Sometimes gemination can be analyzed as a type of reduplication.[ citation needed ]
This section needs expansion. You can help by adding to it. (May 2008) |
This section needs expansion. You can help by adding to it. (December 2009) |
In the Malayo-Polynesian family, reduplication is used to form plurals (among many other functions):
In pre-1972 Indonesian and Malaysian orthography, 2 was shorthand for the reduplication that forms plurals: orang "person", orang-orang or orang2 "people". [8] This orthography has resurfaced widely in text messaging and other forms of electronic communication.
The Nama language uses reduplication to increase the force of a verb: go, "look;", go-go "examine with attention".
Chinese and Japanese do not make morphological use of reduplication, but some words are formed this way, often with a collective sense: 人rén "person", 人人rénrén "everybody"; 時toki "time", tokidoki時々 "sometimes, from time to time". The iteration mark 々 can be used to indicate reduplication, although in Chinese the iteration mark is no longer used in standard writing and is often found only in calligraphy.
Indo-European languages formerly used reduplication to form a number of verb forms, especially in the preterite or perfect. In the older Indo-European languages, many such verbs survive:
Those forms do not survive in Modern English but existed in its parent Germanic languages. Many verbs in the Indo-European languages exhibit reduplication in the present stem, rather than the perfect stem, often with a different vowel from that used for the perfect: Latin gigno, genui ("I beget, I begat") and Greek τίθημι, ἔθηκα, τέθηκα (I place, I placed, I have placed). Other Indo-European verbs used reduplication as a derivational process: compare Latin sto ("I stand") and sisto ("I remain"). All of those Indo-European inherited reduplicating forms are subject to reduction by other phonological laws.
Reduplication can be used to refer to the most prototypical instance of a word's meaning. In such a case, it is called contrastive focus reduplication. Finnish colloquial speech uses the process; nouns can be reduplicated to indicate genuinity, completeness, originality and being uncomplicated, as opposed to being fake, incomplete, complicated or fussy. It can be thought as compound word formation. For example, Söin jäätelöä ja karkkia, sekä tietysti ruokaruokaa. "I ate ice cream and candy, and of course food-food". Here, "food-food" is contrasted to "junk-food". One may say, "En ollut eilen koulussa, koska olin kipeä. Siis kipeäkipeä" ("I wasn't at school yesterday because I was sick. Sick-sick, that is"); that means that one was actually suffering from an illness instead of making up excuses, as usual.
Words can be reduplicated with their case morphemes, as in lomalla lomalla ("away, on vacation, on leave"), where the adessive morpheme --lla appears twice.
In Swiss German, the verbs gah or goh "go", cho "come", la or lo "let" and aafa or aafo "begin" reduplicate when they are combined with other verbs.
Si she chunt comes üse our Chrischtboum Christmas tree cho come schmücke. adorn She comes to adorn our Christmas tree. | Si she lat lets ne him nid not la let schlafe. sleep She doesn't let him sleep. |
In some Salishan languages, reduplication can mark both diminution and plurality, with one process being applied to each end of the word, as in the following example from Shuswap. Note that the transcription is not comparable to the IPA, but the reduplication of both initial and final portions of the root is clear: ṣōk!Emē'’n 'knife' reduplicated as ṣuk!ṣuk!Emen'’me’n 'plural small knives' (Haeberlin 1918:159). Reduplication has been found to be a major part of Salish languages. [9]
At 25–50 weeks after birth, typically developing infants go through a stage of reduplicated or canonical babbling (Stark 198, Oller, 1980). Canonical babbling is characterized by repetition of identical or nearly identical consonant-vowel combinations, such as nanana or idididi. It appears as a progression of language development as infants experiment with their vocal apparatus and home in on the sounds used in their native language. Canonical/reduplicated babbling also appears at a time when general rhythmic behavior, such as rhythmic hand movements and rhythmic kicking, appear. Canonical babbling is distinguished from earlier syllabic and vocal play, which has less structure.
The Proto-Indo-European language used partial reduplication of a consonant and e in many stative aspect verb forms. The perfect or preterite (past) tense of some Ancient Greek, [10] Gothic, Latin, Sanskrit, Old Irish, and Old Norse verbs preserve this reduplication:
Proto-Indo-European also used reduplication for the imperfective aspect. Ancient Greek preserves this reduplication in the present tense of some verbs. Usually, but not always, this is reduplication of a consonant and i, and contrasts with e-reduplication in the perfect: [11]
Reduplication in nouns was rare, the best example being Proto-Indo-European *kʷé-kʷl-os 'wheel' (cf. Lithuanian kãklas 'neck', Sanskrit cakrá 'wheel', Greek κύκλος (kýklos) 'circle'), which doubled *kʷel-o- (cf. Old Prussian kelan 'wheel', Welsh pêl 'ball'), [12] itself likely a deverbative of *kʷelh₁- 'to turn'.
English has several types of reduplication, ranging from informal expressive vocabulary (the first four forms below) to grammatically meaningful forms (the last two below). See also the alliteration section of the irreversible binomial article for cases like flip-flop and dribs and drabs.
Of the above types, only shm-reduplication is productive, meaning that examples of the first three are fixed forms and new forms are not easily accepted.
The double is—such as "What I want is, is to go home"—is in some cases a type of reduplication, which may be regarded as non-standard or incorrect.
More can be learned about English reduplication in Thun (1963), Cooper & Ross (1975), and Nevins & Vaux (2003).
While not common in Dutch, reduplication does exist. Most, but not all (e.g., pipi, blauwblauw (laten), taaitaai (gingerbread)) reduplications in Dutch are loanwords (e.g., koeskoes, bonbon, (ik hoorde het) via via) or imitative (e.g., tamtam, tomtom). [17] Another example is a former safe sex campaign slogan in Flanders: Eerst bla-bla, dan boem-boem (First talk, then have sex; lit. First blah-blah, then boom-boom). In Dutch the verb "gaan" (to go) can be used as an auxiliary verb, which can lead to a triplication: we gaan (eens) gaan gaan (we are going to get going). The use of gaan as an auxiliary verb with itself is considered incorrect, but is commonly used in Flanders. [18] Numerous examples of reduplication in Dutch (and other languages) are discussed by Daniëls (2000).
Afrikaans makes use of reduplication to emphasize the meaning of the word repeated and to denote a plural or event happening in more than one place. For example, krap means "to scratch one's self," while krap-krap-krap means "to scratch one's self vigorously", [19] whereas "dit het plek-plek gereën" means "it rained here and there". [20] Reduplication in Afrikaans has been described extensively in the literature – see for example Botha (1988), Van Huyssteen (2004) and Van Huyssteen & Wissing (2007). Further examples of this include: "koes" (to dodge) being reduplicated in the sentence "Piet hardloop koes-koes weg" (Piet is running away while constantly dodging / cringing); "sukkel" (to struggle) becoming "sukkel-sukkel" (making slow progress; struggling on); and "kierang" (to cheat) becoming "kierang-kierang" to indicate being cheated on repeatedly. [21]
In Italian reduplication was used both to create new words or word associations (tran-tran, via via, leccalecca) and to intensify the meaning (piano piano "very softly").[ citation needed ]
Common in Lingua Franca, particularly but not exclusively for onomatopoeic action descriptions: Spagnoli venir...boum boum...andar; Inglis venir...boum boum bezef...andar; Francés venir...tru tru tru...chapar. ("The Spaniards came, cannonaded, and left. The English came, cannonaded heavily, and left. The French came, trumpeted on bugles, and captured it.") [22]
Common uses for reduplication in French are the creation of hypocoristics for names, whereby Louise becomes Loulou, and Zinedine Zidane becomes Zizou; and in many nursery words, like dada 'horsie' (vs. cheval 'horse'), tati/tata 'auntie' (vs. tante 'aunt'), or tonton 'unkie' (vs. oncle 'uncle').
In Romanian and Catalan, reduplication is not uncommon and it has been used for both the creation of new words (including many from onomatopoeia) and expressions, for example,
In colloquial Mexican Spanish it is common to use reduplicated adverbs such as luego luego (then then) meaning "immediately", or casi casi (almost almost) which intensifies the meaning of 'almost'.
The reduplication in the Russian language serves for various kinds of intensifying of the meaning and exists in several forms: a hyphenated or repeated word (either exact or inflected reduplication), and forms similar to shm-reduplication. [23]
Reduplication is a common feature of Irish and includes the examples rírá, ruaille buaille both meaning "commotion" and fite fuaite meaning "intertwined". [24]
Typically all Indo-Aryan languages, like Hindi, Punjabi, Gujarati and Bengali use partial or echoic reduplication in some form or the other. It is usually used to sound casual, or in a suggestive manner. It is often used to mean etcetera. For example, in Hindi, chai-shai (chai means tea, while this phrase means tea or any other supplementary drink or tea along with snacks). Quite common in casual conversations are a few more examples like shopping-wopping, khana-wana. South Asian Indo Aryan languages are also rich in other forms of reduplication: morphological (expressives), lexical (distributives), and phrasal (aspectual).
मनात
manaa-t
mind-in
हूर हूर
hur-hur
longing
दाटून
daaT.un
choking
येते
yete
comes
'Yearning desire floods into my heart.' Marathi
Reduplication also occurs in the 3rd gaṇa (verb class) of the Sanskrit language: bibheti "he fears", bibharti "he bears", juhoti "he offers", dadāti, "he gives". Even though the general idea is to reduplicate the verb root as a prefix, several sandhi rules change the outcome.
There are a number of constructions in Hindi and Urdu that are constructed by reduplication. Nouns, adjectives, verbs, adverbs, pronouns, all have possibility of reduplications. [26] [27] [28]
(1) Reduplication of numbers | (2) Reduplication of pronouns |
---|---|
baccõ ko children.DAT ek-ek one-one.REDUP tɔfī toffee do. give.IMP give a toffee to each child, one toffee per child. | tumne you.ERG kyā-kyā what-what.REDUP dekhā? saw.MASC.PRF? what (all things) did you see? |
bacce-bacce ko child-child.DAT pacās-pacās fifty-fifty tɔfiyā̃ toffees milī̃. received.PRF.FEM.PL each and every child received 50 toffees each | jo-jo who-who.NOM āẽge will-come unhẽ them.DAT kɛhnā. say.IMP.FUT say to whoever will come (to all and every visitor) |
(3) Reduplication of nouns | (4) Reduplication of adjectives |
baccā-baccā child-child.NOM jāntā know.PTCP hai. be.3.PRES? (each and) every child knows. | ye this garm-garm hot-hot cāy tea piyo. drink.2.IMP drink this hot tea. (emphasis on hotness) |
cāy-śāy tea-tea.NOM ho jāye? happen.PRF.SG.SUBJ? shall we have a cup of tea? (emphasis on meeting over tea) | udhar tither/that way harī-harī green-green ghās grass hai. be.3.PRS there is (so much) green grass that way/over there. (emphasis on the quantity) |
(5) Reduplication of verbs | (6) Reduplication of adverbs |
khāte-khāte eat-eat.PTCP.IPFV mat not bolo. talk.2.IMP do not talk while eating. | kal-kal tomorrow-tomorrow.LOC mẽ happen.3.FUT.PRF hī ho jāyegā.
It'll be done before tomorrow ends. |
soye-soye sleep-sleep.PTCP.PRF mar gaye. die.PRF.MASC.PL he died while sleeping / he died in his sleep. | cillāyī shouted.PRF.SG.FEM zor-zor se. loud-loud.INST she shouted loudly. (emphasis on the loudness) |
In Armenian, reduplication follows the same classification as in Turkish (see below), namely emphatic reduplication, echo reduplication, [29] and doubling. Many appear as lexical entries in Armenian lexicographical sources.
In Turkish, there are three kinds of reduplication. [33]
Emphatic reduplication, also called intensification: A word can be reduplicated partially, such that an emphatic stem is created to be attached to the adjective. This is done by taking the first syllable of the adjective, dropping the syllable-final phoneme, and adding one of four interpolated consonants (p, s, m, r). For example, kırmızı (red) becomes kıpkırmızı (very red); mavi (blue) becomes masmavi (very blue); yeşil (green) becomes yemyeşil (very green), and temiz (clean) becomes tertemiz ("spotless"). The added consonant is unpredictable, grammatically speaking; phonological studies, such as Wedel (1999), shed light on the subject. [34]
Echo reduplication: similar to echo word in other languages, a word can be reduplicated while replacing the initial consonants (not being m, and possibly missing) with m. The meaning of the original word is broadened. For example, tabak means "plate(s)", and tabak mabak then means "plates, dishes and such". This can be applied to all kinds of words, as in yeşil meşil meaning "green, greenish, whatever". Although not used in formal written Turkish, it is a standard accepted construction.
Doubling: A word can be reduplicated totally, turning it into an adverb with a related meaning. For example, zaman zaman (time time) meaning "time to time" or "occasionally"; uzun uzun (long long) meaning "at length." This type is used also in formal Turkish, especially in literature. There are a lot of reduplications in this category which do not, if used as one word, have a place in the Turkish language's vocabulary but is used solely in this way. These words are called mimetic in linguistics. An example is 'şırıl şırıl' (used for the sound of a waterfall). They try to give sounds to not only audible but also non-audible phenomena. For example, 'mışıl mışıl' is used for sleeping soundly.
Reduplication is also used in Dravidian languages like Telugu for the same purpose. [35]
Reduplication is a common phenomenon in Bantu languages and is usually used to form a frequentive verb or for emphasis. [37] [38]
Popular names that have reduplication include
Semitic languages frequently reduplicate consonants, though often not the vowels that appear next to the consonants in some verb form. [39] This can take the shape of reduplicating the antepenultimate consonant (usually the second of three),[ clarification needed ] the last of two consonants, or the last two consonants. [40]
In Hebrew, reduplication is used in nouns, adjectives, adverbs and verbs for various reasons:
There are also numerous examples in the Torah, for example "אם שמוע תשמעו" which means "if you listen diligently".
In Amharic, verb roots can be reduplicated three different ways. These can result in verbs, nouns, or adjectives (which are often derived from verbs).
From the root sbr 'break', antepenultimate reduplication produces täsäbabbärä 'it was shattered' [42] and biconsonantal reduplication produces täsbäräbbärä 'it was shattered repeatedly' and səbərbari 'a shard, a shattered piece'. [43]
From the root kHb 'pile stones into a wall', since the second radical is not fully specified, what some call "hollow", the antepenultimate reduplication process reduplicates the k inserting the vowel a along with the consonant as a place holder for the hollow consonant, which is by some criteria antepenultimate, and produces akakabä 'pile stones repeatedly'. [44] [45]
A small number of native Japanese nouns have collective forms produced by reduplication (possibly with rendaku), such as 人々 hitobito "people" (h → b is rendaku) – these are written with the iteration mark "々" to indicate duplication. This formation is not productive and is limited to a small set of nouns. Similarly to Standard Chinese, the meaning is not that of a true plural, but collectives that refer to a large, given set of the same object; for example, the formal English equivalent of 人々 would be "people" (collective), rather than "persons" (plural individuals).
Japanese also contains a large number of mimetic words formed by reduplication of a syllable. These words include not only onomatopoeia, but also words intended to invoke non-auditory senses or psychological states, such as きらきら kirakira (sparkling or shining). By one count, approximately 43% of Japanese mimetic words are formed by full reduplication, [46] [47] and many others are formed by partial reduplication, as in がささ〜 ga-sa-sa- (rustling) [48] – compare English "a-ha-ha-ha".
Austronesian languages are known for their extensive use of reduplication in both nouns and verbs. [49]
In the Malay language, reduplication is a semi-productive process. It is used for expression of various grammatical functions (such as verbal aspect) and it is part in a number of complex morphological models. Simple reduplication of nouns and pronouns can express at least three meanings:
Reduplication of an adjective can express different things:
Reduplication of a verb can express various things:
Notice that in the first case, the nasalisation of the initial consonant (whereby /p/ becomes /m/) is repeated, while in the second case, it only applies in the repeated word.
The Māori language (New Zealand) uses reduplication in a number of ways. [50]
Reduplication can convey a simple plural meaning, for instance wahine "woman", waahine "women", tangata "person", taangata "people". Biggs calls this "infixed reduplication". It occurs in a small subset of "people" words in most Polynesian languages.
Reduplication can convey emphasis or repetition, for example mate "die", matemate "die in numbers"; and de-emphasis, for example wera "hot" and werawera "warm".
Reduplication can also extend the meaning of a word; for instance paki "pat" becomes papaki "slap or clap once" and pakipaki "applaud"; kimo "blink" becomes kikimo "close eyes firmly"
The Mortlockese language is a Micronesian language spoken primarily on the Mortlock Islands. In the Mortlockese language, reduplication is used to show a habitual or imperfective aspect. For example, /jææjæ/ means "to use something" while the word /jæjjææjæ/ means "to use something habitually or repeatedly". [51] Reduplication is also used in the Mortlockese Language to show extremity or extreme measures. One example of this can be seen in /ŋiimw alɛɛtɛj/ which means "hate him, her, or it". To mean "really hate him, her, or it," the phrase changes to /ŋii~mw al~mw alɛɛtɛj/. [51]
Pingelapese is a Micronesian language spoken on the Pingelap atoll and on two of the eastern Caroline Islands, called the high island of Pohnpei. Pingelapese utilizes both duplication and triplication of a verb or part of a verb to express that something is happening for certain duration of time. No reduplication means that something happens. A reduplicated verb means that something IS happening, and a triplication means that something is STILL happening. For example, saeng means 'to cry' in Pingelapese. When reduplicated and triplicated, the duration of this verb is changed:
Few languages employ triplication in their language. In Micronesia, Pingelapese is one of only two languages that uses triplication, the other being Mokilese. Reduplication and triplication are not to be confused with tense. In order to make a phrase past, present, or future tense, a temporal phrase must be used. [52]
Rapa is the French Polynesian language of the island of Rapa Iti. [53] In terms of reduplication, the indigenous language known as Old Rapa uses reduplication consistent to other Polynesian languages. Reduplication of Old Rapa occurs in four ways: full, rightward, leftward, and medial. Full and rightward are generally more frequently used as opposed to the leftward and medial. Leftward and medial only occur as CV reduplication and partial leftward and medial usually denote emphasis. [53]
Example of reduplication forms: [53]
Base form | Reduplicated form | |
---|---|---|
Full reduplication |
|
|
Rightward reduplication |
|
|
Leftward reduplication |
|
|
Medial reduplication |
|
|
For the Rapa Language the implementation of reduplication has specific implications. The most evident of these are known as iterative, intensification, specification, diminutive, metaphorical, nominalizing, and adjectival. [53]
Iterative:
Intensification:
Specification:
Diminutive:
Metaphorical (typically comparing an animal action with a human action): [53]
Nominalizing:
Adjectival:
Philippine languages are characterized as having the most productive use of reduplication, especially in Tagalog (the basis of the Filipino language). Reduplication in Tagalog is complex. It can be roughly divided into six types: [54] [55] [56]
They can further be divided into "non-significant" (where its significance is not apparent) and "significant" reduplication. 1, 2, and 3 are always non-significant; while 5 and 6 are always significant. 4 can be non-significant when used for nouns (e.g. lalaki, "man"). [54] [55] [56]
Full or partial reduplication among nouns and pronouns can indicate emphasis, intensity, plurality, or causation; as well as a diminutive, superlative, iterative, restrictive, or distributive force. [54] [55] [56]
Adjectives and adverbs employ morphological reduplication for many different reasons such as number agreement when the adjective modifies a plural noun, intensification of the adjective or adverb, and sometimes because the prefix forces the adjective to have a reduplicated stem". [57]
Number agreement for adjectives is entirely optional in Tagalog (e.g., a plural noun does not have to have a plural article marking it): [57]
The entire adjective is repeated for intensification of adjectives or adverbs:
In verbs, reduplication of the root, prefix or infix is employed to convey different grammatical aspects. In "Mag- verbs" reduplication of the root after the prefix "mag-" or "nag-" changes the verb from the infinitive form, or perfective aspect, respectively, to the contemplated or imperfective aspect. [57] Thus:
For ergative verbs (frequently referred to as "object focus" verbs) reduplication of part the infix and the stem occur:
The complete superlative prefix pagka- demands reduplication of the first syllable of the adjective's stem:
Reduplication is not a productive noun derivation process in Wuvulu-Aua as it is in other Austronesian languages. Some nouns exhibit reduplication, though they are considered to be fossilized. [58]
Verb roots can undergo whole or partial reduplication to mark aspect. Actions that are continuous are indicated by a reduplicated initial syllable. A whole reduplication can also be used to indicate imperfective aspect. [59]
The onomatopoeia in Wuvulu language also uses reduplication to describe the sound. These onomatopoeic words can be used as alienable nouns.
This section needs expansion. You can help by adding to it. (July 2022) |
As in many Tibeto-Burman languages, in Burmese, reduplication is used in verbs and adjectives to form adverbs. [61] Many Burmese words, especially adjectives such as လှပ ('beautiful' [l̥a̰pa̰]), which consist of two syllables (when reduplicated, each syllable is reduplicated separately), when reduplicated (လှပ → လှလှပပ 'beautifully' [l̥a̰l̥a̰pa̰pa̰]) become adverbs. [61] This is also true of many Burmese verbs, which become adverbs when reduplicated. [61]
Some nouns are also reduplicated to indicate plurality. For instance, ပြည်, means "country," but when reduplicated to အပြည်ပြည်, it means "many countries" (as in အပြည်ပြည်ဆိုင်ရာ, "international"). Another example is အမျိုး, which means "kinds," but the reduplicated form အမျိုးမျိုး means "multiple kinds."
A few measure words can also be reduplicated to indicate "one or the other":
Reduplication is sometimes employed in verbs and adjectives to enhance the effect of them.
Similar to other Sino-Tibetan languages, adjectives form adverbs by reduplication.
Other than verbs and adjectives, some nouns can be reduplicated to express plurality or a collection in Chinese.
One of the puzzles of Basque is the large number of words that begin with vowels in which the initial and second vowels are the same. Joseba Lakarra proposes that in Pre-Proto-Basque there was extensive reduplication [62] and that later, certain initial consonants were deleted, leaving the VCV pattern of Proto-Basque:
Pre-Proto-Basque | Proto-Basque | Modern Basque | English |
---|---|---|---|
*dar → *da-dar | *adaR | adar | horn (anatomy) |
*dats → *da-dats | *adats | adats | long hair |
*der → *de-der | *edeR | eder | beautiful |
*dol → *do-dol | *odoL | odol | blood |
*gor → *go-gor | *gogoR | gogor | hard |
*nal → *na-nal | *anaL | ahal | can, to be able |
*nan → *na-nan | *anan-tz | ahantz | to forget |
*nin → *ni-nin | *inin-tz | ihintz | dew |
*nol → *no-nol | *onoL | ohol | board |
*nur → *nu-nur | *unuR | hur | hazelnut |
*zal → *za-zal | *azal | azal | bark |
*zen → *ze-zen | *zezen | zezen | bull |
*ten → *te-ten | *eten | eten | break |
*ran → *ra-ran | *aran | aran | plum |
{{cite web}}
: CS1 maint: numeric names: authors list (link)Tuvaluan, often called Tuvalu, is a Polynesian language closely related to the Ellicean group spoken in Tuvalu. It is more or less distantly related to all other Polynesian languages, such as Hawaiian, Māori, Tahitian, Samoan, Tokelauan and Tongan, and most closely related to the languages spoken on the Polynesian Outliers in Micronesia and Northern and Central Melanesia. Tuvaluan has borrowed considerably from Samoan, the language of Christian missionaries in the late 19th and early 20th centuries.
Goemai is an Afro-Asiatic language spoken in the Great Muri Plains region of Plateau State in central Nigeria, between the Jos Plateau and Benue River. Goemai is also the name of the ethnic group of speakers of the Goemai language. The name 'Ankwe' has been used to refer to the people, especially in older literature and to outsiders. As of 2020, it is estimated that there are around 380,000 Goemai speakers.
Ayacucho is a variety of Southern Quechua spoken in the Ayacucho Region, Peru, as well as by immigrants from Ayacucho in Lima. With roughly a million speakers, it is the largest variety of Southern Quechua after Cusco Quechua. The literary standard of Southern Quechua is based on these two closely related Quechua varieties.
Majhi is an Indo-Aryan language spoken in parts of Nepal and formerly in some small pockets of neighboring India.:1 The language is associated with the Majhi people, an ethnic group in those regions who dwell historically near the Saptakoshi River and its tributaries and elsewhere in central and eastern Nepal. The Majhi people generally subsist off of work associated with rivers, including fishing and ferrying.:2 Majhi is written using the Devanagari writing system.
Eastern Pomo, also known as Clear Lake Pomo, is a nearly extinct Pomoan language spoken around Clear Lake in Lake County, California by one of the Pomo peoples.
Tooro or Rutooro is a Bantu language spoken mainly by the Tooro people (Abatooro) from the Tooro Kingdom in western Uganda. There are three main areas where Tooro as a language is mainly used: Kabarole District, Kyenjojo District and Kyegegwa District. Tooro is unusual among Bantu languages as it lacks lexical tone. It is most closely related to Runyoro.
Tübatulabal is an Uto-Aztecan language, traditionally spoken in Kern County, California, United States. It is the traditional language of the Tübatulabal, who still speak the traditional language in addition to English. The language originally had three main dialects: Bakalanchi, Pakanapul and Palegawan.
The Yimas language is spoken by the Yimas people, who populate the Sepik River Basin region of Papua New Guinea. It is spoken primarily in Yimas village, Karawari Rural LLG, East Sepik Province. It is a member of the Lower-Sepik language family. All 250-300 speakers of Yimas live in two villages along the lower reaches of the Arafundi River, which stems from a tributary of the Sepik River known as the Karawari River.
Tsez, also known as Dido, is a Northeast Caucasian language with about 15,000 speakers spoken by the Tsez, a Muslim people in the mountainous Tsunta District of southwestern Dagestan in Russia. The name is said to derive from the Tsez word for 'eagle', but this is most likely a folk etymology. The name Dido is derived from the Georgian word დიდი, meaning 'big'.
Pohnpeian is a Micronesian language spoken as the indigenous language of the island of Pohnpei in the Caroline Islands. Pohnpeian has approximately 30,000 (estimated) native speakers living in Pohnpei and its outlying atolls and islands with another 10,000-15,000 (estimated) living off island in parts of the US mainland, Hawaii, and Guam. It is the second-most widely spoken native language of the Federated States of Micronesia the first being Chuukese.
Aguaruna is an indigenous American language of the Chicham family spoken by the Aguaruna people in Northern Peru. According to Ethnologue, based on the 2007 Census, 53,400 people out of the 55,700 ethnic group speak Aguaruna, making up almost the entire population. It is used vigorously in all domains of life, both written and oral. It is written with the Latin script. The literacy rate in Aguaruna is 60–90%. However, there are few monolingual speakers today; nearly all speakers also speak Spanish. The school system begins with Aguaruna, and as the students progress, Spanish is gradually added. There is a positive outlook and connotation in regard to bilingualism. 50 to 75% of the Aguaruna population are literate in Spanish. A modest dictionary of the language has been published.
Araki is a nearly extinct language spoken in the small island of Araki, south of Espiritu Santo Island in Vanuatu. Araki is gradually being replaced by Tangoa, a language from a neighbouring island.
The Nukak language is a language of uncertain classification, perhaps part of the macrofamily Puinave-Maku. It is very closely related to Kakwa.
Dirasha is a member of the Cushitic branch of the Afro-Asiatic family. It is spoken in the Omo region of Ethiopia, in the hills west of Lake Chamo, around the town of Gidole.
Baiso or Bayso is a Lowland East Cushitic language belonging to the Omo–Tana subgroup, and is spoken in Ethiopia, in the region around Lake Abaya.
In linguistic morphology, inflection is a process of word formation in which a word is modified to express different grammatical categories such as tense, case, voice, aspect, person, number, gender, mood, animacy, and definiteness. The inflection of verbs is called conjugation, while the inflection of nouns, adjectives, adverbs, etc. can be called declension.
Grass Koiari (Koiali) is a Papuan language of Papua New Guinea spoken in the inland Port Moresby area. It is not very close to the other language which shares its name, Mountain Koiali. It is considered a threatened language.
Avá-Canoeiro, known as Avá or Canoe, is a minor Tupi–Guaraní language of the state of Goiás, in Brazil. It can be further divided into two dialects: Tocantins Avá-Canoeiro and Araguaia Avá-Canoeiro. All speakers of the language are monolingual.
Nuaulu is a language indigenous to the island of Seram Island in Indonesia, and it is spoken by the Nuaulu people. The language is split into two dialects, a northern and a southern dialect, between which there a communication barrier. The dialect of Nuaulu referred to on this page is the southern dialect, as described in Bolton 1991.
Wamesa is an Austronesian language of Indonesian New Guinea, spoken across the neck of the Doberai Peninsula or Bird's Head. There are currently 5,000–8,000 speakers. While it was historically used as a lingua franca, it is currently considered an under-documented, endangered language. This means that fewer and fewer children have an active command of Wamesa. Instead, Papuan Malay has become increasingly dominant in the area.