Kwasio language

Last updated
Kwasio
Ngumba, Kola
Native to Cameroon, Equatorial Guinea
Regionalong and near the coast at the border between Cameroon and Equatorial Guinea
Ethnicity Kwasio, Gyele Pygmies
Native speakers
(26,000 cited 1982–2012) [1]
Dialects
  • Bujeba
  • Kwasio
  • Mvumbo
  • Mabi
  • Gyele
  • Kola
Language codes
ISO 639-3 Either:
nmg   Kwasio–Mvumbo
gyi   Gyele–Kola
Glottolog mvum1239
A.81,801 [2]
ELP Gyele
Map of the Gyeli/Kwasio area in Cameroon (dots) with neighbouring languages/peoples Gyelimap.svg
Map of the Gyeli/Kwasio area in Cameroon (dots) with neighbouring languages/peoples

The Kwasio language, also known as Ngumba / Mvumbo, Bujeba, and Gyele / Kola, is a language of Cameroon, spoken in the south along the coast and at the border with Equatorial Guinea by some 70,000[ citation needed ] members of the Ngumba, Kwasio, Gyele and Mabi peoples.[ citation needed ] Many authors [4] [5] [6] view Kwasio and the Gyele/Kola language as distinct. In the Ethnologue, the languages therefore receive different codes: Kwasio has the ISO 639-3 code nmg, [7] while Gyele has the code gyi. [8] The Kwasio, Ngumba, and Mabi are village farmers; the Gyele (also known as the Kola or Koya) are nomadic Pygmy hunter-gatherers living in the rain forest. The Bagyeli (the name the speakers of the language have given themselves) are mostly forager and hunters. They use dogs, traps, machetes, spears, and nets to hunt and catch animals for food. Deforestation has affected their subsistence, and they have recently begun to benefit from selling baskets and meat to tourists. [9]

Contents

Dialects

Dialects are Kwasio (also known as Kwassio, Bisio), Mvumbo (also known as Ngumba, Ngoumba, Mgoumba, Mekuk), and Mabi (Mabea).

The Gyele speak the subdialects of Mvumbo and Gyele in the north Giele, Gieli, Gyeli, Bagiele, Bagyele (Bagyɛlɛ), Bagielli, [10] Bajele, Bajeli, Bogyel, Bogyeli, Bondjiel.

In the south, the Gyele speak Kola, also known as Koya, in the south, also spelled as Likoya, Bako, Bakola, Bakuele, also Bekoe. The local derogatory term for pygmies, Babinga, is also used.

In Equatorial Guinea, the Bujeba dialect is spoken around Southern Bata and South of Rio Benito.

Glottolog adds Shiwa.

ALCAM (2012)

Non-Pygmy varieties

According to ALCAM (2012), the non-Pygmy Kwasio people speak two language varieties, Mvumbo and Mabi, which have moderate mutual intelligibility. They are spoken in Océan Department, Southern Region. The Bisio group of Kwasio people live in Equatorial Guinea, as well as in Gabon where they are known under the ethnonym Shiwo. [11]

Kwasio is geographically the most western of the languages of the A80-A90 Bantu linguistic continuum. It is closely related to Mbwa (Békol) and Bajwe'e, and more distantly to Méka and Béti. [11]

Mabi, the more western dialect, is spoken on the Atlantic coast around Kribi, among Batanga-speaking populations. [11]

Mvumbo is spoken immediately to the east, along the road from Kribi to Lolodorf, in the communes of these two towns, where speakers are mixed mainly with Fang and Ewondo (Beti Fang)-speaking populations. [11]

Pygmy varieties

According to ALCAM (2012), Gyáli and Kola are very close to each other and coexist in the same camps and settlements. On the other hand, they are not in contact with the Baka, the eastern Pygmies. [11]

There are also close linguistic relationships between Bagyáli and the Meka group, although the non-Pygmy Mabi and Mvumbo peoples do not typically like to admit that their language, Kwasio, is closely related to the Pygmy language varieties. [11]

The Bagyáli traditionally inhabit the forests of Océan Department (Southern Region), around Kribi, Bipindi and Lolodorf (in the communes of Kribi, Akom II, Bipindi, and Lolodorf), and are estimated at 4,250 people. [11]

The Bagyáli are also found in Equatorial Guinea. [11]

Phonology

Consonants

Consonants in the Mvùmbò dialect [12]
Labial Alveolar Palatal Velar Glottal
Nasal m n ɲ ŋ
Plosive voiceless p t k ( ʔ )
voiced b d ɡ
prenasal vd. ᵐb ⁿd ᵑɡ
prenasal vl. ᵑk
implosive( ɓ )
Affricate voiceless p͡f t͡s t͡ʃ
voiced b͡v d͡z d͡ʒ
Fricative voiceless f s ( x )( h )
voiced v z
prenasal ᶬv ⁿz
Approximant ɥ l j w
Consonants in the Gyeli dialect [3]
Labial Alveolar Palatal Velar Glottal
Nasal m n ɲ ( ŋ )
Plosive voiceless p t k ʔ
voiced b d ɡ
prenasal vl. ᵐp ⁿt ᵑk
prenasal vd. ᵐb ⁿd ᵑɡ
implosive( ɓ )( ɗ )
Affricate voiceless p͡f ( t͡s ) t͡ʃ ( k͡p )
voiced b͡v ( d͡z ) d͡ʒ
prenasal vl.( ⁿt͡s ) ᶮt͡ʃ
prenasal vd. ᶬb͡v ( ⁿd͡z ) ᶮd͡ʒ ( ᵑᵐɡ͡b )
Fricative voiceless f s
voiced v z
prenasal vl. ᶬf ⁿs
prenasal vd. ᶬv ⁿz
Approximant l j w

Vowels

Vowels in the Mvùmbò dialect [12]
Front Central Back
Close i y u
Close-mid e o
Open-mid ɛ ɔ
Open a
Vowels in the Gyeli dialect [3]
Front Central Back
Close i u
Close-mid e o
Open-mid ɛ ɔ
Open a


Dipthongs in Gyeli/Kwasia are rare and typically only occur in monosyllabic stems of nouns and verbs. There are only 4 dipthongs in the language: /ua/, /uɔ/, /iɛ/, and /ɔa/. These vowels were most likely two separate syllables with a consonant in between them acting as the onset to the second syllable. Through grammaticalization these two syllables likely merged to created the dipthongs used in the language today. [13]

Features

Like the other Niger-Congo languages of Cameroon, Kwasio is a tonal language.

As a Bantu language, it has noun class system. The Kwasio noun class system is somewhat reduced, having retained only 6 genders (a gender being a pairing of a singular and a plural noun class).

Sociolinguistic Context

Gyeli speakers have little to no prestige with outsiders, and the Gyeli community is becoming largely bilingual. Though the secondary language does not appear to be universal for all Gyeli speakers, the invasion and discrimination they face from other communities promotes shame of the Gyeli language and thus pushes for further assimilation. Since they are forced to take undesirable land, they are surrounded by other Bantu farmer communities that do not speak the Gyeli language, and since the Bagyeli don’t generally ascribe to farming for their food as their Bantu neighbors do, they are considered backwards, less-intelligent neighbors who need to succumb to the way the peoples around them live. With the combination of the ever-increasing invasion of the forests from deforestation, infrastructure, and the oil pipeline, and the pressure from the Bantu farmers, the Gyeli language has all but disappeared from outside the Gyeli community. Many Gyeli speakers admit to adopting this discrimination towards their own language and ways, preferring to speak Kwasio or Busu with outsiders rather than their native Gyeli tongue. The Bagyeli have little to no power in their own community or others because they are not politically organized and therefore cannot change their situation. [14]

Syntax

Word Order

Every language orders their subject, object, and verb words within a sentence differently. For the Kwasio/Gyeli language, the word order is SVO (S stands for Subject, V stands for Verb, and O stands for Object). [15]

Within these roles there are other roles that words can have, such as Patient, Theme, and Recipient. The Patient is generally the Object, thus following the word order, the Patient will generally follow the Verb. Themes and Recipients are different types of Patients found within a sentence, and generally one or the other follow the same pattern of the Patient. However, in Kwasio/Gyeli, the indexation that would indicate which element  (Theme or Recipient) is more like P, the indexation doesn't show any preference to either one, therefore the indexation is neutral. [16]

Morphology

Agreement Affixes

Kwasio/Gyeli have many different agreement affixes used but not all of them are segmental. Some of their morphemes are tonal and show object linking. This is called the H-tone and is used to mark an object in a sentence. This can only be done in toneless CV-constructions that are prefixes for nouns. When there are multiple objects in a sentence, only the object closest to the verb gets marked with this morpheme. [17]

Causatives

In Kwasio/Gyeli the causative voice construction can be formed in two ways, either using affixation on the verb or with a direct verb that has the causative meaning that is accompanied by a complenentizer morpheme to give the causative meaning.

An example of affixation on the verb is the concept of 'teach' in Gyeli. To express this concept, the language would add the suffix '-ɛsɛ' onto the root word for 'learn' which is 'gyik' to make the word 'gyikɛse' which would then have the literal meaning, 'make learn'

An example of the a direct verb accompanied by a complementize morpheme is the sentence, 'I make you laugh' which would translate to 'mɛ nzíí sâ nâ wɛ dyɔ.'.

'mɛ-H nzíí-H sâ nâ wɛ-H dyɔ.'.

1SG-PRS PROG-R make COMP 2SG-PRS laugh

'I make you laugh.'

What is important in this sentence are the two words sâ and nâ. 'Sâ' translates to 'make' which has the causative meaning and 'nâ' is a free morpheme that is a complenentizer to add causative meaning. [3]

Presentational Possession

There are many ways that languages illustrate presentational possession, which means the possessum (can be thing being possessed) is presented as a new referent. Kwasio/Gyeli uses the adnominal possessive to express the presentational possession through the bɛ̀ and nà morphemes.

Conjunction

In Kwasio/Gyeli conjunction between clauses is expressed using the 'nà' morpheme. The morpheme is placed in between clauses. Below is an example.

yà lɔ fúálá ná mɛ lɔ láwɔ

ya-H lɔ fúálá ná mɛ lɔ láwɔ

1PL-PRS RETRO end CONJ 1SG RETRO talk

'We just finished and I just spoke.' [3]

See also

The term Bakola is also used for the pygmies of the northern Congo–Gabon border region, which speak the Ngom language.

Related Research Articles

Ganda or Luganda is a Bantu language spoken in the African Great Lakes region. It is one of the major languages in Uganda and is spoken by more than 5.56 million Baganda and other people principally in central Uganda, including the country's capital, Kampala. Typologically, it is an agglutinative, tonal language with subject–verb–object word order and nominative–accusative morphosyntactic alignment.

Linguistics is the scientific study of human language. Someone who engages in this study is called a linguist. See also the Outline of linguistics, the List of phonetics topics, the List of linguists, and the List of cognitive science topics. Articles related to linguistics include:

Kinyarwanda, Rwandan or Rwanda, officially known as Ikinyarwanda, is a Bantu language and the national language of Rwanda. It is a dialect of the Rwanda-Rundi language that is also spoken in adjacent parts of the Democratic Republic of the Congo and in Uganda, where the dialect is known as Rufumbira or Urufumbira. Kinyarwanda is universal among the native population of Rwanda and is mutually intelligible with Kirundi, the national language of neighbouring Burundi. Kinyabwishya and Kinyamulenge are mutually intelligible dialects spoken in the North Kivu and South Kivu provinces of neighbouring DR Congo.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Denaʼina language</span> Athabaskan language spoken in the cook inlet

Denaʼina, also Tanaina, is the Athabaskan language of the region surrounding Cook Inlet. It is geographically unique in Alaska as the only Alaska Athabaskan language to include territory which borders salt water. Four dialects are usually distinguished:

  1. Upper Inlet, spoken in Eklutna, Knik, Susitna, Tyonek
  2. Outer Inlet, spoken in Kenai, Kustatan, Seldovia
  3. Iliamna, spoken in Pedro Bay, Old Iliamna, Lake Iliamna area
  4. Inland, spoken in Nondalton, Lime Village
<span class="mw-page-title-main">Crow language</span> Missouri Valley Siouan language of Montana, US

Crow is a Missouri Valley Siouan language spoken primarily by the Crow Tribe in present-day southeastern Montana. The word Apsáalooke translates to "Children of the Large Beaked Bird", which was later incorrectly translated into English as 'Crow'. It is one of the larger populations of American Indian languages with 4,160 speakers according to the 2015 US Census.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Lolodorf</span> Place in South Province, Cameroon

Lolodorf is a small town-centred region in the south province of the Republic of Cameroon, near the western coast of Africa. It is between Ngoumou and Bipindi, in a zone of the Atlantic Littoral Evergreen Forest.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Tuscarora language</span> Extinct language

Tuscarora, sometimes called Skarò˙rə̨ˀ, is the Iroquoian language of the Tuscarora people, spoken in southern Ontario, Canada, North Carolina and northwestern New York around Niagara Falls, in the United States before becoming extinct in late 2020. The historic homeland of the Tuscarora was in eastern North Carolina, in and around the Goldsboro, Kinston, and Smithfield areas.

Oroha, categorized as an Austronesian language, is one of many languages spoken by Melanesian people in the Solomon Islands. It is also known as Maramasike, Mara Ma-Siki, Oraha, and Oloha, and is used primarily in the southern part of Malaita Island within the Malaita Province. Little Mala is composed of three indigenous languages of the 'Tolo' people which are Na’oni, Pau, and Oroha. They are all slightly different, yet come from the same origin. The three languages may be thought of as different dialects of the same language. The three Tolo villages now harbor schools under the Melanesian Mission.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Cora language</span> Uto-Aztecan language spoken by the Cora people of northern Mexico and western USA

Cora is an indigenous language of Mexico of the Uto-Aztecan language family, spoken by approximately 30,000 people. It is spoken by the ethnic group that is widely known as the Cora, but who refer to themselves as Naáyarite. The Cora inhabit the northern sierra of the Mexican state Nayarit which is named after its indigenous inhabitants. A significant portion of Cora speakers have formed an expatriate community along the southwestern part of Colorado in the United States. Cora is a Mesoamerican language and shows many of the traits defining the Mesoamerican Linguistic Area. Under the General Law of Linguistic Rights of the Indigenous Peoples, it is recognized as a "national language", along with 62 other indigenous languages and Spanish which have the same "validity" in Mexico.

Apurinã, or Ipurina, is a Southern Maipurean language spoken by the Apurinã people of the Amazon basin. It has an active–stative syntax. Apurinã is a Portuguese word used to describe the Popikariwakori people and their language. Apurinã indigenous communities are predominantly found along the Purus River, in the Northwestern Amazon region in Brazil, in the Amazonas state. Its population is currently spread over twenty-seven different indigenous lands along the Purus River. with an estimated total population of 9,500 people. It is predicted, however, that fewer than 30% of the Apurinã population can speak the language fluently. A definite number of speakers cannot be firmly determined because of the regional scattered presence of its people. The spread of Apurinã speakers to different regions was initially caused by conflict or disease, which has consequently led natives to lose the ability to speak the language for lack of practice and also because of interactions with other communities.

Apma is the language of central Pentecost island in Vanuatu. Apma is an Oceanic language. Within Vanuatu it sits between North Vanuatu and Central Vanuatu languages, and combines features of both groups.

The Wuvulu-Aua language is an Austronesian language which is spoken on the Wuvulu and Aua Islands and in the Manus Province of Papua New Guinea.

Komo is a Nilo-Saharan language spoken by the Kwama (Komo) people of Ethiopia, Sudan and South Sudan. It is a member of the Koman languages. The language is also referred to as Madiin, Koma, South Koma, Central Koma, Gokwom and Hayahaya. Many individuals from Komo are multilingual because they are in close proximity to Mao, Kwama and Oromo speakers. Komo is closely related to Kwama, a language spoken by a group who live in the same region of Ethiopia and who also identify themselves as ethnically Komo. Some Komo and Kwama speakers recognize the distinction between the two languages and culture, whereas some people see it as one "ethnolinguistic" community. The 2007 Ethiopian census makes no mention of Kwama, and for this reason its estimate of 8,000 Komo speakers may be inaccurate. An older estimate from 1971 places the number of Komo speakers in Ethiopia at 1,500. The Komo language is greatly understudied; more information is being revealed as researchers are discovering more data about other languages within the Koman family.

The Fwe language, also known as Chifwe, is a Bantu language spoken by the Fwe people in Namibia and Zambia. It is closely related to the Subia language, Chisubia, and is one of several Bantu languages that feature click consonants.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Gyele people</span> Pygmy ethnic group of Gabon and Equatorial Guinea

The Gyele, also known as the Kola (Bakola) or Koya (Bakoya), are the pygmies of southern Cameroon and adjacent areas of Gabon and Equatorial Guinea. They live among Bantu patrons, the Mvumbo and Bassa. They speak a variety of or a language closely related to Mvumbo.

Iyasa is a Bantu language spoken in Cameroon and Equatorial Guinea by the Iyasa and Ndowe coastal fishing peoples. It is also spoken by Pygmies, perhaps Babongo, in Gabon. Approximately 3,000 people speak Iyasa, though some note that this number may be an overestimation.

Temiar is a Central Aslian (Mon–Khmer) language spoken in Western Malaysia by the Temiar people. The Temiar are one of the most numerous Aslian-speaking peoples, numbering around 30,000 in 2017.

Mungbam is a Southern Bantoid language of the Lower Fungom region of Cameroon. It is traditionally classified as a Western Beboid language, but the language family is disputed. Good et al. uses a more accurate name, the 'Yemne-Kimbi group,' but proposes the term 'Beboid.'

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Yatzachi Zapotec</span> Oto-Manguean of Mexico

Yatzachi Zapotec is an Oto-Manguean language of the Zapotecan branch, spoken in northern central Oaxaca, Mexico. 2,500 self-reported being Yatzachi speakers for the 1990 Mexican census, but the actual number of speakers is unknown. The Yatzachi dialect belongs to the Villa Alta group of Zapotec dialects, of which the main dialect is San Bartolomé Zoogocho. The degree of mutual intelligibility between Yatzachi and the San Bartolomé Zoogocho dialect is estimated to be around 90 percent.

Mortlockese, also known as Mortlock or Nomoi, is a language that belongs to the Chuukic group of Micronesian languages in the Federated States of Micronesia spoken primarily in the Mortlock Islands. It is nearly intelligible with Satawalese, with an 18 percent intelligibility and an 82 percent lexical similarity, and Puluwatese, with a 75 percent intelligibility and an 83 percent lexical similarity. The language today has become mutually intelligible with Chuukese, though marked with a distinct Mortlockese accent. Linguistic patterns show that Mortlockese is converging with Chuukese since Mortlockese now has an 80 to 85 percent lexical similarity.

References

  1. Kwasio–Mvumbo at Ethnologue (18th ed., 2015) (subscription required)
    Gyele–Kola at Ethnologue (18th ed., 2015) (subscription required)
  2. Jouni Filip Maho, 2009. New Updated Guthrie List Online
  3. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Grimm, Nadine (2021). A grammar of Gyeli (pdf). Berlin: Language Science Press. p. 511. doi: 10.5281/zenodo.4737370 . ISBN   978-3-98554-007-5.
  4. Rénaud, Patrick (1976). Le bajeli. Phonologie, morphologie nominale. Vol. 1 et 2. Yaoundé: Les Dossiers de l'ALCAM. p. 27.
  5. Grimm, Nadine (2015). A Grammar of Gyeli. Humboldt University Berlin: PhD thesis. p. 8.
  6. Maho, Jouni F. (2009). "New Updated Guthrie List" (PDF). Archived from the original (PDF) on February 3, 2018. Retrieved March 3, 2017.
  7. "Ethnologue: Kwasio" . Retrieved March 3, 2017.
  8. "Ethnologue: Gyele" . Retrieved March 3, 2017.
  9. Grimm, Nadine (2021). A Grammar of Gyeli. Berlin: Language Science Press p.23.
  10. Blench, Roger. Bagyɛlɛ mammal names
  11. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Binam Bikoi, Charles, ed. (2012). Atlas linguistique du Cameroun (ALCAM)[Linguistic Atlas of Cameroon]. Atlas linguistique de l'Afrique centrale (ALAC) (in French). Vol. 1: Inventaire des langues. Yaoundé: CERDOTOLA. ISBN   9789956796069.
  12. 1 2 Um, Emmanuel Ngue (2002). Morphologie Verbale du Mvùmbɔ̀. Université de Yaoundé.
  13. Grimm, Nadine (2021). A Grammar of Gyeli. Language Science Press. pp. 78–79. ISBN   978-3-96110-311-9.
  14. Grimm, Nadine (2021). A Grammar of Gyeli. Language Science Press. pp. 16–17. ISBN   978-3-96110-311-9.
  15. Grimm, Nadine (2021). A grammar of Gyeli. Language Science Press. p. 31. ISBN   978-3-96110-311-9.
  16. Grimm, Nadine (2021). A grammar of Gyeli. Language Science Press. pp. 459, 462. ISBN   978-3-96110-311-9.
  17. Grimm, Nadine (2021). A grammar of Gyeli. Language Science Press. pp. 243–245. ISBN   ISBN978-3-96110-311-9.{{cite book}}: Check |isbn= value: invalid character (help)