Conflict (process)

Last updated

A conflict is a situation in which unacceptable differences in interests, expectations, values, or opinions occur between individuals, or between or in groups.

Contents

Definitions

Depending on the source, there are different definitions for conflicts:

Classification

Jean Rogers, John Wayne, and Ward Bond in the film Conflict (1936) Conflict (1936) 1.jpg
Jean Rogers, John Wayne, and Ward Bond in the film Conflict (1936)

In cases of intragroup conflict, there is a conflict between the overall goals of the general group, and the goals of at least one person in that group. [10] The disagreements may also be examples of interpersonal conflict, a conflict between two or more people. Intrapersonal conflicts are conflicts occurring in an individual, for example a bad conscience or an identity conflict. Intergroup conflict is conflict between two or more groups.

Types

More specific types of conflict include the following.

Geography

Conflicts can also be categorized geographically, as in the North-South conflict and the East-West conflict. Other examples are territorial conflicts such as the Kosovo War, the Iraq-Iran War, the Middle East conflict, the China-Taiwan conflict and the Korean Conflict.

Persons or Groups

Likewise, conflicts can be categorised according to the people involved. Areas in which conflicts frequently occur are, for example, in the family, between parents, between siblings or between parents and children, among friends and acquaintances, in groups, in school, in nature, in business between companies, employers or employees, [15] in science, [16] between generations (generational conflict), between ethnic groups (ethnic conflict) or within or between states (see peace research).

The following are examples of conflict that could be either intragroup or intergroup conflict.

Development

School children fighting in Jamaica Fighting after school in central Jamaica - all shoelaces are open (32361821164).jpg
School children fighting in Jamaica

Conflicts are not static events in themselves, but develop a dynamic that can become uncontrollable in extreme cases. The course of a conflict can be divided into four phases: [23]

Phase of conflictNameDescription
Phase ILatent phaseThe conflict situation emerges
Phase IIConscienceBoth parties recognise the conflict
Phase IIIActionBoth parties react based on their thoughts and emotions
Phase IVInteractionsBoth parties interact with alternating conflict modes

Escalation

Beyond that a conflict can further escalate. Models of escalation in conflicts are the Friedrich Glasl's model of conflict escalation, [2] the conflict curve by Michael S. Lund [24] [25] [26] and the hourglass model by Oliver Ramsbotham. [24] [27] When an escalation is initiated by one party there often is a sequence of escalation behaviour: requests, demands, angry remarks, threats, harassment and abuse. [28]

Models with a fixed order of stages of conflict escalation have been criticized for not representing the probabilistic nature of conflicts. [29]

Behavior

Hierarchy of arguments according to their escalation potential by Graham Graham's Hierarchy of Disagreement-en.svg
Hierarchy of arguments according to their escalation potential by Graham

The various ways in which people react to conflict situations have been presented by Gerhard Schwarz, sometimes in reference to Eric Lippmann, as follows: [30] [31]

These behavior patterns can lead to the solution or dissolution of a conflict in different situations. While the first-mentioned stages are anti- or confrontational in character, the last-mentioned stages represent forms of constructive conflict resolution - with consensus as the highest (to be learned) form. [31] [32] Paul Graham divided forms of argument hierarchically according to their escalation potential and the quality of the argument. [33] In the ‘‘dual concern model’’, conflict types are divided along the two dimensions: orientation towards one's own goal or orientation towards the goal of the conflict partner. [1] [34] [35] Personality tests for conflict behavior are the Kraybill Conflict Style Inventory, [36] the open-source licensed "Ethics Position Questionnaire" [37] and the Thomas-Kilmann Conflict Mode Instrument. Further, more general and comprehensive personality tests are Leadership Derailers, [38] Social value orientation, [39] [40] Hexaco-PI-R [41] and NEO-PI-R, [42] which also include a bit of conflict behaviour.

Interpersonal

Often, further phenomena occur in the course of a conflict. Doubts and uncertainties about one's own position are usually replaced with firm convictions (confirmation bias), without anything having changed in the real probabilities. [43] [1] Moreover, people often stick to their conviction in order not to lose face, even if the conviction is now being questioned by themselves. [44] The reciprocity ("an eye for an eye") favors a conflict escalation [45] and a convergence of behavior when the other side consistently shows competitive or consistently collaborative behavior to achieve their goals. However, collaborative behavior tips more easily into competitive behavior than vice versa. [1] In conflicts, destructive behaviours can also appear: violence, coercion, intimidation, blackmailing, deception und seduction. [28]

Negative emotions such as anger and fear make it difficult to work through the differences. [46] [1] Moreover, anger is often contagious, because a person who is met with anger reacts in turn more often angrily. [47] Likewise, behaviors such as hostile, overly aggressive, choleric, conflict-avoiding, evasive, passive-aggressive, nagging or accusing (without changing anything), non-changing, annoying, pessimistic, superior or indecisive behavior can make conflict resolution difficult. [28]

The attribution of presumed strengths, attitudes, and values to the other conflict party is often distorted during a conflict. [48] [1] Likewise, there is often a false attribution of whether the problem arises from the situation or the character of the participants. [49] As a conflict escalates, the tactics used by each side become more confrontational (harder). However, there are situations where the threatened party fares better when a threat is not met with a counter-threat. [50] [51] [1] A too dominant negotiation style can provoke a blocking stance as a reaction. [52] [53] [1] In conflicts between equally strong parties, a competitive conflict style is avoided if a strong counter-reaction is expected. [54] While at the beginning of a conflict escalation a counter-reaction tends to be disproportionately high, it is rather disproportionately low at a higher level of escalation. Often at the beginning of a conflict escalation, various coalitions are formed to support one's own interests, which later in the conflict lead to conflicts between two groups. [1]

Intergroup

When a conflict occurs not just between two individuals (interpersonal conflict), but between two or more groups (intergroup conflict), additional effects of group dynamics come into play. [55] [1] Five typical emotions have been identified in groups that contribute to escalation: superiority, injustice, vulnerability, mistrust, and helplessness. [56] Additionally, envy, contempt, pity and admiration can also occur between groups. Envy results when the out-group is perceived to have high competence, but low warmth. [57] [58] Envious groups are usually jealous of another group's symbolic and tangible achievements and view that group as competition. Contempt results when the out-group is taken to be low in both competence and warmth. [57] [58] According to Forsyth, contempt is one of the most frequent intergroup emotions. In this situation, the out-group is held responsible for its own failures. In-group members also believe that their conflict with the out-group can never be resolved. Out-groups that are believed by the in-group to be high in warmth but low in competence are pitied. Usually pitied groups are lower in status than the in-group and are not believed to be responsible for their failures. Admiration occurs when an out-group is taken to be high in both warmth and competence, however, admiration is very rare because these two conditions are seldom met. An admired out-group is thought to be completely deserving of its accomplishments. Admiration is thought to be most likely to arise when a member of the in-group can take pride in the accomplishments of the out-group, and when the out-group achieving does not interfere with the in-group. [1] [57] [58]

Groups often exhibit more competitive behavior than individuals within a group do with each other. Merely perceiving one's own group identity already favors discrimination against foreign groups. When individuals with a collaborative conflict style join a group, a switch to a competitive group conflict style (group behavior) can occur. Additionally, other effects of dominant behavior within the group and between groups come into play. Motivations such as greed, fear, and social identity increase in groups. If the potential reward for greed is reduced, the effect of greed diminishes. There is a double standard that manifests itself primarily in an enhancement of the actions of one's own group, but also in a devaluation of the actions of other groups. This also includes distorted generalizations and stereotypes attributed to the other group. It involves both deindividuation (opponents are only perceived as part of a homogeneous group, not as individuals), and dehumanization (opponents are perceived as subhuman). [28] In an experiment, more than half of the participants opted for a choice with less reward if the process was perceived as fair in return. [1] [59]

A key player in inter-group relations and conflict is the collective sentiment a person's own group (in-group) feels toward another group (out-group). These inter-group emotions are usually negative, and range in intensity from feelings of discomfort when interacting with a member of a certain other group to full on hatred for another group and its members. For example, in Fischer's organizational research at the University of Oxford, inter-group conflict was so 'heated' that it became mutually destructive and intractable, resulting in organizational collapse. [60] [61]

Out-group-directed emotions can be expressed both verbally and non-verbally, and according to the stereotype content model, are dictated by two dimensions: the perceived warmth (friendliness) and competence of the other group (skillfulness). Depending on the perceived degree of warmth and competence, the stereotype content model predicts four basic emotions that could be directed toward the out-group (Forsyth, 2010).

Factors

Although the involved parties may hope to reach a solution to their dispute quickly, psychological and interpersonal factors can frustrate their attempts to control the conflict, and in this case, conflict escalation occurs. Conflict escalation "can be understood as an intensification of a conflict with regard to the observed extent and the means used". [62] A number of factors including increased commitment to one's position, use of harder influence tactics, and formation of coalitions propel the escalation of the conflict. [63]

Uncertainty and commitment

As conflicts escalate, group members' doubts and uncertainties are replaced with a firm commitment to their position. People rationalize their choices once they have made them: they seek out information that supports their views, reject information that disconfirms their views, and become more entrenched in their original position (also see confirmatory bias). [43] Additionally, people believe that once they commit to a position publicly, they should stick with it. Sometimes, they may realize the shortcomings of their views, but they continue defending those views and arguing against their opponents just to save face. [64] Finally, if the opponents argue too strongly, reactance may set in and group members become even more committed to the position. [53] [65]

Perception and misperception

Individuals' reactions to the conflict are shaped by their perception of the situation and people in the situation. During the conflict, opponents' inferences about each other's strengths, attitudes, values, and personal qualities tend to be largely distorted. [66]

Misattribution

During the conflict, people explain their opponents' actions in ways that make the problem worse. Fundamental attribution error occurs when one assumes that opponents' behavior was caused by personal (dispositional) rather than situational (environmental) factors. [67] When conflict continues for a while, opponents might decide that this conflict is intractable. People usually expect intractable conflicts to be prolonged, intense, and very hard to resolve. [68]

Misperceiving motivations

During the conflict, opponents often become mistrustful of one another wondering if their cooperative motivations were replaced by competitive ones. This loss of trust makes it difficult to return to the cooperative relationship. People with competitive social value orientations (SVOs) are the most inaccurate in their perception of opponents' motivation. They often think that others compete with them when in fact, there is no competition going on. [69] Competitors are also more biased in their search for information that confirms their suspicions that others compete with them. [70] They also tend to deliberately misrepresent their intentions, sometimes claiming to be more cooperatively oriented than they actually are. [71]

Soft tactics and hard tactics

People use soft tactics at the outset of the conflict, but as it escalates, tactics become stronger and harder. To demonstrate this phenomenon, Mikolic, Parker, and Pruitt (1997) [72] simulated a conflict situation by creating a "birthday card factory" with study participants who were paid a small amount for each card they manufactured using paper, colored markers, and ribbons. The work went well until researchers' confederate who posed as another participant started hoarding production materials. Initially, group members tried to solve the problem with statements and requests. When these methods failed they shifted to demands and complaints, and then to threats, abuse, and anger.

Although hard tactics can overwhelm the opponent, they often intensify conflicts. Morton Deutsch and Robert Krauss (1960) [73] used trucking game experiment to demonstrate that capacity to threaten others intensifies conflict. They also showed that establishing a communication link does not always help to solve the dispute. [74] If one party threatens the other, the threatened party will sometimes fare best if it cannot respond with a counterthreat. [50] [51] Equally powerful opponents, however, learn to avoid the use of power if the fear of retaliation is high. [75]

Reciprocity and upward conflict spiral

In many cases, upward conflict spirals are sustained by the norms of reciprocity: if one group or person criticizes the other, the criticized person or group feels justified in doing the same. In conflict situations, opponents often follow the norm of rough reciprocity, i.e. they give too much (overmatching) or too little (undermatching) in return. At low levels of conflict, opponents overmatch their threats, while at high levels of conflict they undermatch their threats. Overmatching may serve as a strong warning, while undermatching may be used to send conciliatory messages. [76]

Few and many

When conflicts erupt, group members use coalitions to shift the balance of power in their favor, and it is typical for multiparty conflicts to reduce to two-party blocks over time. Coalitions contribute to the conflict because they draw more members of the group into the affray. Individuals in coalitions work not only to ensure their own outcomes but also to worsen outcomes of non-coalition members. Those who are excluded from the coalition react with hostility and try to regain power by forming their own coalition. Thus, coalitions need to be constantly maintained through strategic bargaining and negotiation. [77]

Irritation and anger

It is generally difficult for most people to remain calm and collected in a conflict situation. However, an increase in negative emotions (i.e. anger) only exacerbates the initial conflict. Even when group members want to discuss their positions calmly and dispassionately, once they become committed to their positions, an emotional expression often replaces logical discussion. [46] Anger is also contagious: when group member negotiates with someone who is angry, they become angry themselves. [78]

Styles

Thomas and Kilmann distinguish five typical conflict styles: [79]

Conflict stylePros and ConsSituations
Competitive
(win-lose)
  • Pursuit of own objectives
  • Use of power
  • Can lead to disputes
  • Can cause resentments
  • Emergencies requiring quick decisions
  • Important and unpopular decisions
  • When you are certain you are right (important matters)
  • To defend against others taking advantage
Collaborative
(win-win)
  • Cooperation to everyone's satisfaction
  • Detailed analysis of interests
  • Involves cognitive conflict
  • Can be time-consuming
  • When a compromise is not acceptable
  • To gain support for the cause
  • To maintain or improve relationships
  • To unify perspectives
Compromise-seeking
(½win-½win)
  • Middle ground regarding assertiveness and cooperativity
  • Splitting the difference
  • Neither half nor whole?
  • Temporary solutions for complex conflicts
  • When goals of two equally strong opponents are mutually exclusive
  • Under time pressure
  • Evasive strategy for collaborative or competitive
Avoiding
(lose-lose)
  • Neglect of both interests
  • Conflicts remain unresolved
  • Delaying tactic
  • Hopeless situations outside of one's control
  • Unimportant situations
  • To let others cool down
  • When others can resolve the conflict more effectively
Accommodating
(lose-win)
  • Opposite of competitive
  • Self-sacrifice
  • Selfless generosity
  • Doormat
  • When the other person's issues are much more important
  • To build social capital
  • When clearly inferior and losing
  • When harmony is especially important
  • To allow subordinates to develop

Limited resources can be distributed either according to the previous investment of time, energy and resources, according to equal shares, according to power ratios or according to need. [1] A decision can be reached using various procedures. Ideally, a consensus is worked on collaboratively (and preferably on a win-win solution) because this serves the interests of all parties involved. In situations where the interests are immovable and mutually exclusive, other procedures must be used. Common procedures in which only some of the interests of both sides are served are compromise procedures or distribution imposed by a neutral authority (as in the inquisitorial or arbitration procedure, with parents or a supervisor). Furthermore, a vote or a judicial judgment is often carried out as a competitive procedure, in which the interests of the larger group or the right-preserving side are served first. Likewise, a concession by one side can resolve the conflict if there is a willingness to do so. As a concession implies at least a partial renunciation of one's own interests with little to no compensation, a willingness to do so becomes less likely with increasing escalation.

According to Ramsbotham, conflicts are divided into five phases of conflict development, to which three conflict resolution strategies are assigned: [24]

While the Thomas and Kilmann system describes five typical conflict styles, aligns the measures with the conflict styles and the situations and emphasizes collaboration as a solution, the measures in the Glasl system and the Ramsbotham system are aligned with the escalation level.

Resolution

Conflict resolution is conceptualized as the methods and processes involved in facilitating the peaceful ending of conflict and retribution. Committed group members attempt to resolve group conflicts by actively communicating information about their conflicting motives or ideologies to the rest of group (e.g., intentions; reasons for holding certain beliefs) and by engaging in collective negotiation. [80] Dimensions of resolution typically parallel the dimensions of conflict in the way the conflict is processed. Cognitive resolution is the way disputants understand and view the conflict, with beliefs, perspectives, understandings and attitudes. Emotional resolution is in the way disputants feel about a conflict, the emotional energy. Behavioral resolution is reflective of how the disputants act, their behavior. [81] Ultimately a wide range of methods and procedures for addressing conflict exist, including negotiation, mediation, mediation-arbitration, diplomacy, and creative peacebuilding. [82] [83]

Mediation

Conflict is a social process that is exacerbated when individual members of a group take sides in the debate. Among the methods to resolve conflict is mediation of the dispute by a group member not currently involved in the dispute. More specifically, a mediator is defined as a person who attempts to resolve a conflict between two group members by intervening in this conflict. Put simply, the mediator can be thought of as a disinterested guide directs the disputants through the process of developing a solution to a disagreement. [1] For conflicts with negative interpersonal relationships on a low escalation level, relationship building can help transform the nature of the relationship and improve the communication. [28] As mediation depends on meeting together peacefully, it is more successful in conflicts with low levels of escalation where there is still a will to work on an agreement.

Although the tendency will be for group members who are uninvolved in the dispute to remain uninvolved, in some cases, the sheer intensity of the conflict may escalate to the point where mediation is unavoidable and still feasible. Third party mediation of the conflict opens avenues for communication between group members in conflict. It allows members to express their opinions and request clarification of other member's standpoints while the mediator acts as a form of protection against any shame or "loss of face" that either disputant may experience. This can be done by shedding a positive light on the reconciliation that was made during the mediation process. For instance, if it was negotiated that two cashiers will rotate the weekends they work, the mediator might point out that now each worker gets a weekend off every two weeks. [84] The mediator can also offer assistance in refining solutions and making counter-offers between members, adjusting the time and location of meetings so that they are mutually satisfying for both parties. [84] There are three major mediation approaches:

  1. Inquisitorial procedure: Using this procedure, the mediator asks each of the disputants a series of questions, considers the two sets of responses, and then selects and imposes a mandatory solution on the members. The inquisitorial procedure is the least popular approach to mediation.
  2. Arbitration: Here, mediation involves the two disputants explaining their arguments to the mediator, who creates a solution based on the arguments presented. Arbitration is best for low intensity conflict, but is the most favored mediation style overall.
  3. Moot: The moot approach involves an open discussion between disputants and the mediator about the problems and potential solutions. In the moot approach, the mediator cannot impose a mandatory solution. After arbitration, a moot is the most preferred mediation style. It does not always lead to a tangible result.

In practice, conflict resolution is often interwoven with daily activities, as in organizations, workplaces and institutions. Staff and residents in a youth care setting, for instance, interweave everyday concerns (meals, lessons, breaks, meetings, or other mundane but concerted projects) with interpersonal disputes. [85]

Institutionalization

Military forces, such as the Polish Armed Forces seen here, are an example of an institution that handles conflict. KUNA 10 (05) (cropped).jpg
Military forces, such as the Polish Armed Forces seen here, are an example of an institution that handles conflict.

The institutionalization of conflicts refers to the resolution or settlement of a conflict when it has been passed on to an institution. [86] However, delegation to institutions brings about a lack of freedom in terms of the distribution structure of entitlements and offers. The parties to the conflict are directed by persons not involved in the conflict. In this process, the emotional and factual components of the conflict are separated. The institution or instance proceeds with rules that are mutually recognized by the parties to the conflict.

Feud

Conflicts can be strongly or weakly institutionalized. The feud, a weakly institutionalized conflict, has some rules that are recognized by the parties to the conflict (e.g., existence of a legitimate feud reason, formal announcement, procedure, etc.), all signs of institutionalization, but on the other hand, the conflict is handled by the parties to the conflict themselves (no social differentiation); the emotional and factual component of the conflict are not separated: friends of the respective parties to the conflict not only have the right, but even the duty to assist, and they generate further conflicts.

Justice system

The justice system is a national competitive system (interest of one party is to be served) for the regulation of conflicts. The procedures are divided into criminal procedures and civil procedures. The procedures are used when a legal claim is to be negotiated.

Military

The military is used, among other things, in the event of a very strong escalation of a conflict between states or paramilitary groups. It is a competitive system with comparatively strong damages and collateral damages and is therefore used as a last resort. Early written works on military conflict resolution are The Art of War by Sunzi [87] and On War by Carl von Clausewitz. [88]

Divorce

A divorce is a judicial conflict management system. If there are children, sometimes mediation, counseling or child protective services are used following a separation of parents.

Company conflicts

Conflicts between employees of a company can create conflict cost to the company. [89] There is On-the-job training and Coaching for conflict management.

See also

References

  1. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 Forsyth, D. R. (2019). Group dynamics (7th ed.). Cengage. ISBN   9781337408851.
  2. 1 2 Glasl, Friedrich (July 15, 1982). Bomers, Gerard B. J.; Peterson, Richard B. (eds.). Conflict Management and Industrial Relations. Springer Netherlands. pp. 119–140. doi:10.1007/978-94-017-1132-6_6 via Springer Link.
  3. Schwarz, Gerhard (2014). Konfliktmanagement: Konflikte erkennen, analysieren, lösen (in German). Wiesbaden: Gabler Verlag. p. 36. doi:10.1007/978-3-8349-4598-3. ISBN   978-3-8349-4597-6.
  4. M. Afzalur Rahim (2011), Managing Conflict in Organizations, Transaction Publishers, ISBN   978-1-4128-1456-0
  5. Pfetsch, Frank R. (January 1994). Konflikt und Konfliktbewältigung. Beispiele für Formen zwischenstaatlicher Auseinandersetzungen (in German). Stuttgart: Klett-Schulbuchverl. ISBN   3-12-490400-1.
  6. Nicholson, Michael (1992). Rationality and the analysis of international conflict (in German). Cambridge University Press. ISBN   0-521-39125-3. OCLC   23687612.
  7. Nye, Robert D. (1973). Conflict Among Humans: Some Basic Psychological and Social-psychological Considerations. Springer Publishing Company. ISBN   978-0-8261-1371-9.
  8. Schmidt, Stuart M.; Kochan, Thomas A. (1972). "Conflict: Toward Conceptual Clarity". Administrative Science Quarterly. 17 (3): 359. doi:10.2307/2392149. JSTOR   2392149.
  9. Bernard, J. (1951-03-01). "The Conceptualization of Intergroup Relations: With Special Reference to Conflict" . Social Forces. 29 (3): 243–251. doi:10.2307/2572412. ISSN   0037-7732. JSTOR   2572412.
  10. Sidorenkov, Andrey V; Borokhovski, Evgueni F; Kovalenko, Viktor A (October 2018). "Group size and composition of work groups as precursors of intragroup conflicts". Psychology Research and Behavior Management. 11: 511–523. doi: 10.2147/prbm.s178761 . ISSN   1179-1578. PMC   6207408 . PMID   30498379.
  11. Jowett, Sophia; Lavallee, David (2007). Social psychology in sport. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. ISBN   978-0-7360-5780-6. OCLC   64770988.
  12. 1 2 3 4 Jehn, K. A.; Mannix, E. A. (2001-04-01). "The Dynamic Nature of Conflict: A Longitudinal Study of Intragroup Conflict and Group Performance". Academy of Management Journal. 44 (2): 238–251. ISSN   0001-4273. JSTOR   3069453.
  13. Järvisalo, J.; Saris, N. E. (1975-09-15). "Action of propranolol on mitochondrial functions--effects on energized ion fluxes in the presence of valinomycin". Biochemical Pharmacology. 24 (18): 1701–1705. doi:10.1016/0006-2952(75)90009-x. ISSN   0006-2952. PMID   13.
  14. 1 2 Amason, Allen C.; Sapienza, Harry J. (August 1997). "The Effects of Top Management Team Size and interaction Norms on Cognitive and Affective Conflict". Journal of Management. 23 (4): 495–516. doi:10.1177/014920639702300401. ISSN   0149-2063. S2CID   143833786.
  15. Overton, Amy; Lowry, Ann (2013-11-20). "Conflict Management: Difficult Conversations with Difficult People". Clinics in Colon and Rectal Surgery. 26 (4). Georg Thieme Verlag KG: 259–264. doi: 10.1055/s-0033-1356728 . ISSN   1531-0043. PMC   3835442 . PMID   24436688.
  16. Pietschmann, Herbert (2005). "Vom Umgang mit wissenschaftlichen Konflikten. Movement on the waterfront". Würzburger medizinhistorische Mitteilungen (in German). Vol. 24. pp. 425–433.
  17. "Conflict Of Interest", Encyclopedia of Evaluation, Thousand Oaks California: Sage Publications, Inc., 2005, doi:10.4135/9781412950558.n97, ISBN   978-0-7619-2609-2
  18. 1 2 Lee, Yueh-Ting (2004). The psychology of ethnic and cultural conflict. Westport, Conn: Praeger. ISBN   9780275979836.
  19. MacDonald, Kevin (2009-04-01). "Evolution, Psychology, and a Conflict Theory of Culture". Evolutionary Psychology. 7 (2) 147470490900700206. doi: 10.1177/147470490900700206 . ISSN   1474-7049. S2CID   4247168.
  20. "Organizational conflict | definition in the Cambridge English Dictionary". dictionary.cambridge.org. Retrieved 2020-07-18.
  21. "Definition of role conflict | Dictionary.com". www.dictionary.com. Retrieved 2020-07-18.
  22. "What Is Work-Family Conflict? | Baylor Online". Baylor University MBA Online Degree Program. Retrieved 2020-07-18.
  23. Berkel, Karl (1985). "Konfliktmanagement". In Fritz Neske; Markus Wiener (eds.). Management-Lexikon (in German). Vol. II.
  24. 1 2 3 Bösch, Richard (2017-11-20). "Conflict Escalation". Oxford Research Encyclopedia of International Studies. Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acrefore/9780190846626.013.82. ISBN   978-0-19-084662-6.
  25. Saunders, H. H. (1998). "Review of Preventing Violent Conflicts: A Strategy for Preventive Diplomacy, by M. S. Lund; The Management of Conflict: Interpretations and Interests in Comparative Perspective, by M. H. Ross". Political Psychology. 19 (4): 859–865. JSTOR   3791880.
  26. Lund, Michael S. (1996). Preventing violent conflicts: A strategy for preventive diplomacy. Washington, D.C: US Institute of Peace Press. ISBN   978-1-878379-52-8.
  27. Ramsbotham, Oliver; Miall, Hugh; Woodhouse, Tom (2011-04-11). Contemporary Conflict Resolution. Polity. ISBN   978-0-7456-4973-3.
  28. 1 2 3 4 5 Deutsch, M., ed. (2006). The handbook of conflict resolution: theory and practice (2nd ed.). Jossey-Bass. ISBN   9780787980580.
  29. Bösch, Richard (2017). "Conflict Escalation" . Oxford Research Encyclopedia of International Studies. doi:10.1093/acrefore/9780190846626.013.82. ISBN   978-0-19-084662-6.
  30. Jürg Kuster, Eugen Huber, Robert Lippmann, Alphons Schmid, Emil Schneider, Urs Witschi, Roger Wüst (2008). Handbook of Project Management. Springer-Verlag. pp. 258 =. ISBN   978-3-540-76432-8.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  31. 1 2 Hans Fischer (2015). Learning to Lead Sustainably: The Comprehensive Leadership Concept PENTA for Sustainable Leadership Impact. vdf Hochschulverlag AG. p. 189. ISBN   978-3-7281-3667-1.
  32. Norbert J. Heigl (2014). Understanding and Controlling Conflicts. Springer. p. 24. ISBN   978-3-658-04584-5.
  33. Paul Graham (2008). "How to Disagree" . Retrieved 2022-03-09.
  34. Wallensteen, P. (2015). Understanding conflict resolution (4th ed.). SAGE. ISBN   9781473902107.
  35. Dickmann, Christian R. (2015). Konflikte lösen: Was Sie als Führungskraft über Konflikte im betrieblichen Umfeld wissen sollten[Resolving Conflicts: What You Should Know as a Leader about Conflicts in the Corporate Environment] (in German). epubli. p. 22. ISBN   978-3-7375-2373-8.
  36. Kraybill, Ronald (2005). Style Matters: The Kraybill Conflict Style Inventory. Riverhouse. ISBN   0-9755071-2-5.
  37. O’Boyle, Ernest H.; Forsyth, Donelson R. (2021-06-21). "Individual differences in ethics positions: The EPQ-5". PLOS ONE. 16 (6). Public Library of Science (PLoS): e0251989. Bibcode:2021PLoSO..1651989O. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0251989 . ISSN   1932-6203. PMC   8216522 . PMID   34153037.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: article number as page number (link)
  38. Hogan, Robert; Hogan, Joyce; Warrenfeltz, Rodney (2007). The Hogan Guide. Hogan Assessment Systems. ISBN   978-0-9816457-6-6.
  39. Murphy, Ryan O.; Ackermann, Kurt A.; Handgraaf, Michel J. J. (2011). "Measuring Social Value Orientation". Judgment and Decision Making. 6 (8): 771–781. doi: 10.1017/S1930297500004204 . ISSN   1930-2975.
  40. Au, W. T.; Kwong, J. Y. Y. (2004-06-21). "Measurements and Effects of Social-Value Orientation in Social Dilemmas: A Review". In Suleiman, Ramzi (ed.). Contemporary Psychological Research on Social Dilemmas. Cambridge, UK; New York: Cambridge University Press. pp. 71–98. ISBN   0-521-80892-8.
  41. Lee, Kibeom; Ashton, Michael C. (2004). "Psychometric Properties of the HEXACO Personality Inventory". Multivariate Behavioral Research. 39 (2). Informa UK Limited: 329–358. doi:10.1207/s15327906mbr3902_8. ISSN   0027-3171. PMID   26804579.
  42. Soto, Christopher J.; John, Oliver P. (2017). "The next Big Five Inventory (BFI-2): Developing and assessing a hierarchical model with 15 facets to enhance bandwidth, fidelity, and predictive power" (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 113 (1). American Psychological Association (APA): 117–143. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000096 . ISSN   1939-1315. PMID   27055049.
  43. 1 2 Ross, L.; Ward, A. (2013-06-17). "Naïve Realism in Everyday Life: Implications for Social Conflict and Misunderstanding". In Reed, Edward S.; Turiel, Elliot; Brown, Terrance (eds.). Values and Knowledge. Psychology Press. pp. 103–135. doi:10.4324/9780203773772-6 (inactive 15 July 2025). ISBN   978-0-203-77377-2.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: DOI inactive as of July 2025 (link)
  44. David C. Wilson: ‘‘A Strategy of Change: Concepts and Controversies in the Management of Change’’. ITBP Textbooks Series, Routledge series in analytical management. Thomson, 1992, ISBN 1-86152-383-1.
  45. Youngs, George A. (1986). "Patterns of threat and punishment reciprocity in a conflict setting" . Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 51 (3): 541–546. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.51.3.541. ISSN   1939-1315.
  46. 1 2 De Dreu, C.K.W.; Beersma, B.; Steinel, W.; Kleef, G.A. van (2007). "The psychology of negotiation: Principles and basic processes.". In A. W. Kruglanski; E. T. Higgins (eds.). Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford. pp. 608–629.
  47. Van Kleef, Gerben A.; De Dreu, Carsten K. W.; Manstead, Antony S. R. (2004). "The Interpersonal Effects of Emotions in Negotiations: A Motivated Information Processing Approach" (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 87 (4): 510–528. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.87.4.510 . ISSN   1939-1315. PMID   15491275.
  48. Thompson, L.; Nadler, J. (2006-09-18). "Judgmental biases in conflict resolution and how to overcome them". In Deutsch, Morton; Coleman, Peter T.; Marcus, Eric C. (eds.). The Handbook of Conflict Resolution: Theory and Practice. John Wiley & Sons. pp. 213–235. ISBN   978-0-7879-8666-7.
  49. Ross, Lee (1977). "The Intuitive Psychologist And His Shortcomings: Distortions in the Attribution Process". Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. Vol. 10. Elsevier. pp. 173–220. doi:10.1016/s0065-2601(08)60357-3. ISBN   978-0-12-015210-0.
  50. 1 2 Borah, Lee A. (1963). "The effects of threat in bargaining: Critical and experimental analysis" . The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 66 (1): 37–44. doi:10.1037/h0043937. ISSN   0096-851X. PMID   14013852.
  51. 1 2 Gallo, Philip S. (1966). "Effects of increased incentives upon the use of threat in bargaining" . Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 4 (1): 14–20. doi:10.1037/h0023510. ISSN   1939-1315. PMID   5965187.
  52. Curhan, Jared R; Neale, Margaret A; Ross, Lee (2004). "Dynamic valuation: Preference changes in the context of face-to-face negotiation" (PDF). Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 40 (2): 142–151. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2003.12.002 .
  53. 1 2 Brehm, Sharon S.; Brehm, Jack W. (1981). Psychological Reactance: A Theory of Freedom and Control. Elsevier. doi:10.1016/C2013-0-10423-0. ISBN   978-0-12-129840-1.
  54. Lawler, Edward J.; Ford, Rebecca S.; Blegen, Mary A. (1988). "Coercive Capability in Conflict: A Test of Bilateral Deterrence Versus Conflict Spiral Theory". Social Psychology Quarterly. 51 (2): 93. doi:10.2307/2786833. JSTOR   2786833.
  55. Saguy, Tamar; Reifen-Tagar, Michal (2022-08-02). "The social psychological roots of violent intergroup conflict" . Nature Reviews Psychology. 1 (10): 577–589. doi:10.1038/s44159-022-00083-7. ISSN   2731-0574.
  56. Eidelson, Roy J.; Eidelson, Judy I. (2003). "Dangerous ideas. Five beliefs that propel groups toward conflict". The American Psychologist. 58 (3): 182–192. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.58.3.182. ISSN   0003-066X. PMID   12772423.
  57. 1 2 3 Cuddy, Amy J. C.; Fiske, Susan T.; Glick, Peter (2007). "The BIAS map: Behaviors from intergroup affect and stereotypes" . Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 92 (4): 631–648. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.92.4.631. ISSN   1939-1315. PMID   17469949.
  58. 1 2 3 Fiske, Susan T.; Cuddy, Amy J.C.; Glick, Peter (2007). "Universal dimensions of social cognition: warmth and competence" . Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 11 (2): 77–83. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.005. PMID   17188552.
  59. Sidanius, Jim; Haley, Hillary; Molina, Ludwin; Pratto, Felicia (2007). "Vladimir's Choice and the Distribution of Social Resources: A Group Dominance Perspective". Group Processes & Intergroup Relations. 10 (2): 257–265. doi: 10.1177/1368430207074732 . hdl: 2262/52378 . ISSN   1368-4302.
  60. Fischer, Michael D. (September 2012). "Organizational Turbulence, Trouble and Trauma: Theorizing the Collapse of a Mental Health Setting". Organization Studies. 33 (9): 1153–1173. doi:10.1177/0170840612448155. ISSN   0170-8406. S2CID   52219788.
  61. Fischer, Michael Daniel; Ferlie, Ewan (January 2013). "Resisting hybridisation between modes of clinical risk management: Contradiction, contest, and the production of intractable conflict". Accounting, Organizations and Society. 38 (1): 30–49. doi:10.1016/j.aos.2012.11.002. S2CID   44146410.
  62. Bösch, Richard (2017-11-20). "Conflict Escalation". Oxford Research Encyclopedia of International Studies. Oxford Research Encyclopedia of International Studies. doi:10.1093/acrefore/9780190846626.013.82. ISBN   978-0-19-084662-6 . Retrieved 2020-07-18.
  63. Forsyth, D.R. (2010). Group Dynamics (5th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.
  64. Wilson, D. C. (1992). A Strategy of Change. London: Routledge.
  65. Curhan, Jared R; Neale, Margaret A; Ross, Lee (2004). "Dynamic valuation: Preference changes in the context of face-to-face negotiation" (PDF). Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 40 (2). Elsevier BV: 142–151. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2003.12.002 . ISSN   0022-1031.
  66. Thompson, L.; Nadler, J. (2006-09-18). "Judgmental biases in conflict resolution and how to overcome them". In Deutsch, Morton; Coleman, Peter T.; Marcus, Eric C. (eds.). The Handbook of Conflict Resolution: Theory and Practice. John Wiley & Sons. pp. 213–235. ISBN   978-0-7879-8666-7.
  67. Ross, Lee (1977). "The Intuitive Psychologist And His Shortcomings: Distortions in the Attribution Process". Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. Vol. 10. Elsevier. pp. 173–220. doi:10.1016/s0065-2601(08)60357-3. ISBN   978-0-12-015210-0. ISSN   0065-2601.
  68. Bar-Tal, D. (2007). Living with the conflict: Socio-psychological analysis of the Israeli-Jewish society (in Hebrew). Jerusalem: Carmel.
  69. Sattler, D. N.; Kerr, N. L. (1991). "Might versus morality explored: Motivational and cognitive bases for social motives". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 60 (5): 756–765. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.60.5.756.
  70. Kleef, Gerben A. Van; Dreu, Carsten K.W. De (2002-01-01). "Social value orientation and impression formation: A test of two competing hypotheses about information search in negotiation" . International Journal of Conflict Management. 13 (1). MCB UP Ltd: 59–77. doi:10.1108/eb022867.
  71. Steinel, Wolfgang; De Dreu, Carsten K. W. (2004). "Social Motives and Strategic Misrepresentation in Social Decision Making". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 86 (3): 419–434. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.86.3.419 . hdl: 1887/14958 . ISSN   1939-1315. PMID   15008646.
  72. Mikolic, Joseph M.; Parker, John C.; Pruitt, Dean G. (1997). "Escalation in response to persistent annoyance: Groups versus individuals and gender effects" . Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 72 (1): 151–163. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.72.1.151. ISSN   1939-1315. PMID   9008378.
  73. Deutsch, Morton; Krauss, Robert M. (1960). "The effect of threat upon interpersonal bargaining" . The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 61 (2): 181–189. doi:10.1037/h0042589. ISSN   0096-851X. PMID   13722331.
  74. Krauss, R. M.; Morsella, E. (2011-03-04). "Communication and conflict.". In Deutsch, Morton; Coleman, Peter T.; Marcus, Eric C. (eds.). The Handbook of Conflict Resolution: Theory and Practice. John Wiley & Sons. pp. 144–157. ISBN   978-1-118-04690-6.
  75. Lawler, Edward J.; Ford, Rebecca S.; Blegen, Mary A. (1988). "Coercive Capability in Conflict: A Test of Bilateral Deterrence Versus Conflict Spiral Theory". Social Psychology Quarterly. 51 (2). [Sage Publications, Inc., American Sociological Association]: 93–107. doi:10.2307/2786833. ISSN   0190-2725. JSTOR   2786833.
  76. Youngs, G. A., Jr. (1986). "Patterns of threat and punishment reciprocity in a conflict setting". Journal of Personality & Social Psychology. 51 (3): 541–546. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.51.3.541.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  77. Mannix, Elizabeth A. (1993). "Organizations as Resource Dilemmas: The Effects of Power Balance on Coalition Formation in Small Groups". Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. 55 (1). Elsevier BV: 1–22. doi:10.1006/obhd.1993.1021. ISSN   0749-5978.
  78. Van Kleef, Gerben A.; De Dreu, Carsten K. W.; Manstead, Antony S. R. (2004). "The Interpersonal Effects of Emotions in Negotiations: A Motivated Information Processing Approach" (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 87 (4): 510–528. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.87.4.510 . ISSN   1939-1315. PMID   15491275.
  79. "Understanding conflict: The Thomas-Kilmann Conflict Mode Instrument" (PDF). University of Cambridge Judge Business School. Retrieved February 11, 2024.
  80. Forsyth, Donelson R. (19 March 2009). Group Dynamics (5th ed.). Boston, MA: Wadsworth Cengage Learning. ISBN   978-0495599524.
  81. Mayer, Bernard (27 March 2012). The Dynamics of Conflict: A Guide to Engagement and Intervention (2nd ed.). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. ISBN   978-0470613535.
  82. Rapoport, A. (1989). The origins of violence: Approaches to the study of conflict. New York, NY: Paragon House.
  83. Rapoport, A. (1992). Peace: An idea whose time has come. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.
  84. 1 2 Forsyth 2010
  85. Wästerfors, David (2010-08-18). "Disputes and Going Concerns in an Institution for "Troublesome" Boys" . Journal of Contemporary Ethnography. 40. SAGE PublicationsSage CA: Los Angeles, CA: 39–70. doi:10.1177/0891241610377199.
  86. Ralf Dahrendorf (1974). Pfade aus Utopia (in German). München: Piper. ISBN   978-3-4921-0101-1.
  87. The Art of War. Translated by Lionel Giles. 1910 via Internet Archive.
  88. Carl von Clausewitz (1832–1834). Marie von Clausewitz (ed.). Vom Kriege. Vol. 1–3. Berlin: Ferdinand Dümmler.
  89. Gaba, Vibha; Joseph, John (2023-11-14). "Content and process: organizational conflict and decision making". Frontiers in Psychology. 14 1227966. Frontiers Media SA. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1227966 . ISSN   1664-1078. PMC   10681961 . PMID   38034290.