Stereotype

Last updated

An 18th-century Dutch engraving of the peoples of the world 18th century ethnography.jpg
An 18th-century Dutch engraving of the peoples of the world
A stereotypical caricature of a villain (i.e. generic melodramatic villain stock character, with handlebar moustache and black top-hat), particularly popular in early-20th-century silent films and melodramas and popularized by Snidely Whiplash Villainc.svg
A stereotypical caricature of a villain (i.e. generic melodramatic villain stock character, with handlebar moustache and black top-hat), particularly popular in early-20th-century silent films and melodramas and popularized by Snidely Whiplash
Police officers buying doughnuts and coffee, an example of perceived stereotypical behavior in North America Cops in a Donut Shop 2011 Shankbone.jpg
Police officers buying doughnuts and coffee, an example of perceived stereotypical behavior in North America

In social psychology, a stereotype is a generalized belief about a particular category of people. [2] It is an expectation that people might have about every person of a particular group. The type of expectation can vary; it can be, for example, an expectation about the group's personality, preferences, appearance or ability. Stereotypes are often overgeneralized, inaccurate, and resistant to new information. [3] A stereotype does not necessarily need to be a negative assumption. They may be positive, neutral, or negative.

Contents

Explicit stereotypes

An explicit stereotype refers to stereotypes that one is aware that one holds, and is aware that one is using to judge people. If person A is making judgments about a particular person B from group G, and person A has an explicit stereotype for group G, their decision bias can be partially mitigated using conscious control; however, attempts to offset bias due to conscious awareness of a stereotype often fail at being truly impartial, due to either underestimating or overestimating the amount of bias being created by the stereotype.

Implicit stereotypes

Implicit stereotypes are those that lay on individuals' subconsciousness, that they have no control or awareness of. [4] "Implicit stereotypes are built based on two concepts, associative networks in semantic (knowledge) memory and automatic activation". [5] Implicit stereotypes are automatic and involuntary associations that people make between a social group and a domain or attribute. For example, one can have beliefs that women and men are equally capable of becoming successful electricians but at the same time many can associate electricians more with men than women. [5]

In social psychology, a stereotype is any thought widely adopted about specific types of individuals or certain ways of behaving intended to represent the entire group of those individuals or behaviors as a whole. [6] These thoughts or beliefs may or may not accurately reflect reality. [7] [8] Within psychology and across other disciplines, different conceptualizations and theories of stereotyping exist, at times sharing commonalities, as well as containing contradictory elements. Even in the social sciences and some sub-disciplines of psychology, stereotypes are occasionally reproduced and can be identified in certain theories, for example, in assumptions about other cultures. [9]

Etymology

The term stereotype comes from the French adjective stéréotype and derives from the Greek words στερεός ( stereos ), 'firm, solid' [10] and τύπος ( typos ), 'impression', [11] hence 'solid impression on one or more ideas/theories'.

The term was first used in the printing trade in 1798 by Firmin Didot, to describe a printing plate that duplicated any typography. The duplicate printing plate, or the stereotype, is used for printing instead of the original.

Outside of printing, the first reference to stereotype in English was in 1850, as a noun that meant 'image perpetuated without change'. [12] However, it was not until 1922 that stereotype was first used in the modern psychological sense by American journalist Walter Lippmann in his work Public Opinion . [13]

Relationship with other types of intergroup attitudes

Stereotypes, prejudice, racism, and discrimination [14] are understood as related but different concepts. [15] [16] [17] [18] Stereotypes are regarded as the most cognitive component and often occurs without conscious awareness, whereas prejudice is the affective component of stereotyping and discrimination is one of the behavioral components of prejudicial reactions. [15] [16] [19] In this tripartite view of intergroup attitudes, stereotypes reflect expectations and beliefs about the members of groups perceived as different from one's own, prejudice represents the emotional response, and discrimination refers to actions. [15] [16]

Although related, the three concepts can exist independently of each other. [16] [20] According to Daniel Katz and Kenneth Braly, stereotyping leads to racial prejudice when people emotionally react to the name of a group, ascribe characteristics to members of that group, and then evaluate those characteristics. [17]

Possible prejudicial effects of stereotypes [8] are:

Content

Stereotype content model, adapted from Fiske et al. (2002): Four types of stereotypes resulting from combinations of perceived warmth and competence. Mixed stereotype content model (Fiske et al.).png
Stereotype content model, adapted from Fiske et al. (2002): Four types of stereotypes resulting from combinations of perceived warmth and competence.

Stereotype content refers to the attributes that people think characterize a group. Studies of stereotype content examine what people think of others, rather than the reasons and mechanisms involved in stereotyping. [22]

Early theories of stereotype content proposed by social psychologists such as Gordon Allport assumed that stereotypes of outgroups reflected uniform antipathy. [23] [24] For instance, Katz and Braly argued in their classic 1933 study that ethnic stereotypes were uniformly negative. [22]

By contrast, a newer model of stereotype content theorizes that stereotypes are frequently ambivalent and vary along two dimensions: warmth and competence. Warmth and competence are respectively predicted by lack of competition and status. Groups that do not compete with the in-group for the same resources (e.g., college space) are perceived as warm, whereas high-status (e.g., economically or educationally successful) groups are considered competent. The groups within each of the four combinations of high and low levels of warmth and competence elicit distinct emotions. [25] The model explains the phenomenon that some out-groups are admired but disliked, whereas others are liked but disrespected. This model was empirically tested on a variety of national and international samples and was found to reliably predict stereotype content. [23] [26]

An even more recent model of stereotype content called the agency–beliefs–communion (ABC) model suggested that methods to study warmth and competence in the stereotype content model (SCM) were missing a crucial element, that being, stereotypes of social groups are often spontaneously generated. [27] Experiments on the SCM usually ask participants to rate traits according to warmth and competence but this does not allow participants to use any other stereotype dimensions. [28] The ABC model, proposed by Koch and colleagues in 2016 is an estimate of how people spontaneously stereotype U.S social groups of people using traits. Koch et al. conducted several studies asking participants to list groups and sort them according to their similarity. [27] Using statistical techniques, they revealed three dimensions that explained the similarity ratings. These three dimensions were agency (A), beliefs (B), and communion (C). Agency is associated with reaching goals, standing out and socio-economic status and is related to competence in the SCM, with some examples of traits including poor and wealthy, powerful and powerless, low status and high status. Beliefs is associated with views on the world, morals and conservative-progressive beliefs with some examples of traits including traditional and modern, religious and science-oriented or conventional and alternative. Finally, communion is associated with connecting with others and fitting in and is similar to warmth from the SCM, with some examples of traits including trustworthy and untrustworthy, cold and warm and repellent and likeable. [29] According to research using this model, there is a curvilinear relationship between agency and communion. [30] For example, if a group is high or low in the agency dimension then they may be seen as un-communal, whereas groups that are average in agency are seen as more communal. [31] This model has many implications in predicting behaviour towards stereotyped groups. For example, Koch and colleagues recently proposed that perceived similarity in agency and beliefs increases inter-group cooperation. [32]

Functions

Early studies suggested that stereotypes were only used by rigid, repressed, and authoritarian people. This idea has been refuted by contemporary studies that suggest the ubiquity of stereotypes and it was suggested to regard stereotypes as collective group beliefs, meaning that people who belong to the same social group share the same set of stereotypes. [20] Modern research asserts that full understanding of stereotypes requires considering them from two complementary perspectives: as shared within a particular culture/subculture and as formed in the mind of an individual person. [33]

Relationship between cognitive and social functions

Stereotyping can serve cognitive functions on an interpersonal level, and social functions on an intergroup level. [8] [20] For stereotyping to function on an intergroup level (see social identity approaches: social identity theory and self-categorization theory), an individual must see themselves as part of a group and being part of that group must also be salient for the individual. [20]

Craig McGarty, Russell Spears, and Vincent Y. Yzerbyt (2002) argued that the cognitive functions of stereotyping are best understood in relation to its social functions, and vice versa. [34]

Cognitive functions

Stereotypes can help make sense of the world. They are a form of categorization that helps to simplify and systematize information. Thus, information is more easily identified, recalled, predicted, and reacted to. [20] Stereotypes are categories of objects or people. Between stereotypes, objects or people are as different from each other as possible. [6] Within stereotypes, objects or people are as similar to each other as possible. [6]

Gordon Allport has suggested possible answers to why people find it easier to understand categorized information. [35] First, people can consult a category to identify response patterns. Second, categorized information is more specific than non-categorized information, as categorization accentuates properties that are shared by all members of a group. Third, people can readily describe objects in a category because objects in the same category have distinct characteristics. Finally, people can take for granted the characteristics of a particular category because the category itself may be an arbitrary grouping.

A complementary perspective theorizes how stereotypes function as time- and energy-savers that allow people to act more efficiently. [6] Yet another perspective suggests that stereotypes are people's biased perceptions of their social contexts. [6] In this view, people use stereotypes as shortcuts to make sense of their social contexts, and this makes a person's task of understanding his or her world less cognitively demanding. [6]

Social functions

Social categorization

In the following situations, the overarching purpose of stereotyping is for people to put their collective self (their in-group membership) in a positive light: [36]

  • when stereotypes are used for explaining social events
  • when stereotypes are used for justifying activities of one's own group (ingroup) to another group (outgroup)
  • when stereotypes are used for differentiating the ingroup as positively distinct from outgroups
Explanation purposes
An antisemitic 1873 caricature depicting the stereotypical physical features of a Jewish male Antisemitic caricature 1873.jpg
An antisemitic 1873 caricature depicting the stereotypical physical features of a Jewish male

As mentioned previously, stereotypes can be used to explain social events. [20] [36] Henri Tajfel [20] described his observations of how some people found that the antisemitic fabricated contents of The Protocols of the Elders of Zion only made sense if Jews have certain characteristics. Therefore, according to Tajfel, [20] Jews were stereotyped as being evil and yearning for world domination to match the antisemitic "facts" as presented in The Protocols of the Elders of Zion.

Justification purposes

People create stereotypes of an outgroup to justify the actions that their in-group has committed (or plans to commit) towards that outgroup. [20] [35] [36] For example, according to Tajfel, [20] Europeans stereotyped African, Indian, and Chinese people as being incapable of achieving financial advances without European help. This stereotype was used to justify European colonialism in Africa, India, and China.

Intergroup differentiation

An assumption is that people want their ingroup to have a positive image relative to outgroups, and so people want to differentiate their ingroup from relevant outgroups in a desirable way. [20] If an outgroup does not affect the ingroup's image, then from an image preservation point of view, there is no point for the ingroup to be positively distinct from that outgroup. [20]

People can actively create certain images for relevant outgroups by stereotyping. People do so when they see that their ingroup is no longer as clearly and/or as positively differentiated from relevant outgroups, and they want to restore the intergroup differentiation to a state that favours the ingroup. [20] [36]

Self-categorization

Stereotypes can emphasize a person's group membership in two steps: Stereotypes emphasize the person's similarities with ingroup members on relevant dimensions, and also the person's differences from outgroup members on relevant dimensions. [24] People change the stereotype of their ingroups and outgroups to suit context. [24] Once an outgroup treats an ingroup member badly, they are more drawn to the members of their own group. [37] This can be seen as members within a group are able to relate to each other though a stereotype because of identical situations. A person can embrace a stereotype to avoid humiliation such as failing a task and blaming it on a stereotype. [38]

Social influence and consensus

Stereotypes are an indicator of ingroup consensus. [36] When there are intragroup disagreements over stereotypes of the ingroup and/or outgroups, ingroup members take collective action to prevent other ingroup members from diverging from each other. [36]

John C. Turner proposed in 1987 [36] that if ingroup members disagree on an outgroup stereotype, then one of three possible collective actions follow: First, ingroup members may negotiate with each other and conclude that they have different outgroup stereotypes because they are stereotyping different subgroups of an outgroup (e.g., Russian gymnasts versus Russian boxers). Second, ingroup members may negotiate with each other, but conclude that they are disagreeing because of categorical differences amongst themselves. Accordingly, in this context, it is better to categorise ingroup members under different categories (e.g., Democrats versus Republican) than under a shared category (e.g., American). Finally, ingroup members may influence each other to arrive at a common outgroup stereotype.

Formation

Different disciplines give different accounts of how stereotypes develop: Psychologists may focus on an individual's experience with groups, patterns of communication about those groups, and intergroup conflict. As for sociologists, they may focus on the relations among different groups in a social structure. They suggest that stereotypes are the result of conflict, poor parenting, and inadequate mental and emotional development. Once stereotypes have formed, there are two main factors that explain their persistence. First, the cognitive effects of schematic processing (see schema) make it so that when a member of a group behaves as we expect, the behavior confirms and even strengthens existing stereotypes. Second, the affective or emotional aspects of prejudice render logical arguments against stereotypes ineffective in countering the power of emotional responses. [39]

Correspondence bias

Correspondence bias refers to the tendency to ascribe a person's behavior to disposition or personality, and to underestimate the extent to which situational factors elicited the behavior. Correspondence bias can play an important role in stereotype formation. [40]

For example, in a study by Roguer and Yzerbyt (1999) participants watched a video showing students who were randomly instructed to find arguments either for or against euthanasia. The students that argued in favor of euthanasia came from the same law department or from different departments. Results showed that participants attributed the students' responses to their attitudes although it had been made clear in the video that students had no choice about their position. Participants reported that group membership, i.e., the department that the students belonged to, affected the students' opinions about euthanasia. Law students were perceived to be more in favor of euthanasia than students from different departments despite the fact that a pretest had revealed that subjects had no preexisting expectations about attitudes toward euthanasia and the department that students belong to. The attribution error created the new stereotype that law students are more likely to support euthanasia. [41]

Nier et al. (2012) found that people who tend to draw dispositional inferences from behavior and ignore situational constraints are more likely to stereotype low-status groups as incompetent and high-status groups as competent. Participants listened to descriptions of two fictitious groups of Pacific Islanders, one of which was described as being higher in status than the other. In a second study, subjects rated actual groups – the poor and wealthy, women and men – in the United States in terms of their competence. Subjects who scored high on the measure of correspondence bias stereotyped the poor, women, and the fictitious lower-status Pacific Islanders as incompetent whereas they stereotyped the wealthy, men, and the high-status Pacific Islanders as competent. The correspondence bias was a significant predictor of stereotyping even after controlling for other measures that have been linked to beliefs about low status groups, the just-world fallacy and social dominance orientation. [42]

Based on the anti-public sector bias, [43] Döring and Willems (2021) [44] found that employees in the public sector are considered as less professional compared to employees in the private sector. They build on the assumption that the red-tape and bureaucratic nature of the public sector spills over in the perception that citizens have about the employees working in the sector. With an experimental vignette study, they analyze how citizens process information on employees' sector affiliation, and integrate non-work role-referencing to test the stereotype confirmation assumption underlying the representativeness heuristic. The results show that sector as well as non-work role-referencing influences perceived employee professionalism but has little effect on the confirmation of particular public sector stereotypes. [45] Moreover, the results do not confirm a congruity effect of consistent stereotypical information: non-work role-referencing does not aggravate the negative effect of sector affiliation on perceived employee professionalism.

Illusory correlation

Research has shown that stereotypes can develop based on a cognitive mechanism known as illusory correlation – an erroneous inference about the relationship between two events. [6] [46] [47] If two statistically infrequent events co-occur, observers overestimate the frequency of co-occurrence of these events. The underlying reason is that rare, infrequent events are distinctive and salient and, when paired, become even more so. The heightened salience results in more attention and more effective encoding, which strengthens the belief that the events are correlated. [48] [49] [50]

In the inter-group context, illusory correlations lead people to misattribute rare behaviors or traits at higher rates to minority group members than to majority groups, even when both display the same proportion of the behaviors or traits. Black people, for instance, are a minority group in the United States and interaction with blacks is a relatively infrequent event for an average white American. [51] Similarly, undesirable behavior (e.g. crime) is statistically less frequent than desirable behavior. Since both events "blackness" and "undesirable behavior" are distinctive in the sense that they are infrequent, the combination of the two leads observers to overestimate the rate of co-occurrence. [48] Similarly, in workplaces where women are underrepresented and negative behaviors such as errors occur less frequently than positive behaviors, women become more strongly associated with mistakes than men. [52]

In a landmark study, David Hamilton and Richard Gifford (1976) examined the role of illusory correlation in stereotype formation. Subjects were instructed to read descriptions of behaviors performed by members of groups A and B. Negative behaviors outnumbered positive actions and group B was smaller than group A, making negative behaviors and membership in group B relatively infrequent and distinctive. Participants were then asked who had performed a set of actions: a person of group A or group B. Results showed that subjects overestimated the frequency with which both distinctive events, membership in group B and negative behavior, co-occurred, and evaluated group B more negatively. This despite the fact the proportion of positive to negative behaviors was equivalent for both groups and that there was no actual correlation between group membership and behaviors. [48] Although Hamilton and Gifford found a similar effect for positive behaviors as the infrequent events, a meta-analytic review of studies showed that illusory correlation effects are stronger when the infrequent, distinctive information is negative. [46]

Hamilton and Gifford's distinctiveness-based explanation of stereotype formation was subsequently extended. [49] A 1994 study by McConnell, Sherman, and Hamilton found that people formed stereotypes based on information that was not distinctive at the time of presentation, but was considered distinctive at the time of judgement. [53] Once a person judges non-distinctive information in memory to be distinctive, that information is re-encoded and re-represented as if it had been distinctive when it was first processed. [53]

Common environment

One explanation for why stereotypes are shared is that they are the result of a common environment that stimulates people to react in the same way. [6]

The problem with the 'common environment' is that explanation in general is that it does not explain how shared stereotypes can occur without direct stimuli. [6] Research since the 1930s suggested that people are highly similar with each other in how they describe different racial and national groups, although those people have no personal experience with the groups they are describing. [54]

Socialization and upbringing

Another explanation says that people are socialised to adopt the same stereotypes. [6] Some psychologists believe that although stereotypes can be absorbed at any age, stereotypes are usually acquired in early childhood under the influence of parents, teachers, peers, and the media.

If stereotypes are defined by social values, then stereotypes only change as per changes in social values. [6] The suggestion that stereotype content depends on social values reflects Walter Lippman's argument in his 1922 publication that stereotypes are rigid because they cannot be changed at will. [17]

Studies emerging since the 1940s refuted the suggestion that stereotype contents cannot be changed at will. Those studies suggested that one group's stereotype of another group would become more or less positive depending on whether their intergroup relationship had improved or degraded. [17] [55] [56] Intergroup events (e.g., World War II, Persian Gulf conflicts) often changed intergroup relationships. For example, after WWII, Black American students held a more negative stereotype of people from countries that were the United States's WWII enemies. [17] If there are no changes to an intergroup relationship, then relevant stereotypes do not change. [18]

Intergroup relations

According to a third explanation, shared stereotypes are neither caused by the coincidence of common stimuli, nor by socialisation. This explanation posits that stereotypes are shared because group members are motivated to behave in certain ways, and stereotypes reflect those behaviours. [6] It is important to note from this explanation that stereotypes are the consequence, not the cause, of intergroup relations. This explanation assumes that when it is important for people to acknowledge both their ingroup and outgroup, they will emphasise their difference from outgroup members, and their similarity to ingroup members. [6] International migration creates more opportunities for intergroup relations, but the interactions do not always disconfirm stereotypes. They are also known to form and maintain them. [57]

Activation

The dual-process model of cognitive processing of stereotypes asserts that automatic activation of stereotypes is followed by a controlled processing stage, during which an individual may choose to disregard or ignore the stereotyped information that has been brought to mind. [19]

A number of studies have found that stereotypes are activated automatically. Patricia Devine (1989), for example, suggested that stereotypes are automatically activated in the presence of a member (or some symbolic equivalent) of a stereotyped group and that the unintentional activation of the stereotype is equally strong for high- and low-prejudice persons. Words related to the cultural stereotype of blacks were presented subliminally. During an ostensibly unrelated impression-formation task, subjects read a paragraph describing a race-unspecified target person's behaviors and rated the target person on several trait scales. Results showed that participants who received a high proportion of racial words rated the target person in the story as significantly more hostile than participants who were presented with a lower proportion of words related to the stereotype. This effect held true for both high- and low-prejudice subjects (as measured by the Modern Racism Scale). Thus, the racial stereotype was activated even for low-prejudice individuals who did not personally endorse it. [19] [58] [59] Studies using alternative priming methods have shown that the activation of gender and age stereotypes can also be automatic. [60] [61]

Subsequent research suggested that the relation between category activation and stereotype activation was more complex. [59] [62] Lepore and Brown (1997), for instance, noted that the words used in Devine's study were both neutral category labels (e.g., "Blacks") and stereotypic attributes (e.g., "lazy"). They argued that if only the neutral category labels were presented, people high and low in prejudice would respond differently. In a design similar to Devine's, Lepore and Brown primed the category of African-Americans using labels such as "blacks" and "West Indians" and then assessed the differential activation of the associated stereotype in the subsequent impression-formation task. They found that high-prejudice participants increased their ratings of the target person on the negative stereotypic dimensions and decreased them on the positive dimension whereas low-prejudice subjects tended in the opposite direction. The results suggest that the level of prejudice and stereotype endorsement affects people's judgements when the category – and not the stereotype per se – is primed. [63]

Research has shown that people can be trained to activate counterstereotypic information and thereby reduce the automatic activation of negative stereotypes. In a study by Kawakami et al. (2000), for example, participants were presented with a category label and taught to respond "No" to stereotypic traits and "Yes" to nonstereotypic traits. After this training period, subjects showed reduced stereotype activation. [64] [65] This effect is based on the learning of new and more positive stereotypes rather than the negation of already existing ones. [65]

Automatic behavioral outcomes

Empirical evidence suggests that stereotype activation can automatically influence social behavior. [66] [67] [68] [69] For example, Bargh, Chen, and Burrows (1996) activated the stereotype of the elderly among half of their participants by administering a scrambled-sentence test where participants saw words related to age stereotypes. Subjects primed with the stereotype walked significantly more slowly than the control group (although the test did not include any words specifically referring to slowness), thus acting in a way that the stereotype suggests that elderly people will act. And the stereotype of the elder will affect the subjective perception of them through depression. [70] In another experiment, Bargh, Chen, and Burrows also found that because the stereotype about blacks includes the notion of aggression, subliminal exposure to black faces increased the likelihood that randomly selected white college students reacted with more aggression and hostility than participants who subconsciously viewed a white face. [71] Similarly, Correll et al. (2002) showed that activated stereotypes about blacks can influence people's behavior. In a series of experiments, black and white participants played a video game, in which a black or white person was shown holding a gun or a harmless object (e.g., a mobile phone). Participants had to decide as quickly as possible whether to shoot the target. When the target person was armed, both black and white participants were faster in deciding to shoot the target when he was black than when he was white. When the target was unarmed, the participants avoided shooting him more quickly when he was white. Time pressure made the shooter bias even more pronounced. [72]

Accuracy

A magazine feature from Beauty Parade from March 1952 stereotyping women drivers. It features Bettie Page as the model. Bettie Page driving.jpg
A magazine feature from Beauty Parade from March 1952 stereotyping women drivers. It features Bettie Page as the model.

Stereotypes can be efficient shortcuts and sense-making tools. They can, however, keep people from processing new or unexpected information about each individual, thus biasing the impression formation process. [6] Early researchers believed that stereotypes were inaccurate representations of reality. [54] A series of pioneering studies in the 1930s found no empirical support for widely held racial stereotypes. [17] By the mid-1950s, Gordon Allport wrote that, "It is possible for a stereotype to grow in defiance of all evidence." [35]

Research on the role of illusory correlations in the formation of stereotypes suggests that stereotypes can develop because of incorrect inferences about the relationship between two events (e.g., membership in a social group and bad or good attributes). This means that at least some stereotypes are inaccurate. [46] [48] [50] [53]

A 1995 book by Yueh-Ting Lee et al. argued that stereotypes are sometimes accurate. [73] Similarly, a 2015 study by Jussim et al. reviewed four studies of racial stereotypes, and seven studies of gender stereotypes regarding demographic characteristics, academic achievement, personality and behavior, and argued that some aspects of ethnic and gender stereotypes are accurate while stereotypes concerning political affiliation and nationality are much less accurate. [74]

A 2005 study by Terracciano et al. found that stereotypic beliefs about nationality do not reflect the actual personality traits of people from different cultures. [75]

In a 1973 paper, Marlene MacKie argued that while stereotypes are inaccurate, this is a definition rather than empirical claim – stereotypes were simply defined as inaccurate, even though the supposed inaccuracy of stereotypes was treated as though it was an empirical discovery. [76]

Effects

Attributional ambiguity

Attributive ambiguity refers to the uncertainty that members of stereotyped groups experience in interpreting the causes of others' behavior toward them. Stereotyped individuals who receive negative feedback can attribute it either to personal shortcomings, such as lack of ability or poor effort, or the evaluator's stereotypes and prejudice toward their social group. Alternatively, positive feedback can either be attributed to personal merit or discounted as a form of sympathy or pity. [77] [78] [79]

Crocker et al. (1991) showed that when black participants were evaluated by a white person who was aware of their race, black subjects mistrusted the feedback, attributing negative feedback to the evaluator's stereotypes and positive feedback to the evaluator's desire to appear unbiased. When the black participants' race was unknown to the evaluator, they were more accepting of the feedback. [80]

Attributional ambiguity has been shown to affect a person's self-esteem. When they receive positive evaluations, stereotyped individuals are uncertain of whether they really deserved their success and, consequently, they find it difficult to take credit for their achievements. In the case of negative feedback, ambiguity has been shown to have a protective effect on self-esteem as it allows people to assign blame to external causes. Some studies, however, have found that this effect only holds when stereotyped individuals can be absolutely certain that their negative outcomes are due to the evaluators's prejudice. If any room for uncertainty remains, stereotyped individuals tend to blame themselves. [78]

Attributional ambiguity can also make it difficult to assess one's skills because performance-related evaluations are mistrusted or discounted. Moreover, it can lead to the belief that one's efforts are not directly linked to the outcomes, thereby depressing one's motivation to succeed. [77]

Stereotype threat

The effect of stereotype threat (ST) on math test scores for girls and boys. Data from Osborne (2007). Stereotype threat - osborne 2007.png
The effect of stereotype threat (ST) on math test scores for girls and boys. Data from Osborne (2007).

Stereotype threat occurs when people are aware of a negative stereotype about their social group and experience anxiety or concern that they might confirm the stereotype. [82] Stereotype threat has been shown to undermine performance in a variety of domains. [83] [84]

Claude M. Steele and Joshua Aronson conducted the first experiments showing that stereotype threat can depress intellectual performance on standardized tests. In one study, they found that black college students performed worse than white students on a verbal test when the task was framed as a measure of intelligence. When it was not presented in that manner, the performance gap narrowed. Subsequent experiments showed that framing the test as diagnostic of intellectual ability made black students more aware of negative stereotypes about their group, which in turn impaired their performance. [85] Stereotype threat effects have been demonstrated for an array of social groups in many different arenas, including not only academics but also sports, [86] chess [87] and business. [88]

Some researchers have suggested that stereotype threat should not be interpreted as a factor in real-life performance gaps, and have raised the possibility of publication bias. [89] [90] [91] Other critics have focused on correcting what they claim are misconceptions of early studies showing a large effect. [92] However, meta-analyses and systematic reviews have shown significant evidence for the effects of stereotype threat, though the phenomenon defies over-simplistic characterization. [93] [94] [95] [96] [97] [98] [99] [100]

Self-fulfilling prophecy

Stereotypes lead people to expect certain actions from members of social groups. These stereotype-based expectations may lead to self-fulfilling prophecies, in which one's inaccurate expectations about a person's behavior, through social interaction, prompt that person to act in stereotype-consistent ways, thus confirming one's erroneous expectations and validating the stereotype. [101] [102] [103]

Word, Zanna, and Cooper (1974) demonstrated the effects of stereotypes in the context of a job interview. White participants interviewed black and white subjects who, prior to the experiments, had been trained to act in a standardized manner. Analysis of the videotaped interviews showed that black job applicants were treated differently: They received shorter amounts of interview time and less eye contact; interviewers made more speech errors (e.g., stutters, sentence incompletions, incoherent sounds) and physically distanced themselves from black applicants. In a second experiment, trained interviewers were instructed to treat applicants, all of whom were white, like the whites or blacks had been treated in the first experiment. As a result, applicants treated like the blacks of the first experiment behaved in a more nervous manner and received more negative performance ratings than interviewees receiving the treatment previously afforded to whites. [104]

A 1977 study by Snyder, Tanke, and Berscheid found a similar pattern in social interactions between men and women. Male undergraduate students were asked to talk to female undergraduates, whom they believed to be physically attractive or unattractive, on the phone. The conversations were taped and analysis showed that men who thought that they were talking to an attractive woman communicated in a more positive and friendlier manner than men who believed that they were talking to unattractive women. This altered the women's behavior: Female subjects who, unknowingly to them, were perceived to be physically attractive behaved in a friendly, likeable, and sociable manner in comparison with subjects who were regarded as unattractive. [105]

A 2005 study by J. Thomas Kellow and Brett D. Jones looked at the effects of self-fulfilling prophecy on African American and Caucasian high school freshman students. Both white and black students were informed that their test performance would be predictive of their performance on a statewide, high stakes standardized test. They were also told that historically, white students had outperformed black students on the test. This knowledge created a self-fulfilling prophecy in both the white and black students, where the white students scored statistically significantly higher than the African American students on the test. The stereotype threat of underperforming on standardized tests affected the African American students in this study. [106]

In accountancy, there is a popular stereotype which represents members of the profession as being humorless, introspective beancounters. [107] [108]

Discrimination and prejudice

Because stereotypes simplify and justify social reality, they have potentially powerful effects on how people perceive and treat one another. [109] As a result, stereotypes can lead to discrimination in labor markets and other domains. [110] For example, Tilcsik (2011) has found that employers who seek job applicants with stereotypically male heterosexual traits are particularly likely to engage in discrimination against gay men, suggesting that discrimination on the basis of sexual orientation is partly rooted in specific stereotypes and that these stereotypes loom large in many labor markets. [21] Agerström and Rooth (2011) showed that automatic obesity stereotypes captured by the Implicit Association Test can predict real hiring discrimination against the obese. [111] Similarly, experiments suggest that gender stereotypes play an important role in judgments that affect hiring decisions. [98] [112]

Stereotypes can cause racist prejudice. For example, scientists and activists have warned that the use of the stereotype "Nigerian Prince" for referring to Advance-fee scammers is racist, i.e. "reducing Nigeria to a nation of scammers and fraudulent princes, as some people still do online, is a stereotype that needs to be called out". [113]

Self-stereotyping

Stereotypes can affect self-evaluations and lead to self-stereotyping. [8] [114] For instance, Correll (2001, 2004) found that specific stereotypes (e.g., the stereotype that women have lower mathematical ability) affect women's and men's evaluations of their abilities (e.g., in math and science), such that men assess their own task ability higher than women performing at the same level. [115] [116] Similarly, a study by Sinclair et al. (2006) has shown that Asian American women rated their math ability more favorably when their ethnicity and the relevant stereotype that Asian Americans excel in math was made salient. In contrast, they rated their math ability less favorably when their gender and the corresponding stereotype of women's inferior math skills was made salient. Sinclair et al. found, however, that the effect of stereotypes on self-evaluations is mediated by the degree to which close people in someone's life endorse these stereotypes. People's self-stereotyping can increase or decrease depending on whether close others view them in stereotype-consistent or inconsistent manner. [117]

Stereotyping can also play a central role in depression, when people have negative self-stereotypes about themselves. According to Cox, Abramson, Devine, and Hollon (2012)., [8] stereotyping can also play a central role in depression, which is characterized by negative self-schemas. Stereotypes and self-schemas are the same type of cognitive structure, therefore, they suggest that an integrated perspective of prejudice and depression provides useful insight on how stereotypes are acquired. Negative stereotypes are set in motion within the Source, who conveys the prejudice towards the Target, which in turn will lead the Target to suffer from depression. Members of stigmatized groups may internalize the negative evaluation of their group and develop depression. People may also show prejudice internalization through self-stereotyping because of negative childhood experiences such as verbal and physical abuse. This depression that is caused by prejudice (i.e., "deprejudice") can be related to group membership (e.g., Me–Gay–Bad) or not (e.g., Me–Bad). If someone holds prejudicial beliefs about a stigmatized group and then becomes a member of that group, they may internalize their prejudice and develop depression. People may also show prejudice internalization through self-stereotyping because of negative childhood experiences such as verbal and physical abuse. [118]

Substitute for observations

Stereotypes are traditional and familiar symbol clusters, expressing a more or less complex idea in a convenient way. They are often simplistic pronouncements about gender, racial, ethnic, and cultural backgrounds and they can become a source of misinformation and delusion. For example, in a school when students are confronted with the task of writing a theme, they think in terms of literary associations, often using stereotypes picked up from books, films, and magazines that they have read or viewed.

The danger in stereotyping lies not in its existence, but in the fact that it can become a substitute for observation and a misinterpretation of a cultural identity. [119] Promoting information literacy is a pedagogical approach that can effectively combat the entrenchment of stereotypes. The necessity for using information literacy to separate multicultural "fact from fiction" is well illustrated with examples from literature and media. [120]

Role in art and culture

American political cartoon titled The Usual Irish Way of Doing Things, depicting a drunken Irishman lighting a powder keg and swinging a bottle. Published in Harper's Weekly, 1871. TheUsualIrishWayofDoingThings.jpg
American political cartoon titled The Usual Irish Way of Doing Things, depicting a drunken Irishman lighting a powder keg and swinging a bottle. Published in Harper's Weekly , 1871.

Stereotypes are common in various cultural media, where they take the form of dramatic stock characters. The instantly recognizable nature of stereotypes mean that they are effective in advertising and situation comedy. [121] Alexander Fedorov (2015) proposed a concept of media stereotypes analysis. This concept refers to identification and analysis of stereotypical images of people, ideas, events, stories, themes, etc. in media context. [122]

The characters that do appear in movies greatly affect how people worldwide perceive gender relations, race, and cultural communities. Because approximately 85% of worldwide ticket sales are directed toward Hollywood movies, the American movie industry has been greatly responsible for portraying characters of different cultures and diversity to fit into stereotypical categories. [123] This has led to the spread and persistence of gender, racial, ethnic, and cultural stereotypes seen in the movies. [89]

For example, Russians are usually portrayed as ruthless agents, brutal mobsters and villains in Hollywood movies. [124] [125] [126] According to Russian American professor Nina L. Khrushcheva, "You can't even turn the TV on and go to the movies without reference to Russians as horrible." [127] The portrayals of Latin Americans in film and print media are restricted to a narrow set of characters. Latin Americans are largely depicted as sexualized figures such as the Latino macho or the Latina vixen, gang members, (illegal) immigrants, or entertainers. By comparison, they are rarely portrayed as working professionals, business leaders or politicians. [98]

In Hollywood films, there are several Latin American stereotypes that have historically been used. Some examples are El Bandido, the Halfbreed Harlot, The Male Buffoon, The Female Clown, The Latin Lover, The Dark Lady, The Wise Old Man, and The Poor Peon. Many Hispanic characters in Hollywood films consists of one or more of these basic stereotypes, but it has been rare to view Latin American actors representing characters outside of this stereotypical criteria. [128]

Media stereotypes of women first emerged in the early 20th century. Various stereotypic depictions or "types" of women appeared in magazines, including Victorian ideals of femininity, the New Woman, the Gibson Girl, the femme fatale, and the Flapper. [88] [129]

Stereotypes are also common in video games, with women being portrayed as stereotypes such as the "damsel in distress" or as sexual objects (see Gender representation in video games). [130] Studies show that minorities are portrayed most often in stereotypical roles such as athletes and gangsters [131] (see Race and video games).

In literature and art, stereotypes are clichéd or predictable characters or situations. Throughout history, storytellers have drawn from stereotypical characters and situations to immediately connect the audience with new tales. [132]

Role in sports

Female athletes encounter various pressures and stereotypes, which have significant psychological consequences. These stereotypes give rise to challenges in athletes' lives, including diminished self-esteem, leading to more profound psychological impacts.

Female athletes have made considerable strides in overcoming obstacles. They have transitioned from being unable to compete competitively due to biological misconceptions to having equal opportunities as male athletes, thanks to Title IX. [133] Today, there is greater societal acceptance of female athletes. However, the intersection of being a female athlete adds additional pressures. Not only are they expected to excel in competition, but they are also required to conform to societal expectations of femininity. Furthermore, female athletes often face scrutiny and criticism regarding their appearance compared to non-athletic women.

Young athletes, in particular, confront an intensified amount of pressure, leading some to quit sports because it is no longer enjoyable and the implications of being a young female athlete become overwhelming. They are unfairly labeled as gay or delicate and subjected to derogatory comments such as "like a girl." Additionally, they grapple with body image concerns that can give rise to severe health issues. Even specific sports contribute to the scrutiny female athletes face, with criticism directed at the uniforms required for competition. [133]

The proliferation of stereotypes in women's sports has resulted in a decline in female participation. These social stigmas, including being labeled as gay or delicate, and the expectation to play in a manner deemed "like a girl," have contributed to body image issues, eating disorders, and depression among numerous female athletes.

See also

Gender

Examples of stereotypes

Cultural and ethnic
Sexuality related
Other

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Prejudice</span> Attitudes based on preconceived categories

Prejudice can be an affective feeling towards a person based on their perceived group membership. The word is often used to refer to a preconceived evaluation or classification of another person based on that person's perceived personal characteristics, such as political affiliation, sex, gender, gender identity, beliefs, values, social class, age, disability, religion, sexuality, race, ethnicity, language, nationality, culture, complexion, beauty, height, body weight, occupation, wealth, education, criminality, sport-team affiliation, music tastes or other perceived characteristics.

The out-group homogeneity effect is the perception of out-group members as more similar to one another than are in-group members, e.g. "they are alike; we are diverse". Perceivers tend to have impressions about the diversity or variability of group members around those central tendencies or typical attributes of those group members. Thus, outgroup stereotypicality judgments are overestimated, supporting the view that out-group stereotypes are overgeneralizations. The term "outgroup homogeneity effect", "outgroup homogeneity bias" or "relative outgroup homogeneity" have been explicitly contrasted with "outgroup homogeneity" in general, the latter referring to perceived outgroup variability unrelated to perceptions of the ingroup.

In-group favoritism, sometimes known as in-group–out-group bias, in-group bias, intergroup bias, or in-group preference, is a pattern of favoring members of one's in-group over out-group members. This can be expressed in evaluation of others, in allocation of resources, and in many other ways.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">In-group and out-group</span> Sociological notions

In social psychology and sociology, an in-group is a social group to which a person psychologically identifies as being a member. By contrast, an out-group is a social group with which an individual does not identify. People may for example identify with their peer group, family, community, sports team, political party, gender, sexual orientation, religion, or nation. It has been found that the psychological membership of social groups and categories is associated with a wide variety of phenomena.

In psychology and other social sciences, the contact hypothesis suggests that intergroup contact under appropriate conditions can effectively reduce prejudice between majority and minority group members. Following WWII and the desegregation of the military and other public institutions, policymakers and social scientists had turned an eye towards the policy implications of interracial contact. Of them, social psychologist Gordon Allport united early research in this vein under intergroup contact theory.

Social identity is the portion of an individual's self-concept derived from perceived membership in a relevant social group.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Black sheep</span> Idiom for oddness or disreputability

In the English language, black sheep is an idiom that describes a member of a group who is different from the rest, especially a family member who does not fit in. The term stems from sheep whose fleece is colored black rather than the more common white; these sheep stand out in the flock and their wool is worth less as it will not dye.

Aversive racism is a social scientific theory proposed by Samuel L. Gaertner & John F. Dovidio (1986), according to which negative evaluations of racial/ethnic minorities are realized by a persistent avoidance of interaction with other racial and ethnic groups. As opposed to traditional, overt racism, which is characterized by overt hatred for and discrimination against racial/ethnic minorities, aversive racism is characterized by more complex, ambivalent expressions and attitudes nonetheless with prejudicial views towards other races. Aversive racism arises from unconscious personal beliefs taught during childhood. Subtle racist behaviors are usually targeted towards African Americans. Workplace discrimination is one of the best examples of aversive racism. Biased beliefs on how minorities act and think affect how individuals interact with minority members.

Self-categorization theory is a theory in social psychology that describes the circumstances under which a person will perceive collections of people as a group, as well as the consequences of perceiving people in group terms. Although the theory is often introduced as an explanation of psychological group formation, it is more accurately thought of as general analysis of the functioning of categorization processes in social perception and interaction that speaks to issues of individual identity as much as group phenomena. It was developed by John Turner and colleagues, and along with social identity theory it is a constituent part of the social identity approach. It was in part developed to address questions that arose in response to social identity theory about the mechanistic underpinnings of social identification.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Ultimate attribution error</span>

The ultimate attribution error is a type of attribution error which describes how attributions of outgroup behavior are more negative than ingroup behavior. As a cognitive bias, the error results in negative outgroup behavior being more likely to be attributed to factors internal and specific to the actor, such as personality, and the attribution of negative ingroup behavior to external factors such as luck or circumstance. The bias reinforces negative stereotypes and prejudice about the outgroup and favouritism of the ingroup through positive stereotypes. The theory also extends to the bias that positive acts performed by ingroup members are more likely a result of their personality.

In social psychology, collective narcissism is the tendency to exaggerate the positive image and importance of a group to which one belongs. The group may be defined by ideology, race, political beliefs/stance, religion, sexual orientation, social class, language, nationality, employment status, education level, cultural values, or any other ingroup. While the classic definition of narcissism focuses on the individual, collective narcissism extends this concept to similar excessively high opinions of a person's social group, and suggests that a group can function as a narcissistic entity.

An implicit bias or implicit stereotype is the pre-reflective attribution of particular qualities by an individual to a member of some social out group.

The imagined contact hypothesis is an extension of the contact hypothesis, a theoretical proposition centred on the psychology of prejudice and prejudice reduction. It was originally developed by Richard J. Crisp and Rhiannon N. Turner and proposes that the mental simulation, or imagining, of a positive social interaction with an outgroup member can lead to increased positive attitudes, greater desire for social contact, and improved group dynamics. Empirical evidence supporting the imagined contact hypothesis demonstrates its effectiveness at improving explicit and implicit attitudes towards and intergroup relations with a wide variety of stigmatized groups including religious minorities, the mentally ill, ethnic minorities, sexual minorities, and obese individuals. Researchers have identified a number of factors that influence the effectiveness of the imagined contact hypothesis including vividness of the imagery and how typical the imagined outgroup individual is. While some researchers question the effectiveness of the imagined contact hypothesis, empirical evidence does suggest it is effective at improving attitudes towards outgroups.

Intergroup anxiety is the social phenomenon identified by Walter and Cookie Stephan in 1985 that describes the ambiguous feelings of discomfort or anxiety when interacting with members of other groups. Such emotions also constitute intergroup anxiety when one is merely anticipating interaction with members of an outgroup. Expectations that interactions with foreign members of outgroups will result in an aversive experience is believed to be the cause of intergroup anxiety, with an affected individual being anxious or unsure about a number of issues. Methods of reducing intergroup anxiety and stress including facilitating positive intergroup contact.

Intergroup relations refers to interactions between individuals in different social groups, and to interactions taking place between the groups themselves collectively. It has long been a subject of research in social psychology, political psychology, and organizational behavior.

In social psychology, a metastereotype is a stereotype that members of one group have about the way in which they are stereotypically viewed by members of another group. In other words, it is a stereotype about a stereotype. They have been shown to have adverse effects on individuals that hold them, including on their levels of anxiety in interracial conversations. Meta-stereotypes held by African Americans regarding the stereotypes White Americans have about them have been found to be largely both negative and accurate. People portray meta-stereotypes of their ingroup more positively when talking to a member of an outgroup than to a fellow member of their ingroup.

Diversity ideology refers to individual beliefs regarding the nature of intergroup relations and how to improve them in culturally diverse societies. A large amount of scientific literature in social psychology studies diversity ideologies as prejudice reduction strategies, most commonly in the context of racial groups and interracial interactions. In research studies on the effects of diversity ideology, social psychologists have either examined endorsement of a diversity ideology as individual difference or used situational priming designs to activate the mindset of a particular diversity ideology. It is consistently shown that diversity ideologies influence how individuals perceive, judge and treat cultural outgroup members. Different diversity ideologies are associated with distinct effects on intergroup relations, such as stereotyping and prejudice, intergroup equality, and intergroup interactions from the perspectives of both majority and minority group members. Beyond intergroup consequences, diversity ideology also has implications on individual outcomes, such as whether people are open to cultural fusion and foreign ideas, which in turn predict creativity.

In social psychology, social projection is the psychological process through which an individual expects behaviors or attitudes of others to be similar to their own. Social projection occurs between individuals as well as across ingroup and outgroup contexts in a variety of domains. Research has shown that aspects of social categorization affect the extent to which social projection occurs. Cognitive and motivational approaches have been used to understand the psychological underpinnings of social projection as a phenomenon. Cognitive approaches emphasize social projection as a heuristic, while motivational approaches contextualize social projection as a means to feel connected to others. In contemporary research on social projection, researchers work to further distinguish between the effects of social projection and self-stereotyping on the individual’s perception of others.

An empathy gap, sometimes referred to as an empathy bias, is a breakdown or reduction in empathy where it might otherwise be expected to occur. Empathy gaps may occur due to a failure in the process of empathizing or as a consequence of stable personality characteristics, and may reflect either a lack of ability or motivation to empathize.

Social identity threat is a theory in social psychology derived from social identity theory to explain the different types of threats that arise from group identity being threatened as opposed to personal identity. This theory distinguishes between four distinct types of social identity threats: categorization threat, distinctiveness threat, threats to the value of social identity, and acceptance threat. Each type is associated with particular social contexts that make the threats more or less likely to occur. This theory emphasizes how the level of commitment with the social identity shapes the nature of the threat experienced.

References

  1. Critchfield, Austi (15 November 2017). "This is Why Doughnuts Are Associated With Cops". Spoon University. Archived from the original on 19 November 2017. Retrieved 2 November 2020. The funny thing is, the whole cop and doughnuts thing is completely out of date – today, an officer could just as easily swing through a McDonald's drive through as he could a Krispy Kreme. Yet, the stereotype endures, even though police aren't seen at doughnut shops in nearly the numbers they used to have been. In a way, it's become a stereotype of itself, which is pretty meta.
  2. Cardwell, Mike (1999). Dictionary of Psychology. Chicago: Fitzroy Dearborn. ISBN   9781579580643.
  3. Meyer, David G. (2008). Intuition: Its Powers and Perils. Yale University Press. pp. 114–115. ISBN   9780300130270.
  4. "Frequently Asked Questions". Implicit.Harvard.edu. Retrieved 14 April 2018.
  5. 1 2 Hinton, Perry (1 September 2017). "Implicit stereotypes and the predictive brain: Cognition and culture in "biased" person perception". Palgrave Communications. 3 (1): 1–9. doi: 10.1057/palcomms.2017.86 . S2CID   54036730.
  6. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 McGarty, Craig; Yzerbyt, Vincent Y.; Spears, Russel (2002). "Social, cultural and cognitive factors in stereotype formation" (PDF). Stereotypes as explanations: The formation of meaningful beliefs about social groups. Cambridge University Press. pp. 1–15. ISBN   9780521800471.
  7. Judd, Charles M.; Park, Bernadette (1993). "Definition and assessment of accuracy in social stereotypes". Psychological Review . 100 (1): 109–128. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.100.1.109. PMID   8426877.
  8. 1 2 3 4 5 Cox, William T. L.; Abramson, Lyn Y.; Devine, Patricia G.; Hollon, Steven D. (September 2012). "Stereotypes, Prejudice, and Depression: The Integrated Perspective". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 7 (5): 427–449. doi:10.1177/1745691612455204. PMID   26168502. S2CID   1512121.
  9. Chakkarath, Pradeep (March 2010). "Stereotypes in social psychology: The 'West-East' differentiation as a reflection of Western traditions of thought". Psychological Studies. 55 (1): 18–25. doi:10.1007/s12646-010-0002-9. S2CID   144061506.
  10. Liddell, Henry George; Scott, Robert. "στερεός". A Greek-English Lexicon via Perseus Digital Library.
  11. Liddell, Henry George; Scott, Robert. "τύπος". A Greek-English Lexicon via Perseus Digital Library.
  12. Harper, Douglas. "stereotype – Origin and meaning". Online Etymology Dictionary. Retrieved 22 October 2017.
  13. Kleg, Milton (1993). Hate, Prejudice and Racism . Albany: State University of New York Press. pp.  135–137. ISBN   9780585054919.
  14. Vega, Tanzina. "Working while brown: What discrimination looks like now". CNNMoney. Retrieved 26 March 2018.
  15. 1 2 3 Fiske, Susan T. (1998). "Stereotyping, ⁰Prejudice, and Discrimination". In Gilbert, Daniel T.; Fiske, Susan T.; Lindzey, Gardner (eds.). The Handbook of Social Psychology. Vol. 2 (4th ed.). Boston, Massachusetts: McGraw-Hill. p. 357. ISBN   9780195213768.
  16. 1 2 3 4 Denmark, Florence L. (2010). "Prejudice and Discrimination". In Weiner, Irving B.; Craigheaid, W. Edward (eds.). The Corsini Encyclopedia of Psychology. Vol. 3 (4th ed.). Hoboken, New Jersey: John Wiley. p. 1277. ISBN   9780470479216.
  17. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Katz, Daniel; Braly, Kenneth W. (1935). "Racial prejudice and racial stereotypes". The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology . 30 (2): 175–193. doi:10.1037/h0059800.
  18. 1 2 Oakes, P. J.; Haslam, S. A.; Turner, J. C. (1994). Stereotyping and Social Reality. Oxford: Blackwell.
  19. 1 2 3 Devine, Patricia G. (1989). "Stereotypes and Prejudice: Their Automatic and Controlled Components". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 56 (1): 5–18. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.56.1.5. S2CID   33975027.
  20. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 Tajfel, Henri (1981). "Social stereotypes and social groups". In Turner, John C.; Giles, Howard (eds.). Intergroup behaviour. Oxford: Blackwell. pp. 144–167. ISBN   9780631117117.
  21. 1 2 Tilcsik, András (2011). "Pride and Prejudice: Employment Discrimination against Openly Gay Men in the United States". American Journal of Sociology . 117 (2): 586–626. doi:10.1086/661653. hdl: 1807/34998 . PMID   22268247. S2CID   23542996.
  22. 1 2 Operario, Don; Fiske, Susan T. (2003). "Stereotypes: Content, Structures, Processes, and Context". In Brown, Rupert; Gaertner, Samuel L. (eds.). Blackwell Handbook of Social Psychology: Intergroup Processes. Malden, Massachusetts: Blackwell. pp. 22–44. ISBN   9781405106542.
  23. 1 2 Fiske, Susan T.; Cuddy, Amy J. C.; Glick, Peter; Xu, Jun (2002). "A Model of (Often Mixed) Stereotype Content: Competence and Warmth Respectively Follow From Perceived Status and Competition" (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology . 82 (6): 878–902. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.320.4001 . doi:10.1037/0022-3514.82.6.878. PMID   12051578. S2CID   17057403.
  24. 1 2 3 Cuddy, Amy J. C.; Fiske, Susan T. (2002). "Doddering But Dear: Process, Content, and Function in Stereotyping of Older Persons". In Nelson, Todd D. (ed.). Ageism: Stereotyping and Prejudice against Older Persons. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press. pp. 7–8. ISBN   9780262140775.
  25. Dovidio, John F.; Gaertner, Samuel L. (2010). "Intergroup Bias". In Susan T., Fiske; Gilbert, Daniel T.; Lindzey, Gardner (eds.). Handbook of Social Psychology. Vol. 2 (5th ed.). Hooboken, New Jersey: John Wiley. p.  1085. ISBN   9780470137475.
  26. Cuddy, Amy J. C.; Fiske, Susan T.; Kwan, Virginia S. Y.; Glick, Peter; Demoulin, Stéphanie; Leyens, Jacques-Philippe; Bond, Michael Harris; Croizet, Jean-Claude; Ellemers, Naomi; Sleebos, Ed; Htun, Tin Tin; Kim, Hyun-Jeong; Maio, Greg; Perry, Judi; Petkova, Kristina; Todorov, Valery; Rodríguez-Bailón, Rosa; Morales, Elena; Moya, Miguel; Palacios, Marisol; Smith, Vanessa; Perez, Rolando; Vala, Jorge; Ziegler, Rene (March 2009). "Stereotype content model across cultures: Towards universal similarities and some differences". British Journal of Social Psychology. 48 (1): 1–33. doi:10.1348/014466608X314935. PMC   3912751 . PMID   19178758.
  27. 1 2 Koch, A.; Imhoff, R.; Dotsch, R.; Unkelbach, C.; & Alves, H. (2016). "The ABC of Stereotypes About Groups: Agency/Socioeconomic Success, Conservative-Progressive Beliefs, and Communion". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 110, 675–709. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000046
  28. "Groups' warmth is a personal matter: Understanding consensus on stereotype dimensions reconciles adversarial models of social evaluation". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 89, 103995. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2020.103995
  29. Koch, A.; Yzerbyt, V.; Abele, A.; Ellemers, N.; & Fiske, S. T. (2021). "Chapter One – Social evaluation: Comparing models across interpersonal, intragroup, intergroup, several-group, and many-group contexts". In: Gawronski, B. (ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 63, pp. 1–68). Academic Press. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.aesp.2020.11.001
  30. Dragon, P.; Holland, R.; Szumowska, E.; Suska, A.; Kossowska, M.; & Wigboldus, D. (2022). "Explaining the relationship between agency and communion by means of lay personality theories and connectionist modelling". https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/g847b"
  31. Cao, Y. T.; Sotnikova, A.; Daum'e, H.; Rudinger, R.; & Zou, L. X. (2022). "Theory-grounded Measurement of U.S. Social Stereotypes in English Language Models". North American Chapter of the Association for Computational Linguistics.
  32. Koch, A.; Dorrough, A.; Glöckner, A.; & Imhoff, R. (2020). "The ABC of society: Perceived similarity in agency/socioeconomic success and conservative-progressive beliefs increases intergroup cooperation". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 90, 103996. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2020.103996
  33. Macrae, C. N.; Stangor, C.; Hewstone, M., eds. (1995). Stereotypes and Stereotyping. p. 4.
  34. McGarty, Craig; Spears, Russel; Yzerbyt, Vincent Y. (2002). "Conclusion: stereotypes are selective, variable and contested explanations". Stereotypes as explanations: The formation of meaningful beliefs about social groups . Cambridge University Press. pp.  186–199. ISBN   9780521800471.
  35. 1 2 3 Allport, Gordon W. (1954). The Nature of Prejudice . Cambridge, Massachusetts: Addison-Wesley. p.  189. ISBN   9780201001754.
  36. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Haslam, S. Alexander; Turner, John C.; Oakes, Penelope J.; Reynolds, Katherine J.; Doosje, Bertjan (2002). "From personal pictures in the head to collective tools in the world: How shared stereotypes allow groups to represent and change social reality". Stereotypes as Explanations. pp. 157–185. doi:10.1017/CBO9780511489877.009. ISBN   9780521804820.
  37. Cadinu, Mara; Latrofa, Marcella; Carnaghi, Andrea (2013). "Comparing Self-stereotyping with In-group-stereotyping and Out-group-stereotyping in Unequal-status Groups: The Case of Gender". Self and Identity. 12 (6): 582–596. doi:10.1080/15298868.2012.712753. S2CID   145053003.
  38. Burkley, Melissa; Blanton, Hart (2008). "Endorsing a negative in-group stereotype as a self-protective strategy: Sacrificing the group to save the self". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 44: 37–49. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2007.01.008.
  39. Aronson, E.; Wilson, T. D.; Akert, R. M. (2010). Social Psychology (7th ed.). New York: Pearson.
  40. Mackie, Diane M.; Hamilton, David L.; Susskind, Joshua; Rosselli, Francine (1996). "Social Psychological Foundations of Stereotype Formation". In MacRae, C. Neil; Stangor, Charles; Hewstone, Miles (eds.). Stereotypes and Stereotyping. New York: Guilford Press. pp. 48–49. ISBN   9781572300538.
  41. Rogier, Anouk; Yzerbyt, Vincent (1999). "Social attribution, correspondence bias, and the emergence of stereotypes" (PDF). Swiss Journal of Psychology. 58 (4): 233–240. doi:10.1024//1421-0185.58.4.233. Archived from the original (PDF) on 29 July 2013. Retrieved 5 April 2013.
  42. Nier, Jason A.; Bajaj, Priya; McLean, Meghan C.; Schwartz, Elizabeth (2012). "Group status, perceptions of agency, and the correspondence bias: Attributional processes in the formation of stereotypes about high and low status groups". Group Processes & Intergroup Relations . 16 (4): 476–487. doi:10.1177/1368430212454925. S2CID   145399213.
  43. Marvel, John D. (30 January 2015). "Unconscious Bias in Citizens' Evaluations of Public Sector Performance". Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory: muu053. doi: 10.1093/jopart/muu053 . ISSN   1053-1858.
  44. Döring, Matthias; Willems, Jurgen (22 September 2021). "Processing stereotypes: Professionalism confirmed or disconfirmed by sector affiliation?". International Public Management Journal. 26 (2): 221–239. doi: 10.1080/10967494.2021.1971125 . ISSN   1096-7494. S2CID   239261451.
  45. Willems, Jurgen (2020). "Public servant stereotypes: It is not (at) all about being lazy, greedy and corrupt". Public Administration. 98 (4): 807–823. doi: 10.1111/padm.12686 . ISSN   1467-9299. S2CID   221760839.
  46. 1 2 3 Mullen, Brian; Johnson, Craig (1990). "Distinctiveness-based illusory correlations and stereotyping: A meta-analytic integration". British Journal of Social Psychology . 29 (1): 11–28. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8309.1990.tb00883.x.
  47. Meiser, Thorsten (2006). "Contingency Learning and Biased Group Impressions". In Fiedler, Klaus; Justin, Peter (eds.). Information Sampling and Adaptive Cognition. Cambridge University Press. pp. 183–209. ISBN   9780521831598.
  48. 1 2 3 4 Hamilton, David L.; Gifford, Robert K. (1976). "Illusory correlation in interpersonal perception: A cognitive basis of stereotypic judgments". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology . 12 (4): 392–407. doi:10.1016/S0022-1031(76)80006-6.
  49. 1 2 Berndsen, Mariëtte; Spears, Russel; van der Pligt, Joop; McGarty, Craig (2002). "Illusory correlation and stereotype formation: making sense of group differences and cognitive biases". In McGarty, Craig; Yzerbyt, Vincent Y.; Spears, Russel (eds.). Stereotypes as explanations: The formation of meaningful beliefs about social groups. Cambridge University Press. pp. 90–110. ISBN   9780521800471.
  50. 1 2 Stroessner, Steven J.; Plaks, Jason E. (2001). "Illusory Correlation and Stereotype Formation: Tracing the Arc of Research Over a Quarter Century". In Moskowitz, Gordon B. (ed.). Cognitive Social Psychology: The Princeton Symposium on the Legacy and Future of Social Cognition. Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 247–259. ISBN   9780805834147.
  51. Ajrouch, Kristine J.; Antonucci, Toni C.; Janevic, Mary R. (1 March 2001). "Social Networks Among Blacks and Whites: The Interaction Between Race and Age". The Journals of Gerontology. 56 (2): S112–S118. doi: 10.1093/geronb/56.2.S112 . PMID   11245365.
  52. Moskowitz, Gordon B. (2005). Social Cognition: Understanding Self and Others. New York: Guilford Press. p. 182. ISBN   9781593850852.
  53. 1 2 3 McConnell, Allen R.; Sherman, Steven J.; Hamilton, David L. (1994). "Illusory correlation in the perception of groups: an extension of the distinctiveness-based account" (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 67 (3): 414–429. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.67.3.414. PMID   7965600.
  54. 1 2 Katz, Daniel; Braley, Kenneth (1933). "Racial stereotypes of one hundred college students". The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 28 (3): 280–290. doi:10.1037/h0074049.
  55. Meenes, Max (1943). "A Comparison of Racial Stereotypes of 1935 and 1942". Journal of Social Psychology . 17 (2): 327–336. doi:10.1080/00224545.1943.9712287.
  56. Haslam, S. Alexander; Turner, John C.; Oakes, Penelope J.; McGarty, Craig; Hayes, Brett K. (1992). "Context-dependent variation in social stereotyping 1: The effects of intergroup relations as mediated by social change and frame of reference". European Journal of Social Psychology. 22 (1): 3–20. doi:10.1002/ejsp.2420220104.
  57. Palmer, Wayne (2019). "International Migration and Stereotype Formation: Indonesian Migrants in Hong Kong". Journal of International Migration and Integration. 21 (3): 731–744. doi:10.1007/s12134-019-00680-1. ISSN   1488-3473. S2CID   195363006.
  58. Devine, Patricia G.; Monteith, Margo J. (1999). "Automaticty and Control in Stereotyping". In Chaiken, Shelly; Trope, Yaacov (eds.). Dual-Process Theories in Social Psychology. New York: Guilford Press. pp. 341–342. ISBN   9781572304215.
  59. 1 2 Bargh, John A. (1994). "The Four Horsemen of Automaticity: Awareness, Intention, Efficiency, Control in Social Cognition". In Wyer, Robert S.; Srull, Thomas K. (eds.). Handbook of Social Cognition. Vol. 2 (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Earlbaum. p.  21. ISBN   9780805810561.
  60. Banaji, Mahzarin R.; Hardin, Curtis D. (1996). "Automatic Stereotyping" (PDF). Psychological Science . 7 (3): 136–141. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1996.tb00346.x. S2CID   207858486. Archived from the original (PDF) on 21 May 2013. Retrieved 12 January 2013.
  61. Perdue, Charles W.; Gurtman, Michael B. (1990). "Evidence for the automaticity of ageism". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 26 (3): 199–216. doi:10.1016/0022-1031(90)90035-K.
  62. Brown, Rupert (2010). Prejudice: Its Social Psychology (2nd ed.). Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell. p. 88. ISBN   9781405113069.
  63. Lepore, Lorella; Brown, Rupert (1997). "Category and Stereotype Activation: Is Prejudice Inevitable?" (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 72 (2): 275–287. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.72.2.275. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2 January 2014.
  64. Kawakami, Kerry; Dovidio, John F.; Moll, Jasper; Hermsen, Sander; Russin, Abby (2000). "Just say no (to stereotyping): Effects of training in the negation of stereotypic associations on stereotype activation". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 78 (5): 871–888. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.5.871. PMID   10821195.
  65. 1 2 Gawronski, Bertram; Deutsch, Roland; Mbirkou, Sawsan; Seibt, Beate; Strack, Fritz (2008). "When 'Just Say No' is not enough: Affirmation versus negation training and the reduction of automatic stereotype activation". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 44 (2): 370–377. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2006.12.004.
  66. Wheeler, S. Christian; Petty, Richard E. (2001). "The Effects of Stereotype Activation on Behavior: A Review of Possible Mechanisms" (PDF). Psychological Bulletin. 127 (6): 797–826. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.127.6.797. PMID   11726072 . Retrieved 10 June 2013.
  67. Devos, Thierry; Huynh, Que-Lam; Banaji, Mahzarin D. (2012). "Implicit self and identity". In Leary, Mark R.; Tangney, June Price (eds.). Handbook of self and identity (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press. pp. 164–165. ISBN   9781462503056.
  68. Dijksterhuis, Ap (2001). "Automatic social influence: The perception-behavior links as an explanatory mechanism for behavior matching". In Forgas, Joseph P.; Williams, Kipling D. (eds.). Social influence: direct and indirect processes. Philadelphia: Psychology Press. pp. 99–100. ISBN   9781841690391.
  69. Operario, Din; Fiske, Susan T. (2001). "Causes and Consequences of Stereotypes in Organizations". In London, Manuel (ed.). How People Evaluate Others in Organizations. Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum. p.  2012. ISBN   9780805836127.
  70. Tschanz, Joann T.; Andersen, Aaron (2011). "Alzheimer's Dementia". Encyclopedia of Clinical Neuropsychology. pp. 99–105. doi:10.1007/978-0-387-79948-3_489. ISBN   9780387799476.
  71. Bargh, John A.; Chen, Mark; Burrows, Lara (1996). "Automaticity of Social Behavior: Direct Effects of Trait Construct and Stereotype Activation on Action" (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 71 (2): 230–244. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.320.8563 . doi:10.1037/0022-3514.71.2.230. PMID   8765481. S2CID   6654763. Archived from the original (PDF) on 9 October 2013. Retrieved 10 June 2013.
  72. Correll, Joshua; Park, Bernadette; Judd, Charles M.; Wittenbrink, Bernd (2002). "The Police Officer's Dilemma: Using Ethnicity to Disambiguate Potentially Threatening Individuals". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 83 (6): 1314–1329. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.466.7243 . doi:10.1037/0022-3514.83.6.1314. PMID   12500813.
  73. Lee, Yueh-Ting; Jussim, Lee J.; McCauley, Clark R., eds. (September 1995). Stereotype Accuracy: Toward Appreciating Group Differences. American Psychological Association. ISBN   9781557983077.
  74. Jussim, Lee; Cain, Thomas R.; Crawford, Jarret T.; Harber, Kent; Cohen, Florette (2009). "The unbearable accuracy of stereotypes". In Nelson, Todd D. (ed.). Handbook of prejudice, stereotyping, and discrimination . New York: Psychology Press. pp.  199–227. ISBN   9780805859522.
  75. Terracciano, A.; Abdel-Khalek, A. M.; Adám, N.; Adamovová, L.; Ahn, C. K.; Ahn, H. N.; Alansari, B. M.; Alcalay, L.; Allik, J. (2005). "National Character Does Not Reflect Mean Personality Trait Levels in 49 Cultures". Science . 310 (5745): 96–100. Bibcode:2005Sci...310...96T. doi:10.1126/science.1117199. PMC   2775052 . PMID   16210536.
  76. Mackie, Marlene. "Arriving at "truth" by definition: The case of stereotype inaccuracy." Social problems 20, no. 4 (1973): 431–447.
  77. 1 2 Zemore, Sarah E.; Fiske, Susan T.; Kim, Hyun-Jeong (2000). "Gender Stereotypes and the Dynamics of Social Interaction". In Eckes, Thomas; Trautner, Hanns Martin (eds.). The Developmental Social Psychology of Gender. Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 229–230. ISBN   9780585300658.
  78. 1 2 Crocker, Jennifer; Major, Brenda; Stelle, Claude (1998). "Social Stigma". In Gilbert, Daniel T.; Fiske, Susan T.; Lindzey, Gardner (eds.). The Handbook of Social Psychology. Vol. 2 (4th ed.). Oxford University Press. pp. 519–521. ISBN   9780195213768.
  79. Whiteley, Bernard E.; Kite, Mary E. (2010). The Psychology of Prejudice and Discrimination (2nd ed.). Belmont, California: Wadsworth / Cengage Learning. pp. 428–435. ISBN   9780495599647.
  80. Crocker, Jennifer; Voelkl, Kristin; Testa, Maria; Major, Brenda (1991). "Social stigma: The affective consequences of attributional ambiguity". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology . 60 (2): 218–228. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.60.2.218.
  81. Osborne, Jason W. (2007). "Linking Stereotype Threat and Anxiety". Educational Psychology. 27 (1): 135–154. doi:10.1080/01443410601069929. S2CID   144555070.
  82. Quinn, Diane M.; Kallen, Rachel W.; Spencer, Steven J. (2010). "Stereotype Threat". In Hewstone, Miles; Glick, Peter; Dovidio, John F.; Esses, Victoria M. (eds.). The SAGE Handbook of Prejudice, Stereotyping and Discrimination. Thousand Oaks, California: SAGE Publications. pp. 379–394. ISBN   9781412934534.
  83. Inzlicht, Michael; Tullett, Alexa M.; Gutsell, Jennifer N. (2012). "Stereotype Threat Spillover: The Short- and Long-term Effects of Coping with Threats to Social Identity". In Inzlicht, Michael; Schmader, Toni (eds.). Stereotype Threat: Theory, Process, and Application. New York: Oxford University Press. p. 108. ISBN   9780199732449.
  84. Aronson, Joshua; Stelle, Claude M. (2005). "Chapter 24: Stereotypes and the Fragility of Academic Competence, Motivation, and Self-Concept". In Elliot, Andrew J.; Dweck, Carol S. (eds.). Handbook of Competence and Motivation. New York: Guilford Press. pp. 436, 443. ISBN   9781593851231.
  85. Steele, Claude M.; Aronson, Joshua (November 1995). "Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans" (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 69 (5): 797–811. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.797. PMID   7473032. S2CID   4665022.
  86. Stone, Jeff; Lynch, Christian I.; Sjomeling, Mike; Darley, John M. (1999). "Stereotype threat effects on Black and White athletic performance". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 77 (6): 1213–1227. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.687.5039 . doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.6.1213. S2CID   14568298.
  87. Maass, Anne; D'Ettole, Claudio; Cadinu, Mara (2008). "Checkmate? The role of gender stereotypes in the ultimate intellectual sport" (PDF). European Journal of Social Psychology . 38 (2): 231–245. doi:10.1002/ejsp.440. S2CID   144308852. Archived from the original (PDF) on 20 November 2012. Retrieved 5 September 2012.
  88. Gupta, V. K.; Bhawe, N. M. (2007). "The Influence of Proactive Personality and Stereotype Threat on Women's Entrepreneurial Intentions". Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies . 13 (4): 73–85. doi:10.1177/10717919070130040901. S2CID   145318243.
  89. Ganley, C. M.; Mingle, L. A.; Ryan, A. M.; Ryan, K.; Vasilyeva, M.; Perry, M. (October 2013). "An examination of stereotype threat effects on girls' mathematics performance" (PDF). Developmental Psychology. 49 (10): 1886–1897. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.353.4436 . doi:10.1037/a0031412. PMID   23356523. Archived from the original (PDF) on 19 July 2014.
  90. Stoet, G.; Geary, D. C. (2012). "Can stereotype threat explain the gender gap in mathematics performance and achievement?". Review of General Psychology. 16: 93–102. doi:10.1037/a0026617. S2CID   145724069. Pdf. Archived 12 January 2016 at the Wayback Machine
  91. Flore, Paulette C.; Wicherts, Jelte M. (2014). "Does stereotype threat influence performance of girls in stereotyped domains? A meta-analysis". Journal of School Psychology. 53 (1): 25–44. doi:10.1016/j.jsp.2014.10.002. ISSN   0022-4405. PMID   25636259. S2CID   206516995.
  92. Sackett, P. R.; Hardison, C. M.; Cullen, M. J. (January 2004). "On interpreting stereotype threat as accounting for African American-White differences on cognitive tests" (PDF). The American Psychologist. 59 (1): 7–13. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.59.1.7. PMID   14736315. Archived from the original (PDF) on 4 April 2013.
  93. Pennington, C. R.; Heim, D.; Levy, A. R.; Larkin, D. T. (11 January 2016). "Twenty Years of Stereotype Threat Research: A Review of Psychological Mediators". PLOS ONE. 11 (1): e0146487. Bibcode:2016PLoSO..1146487P. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0146487 . PMC   4713435 . PMID   26752551.
  94. Nguyen, H. H.; Ryan, A. M. (November 2008). "Does stereotype threat affect test performance of minorities and women? A meta-analysis of experimental evidence". The Journal of Applied Psychology. 93 (6): 1314–1334. doi:10.1037/a0012702. PMID   19025250. S2CID   36769821.
  95. Walton, G. M.; Spencer, S. J. (September 2009). "Latent ability: grades and test scores systematically underestimate the intellectual ability of negatively stereotyped students". Psychological Science. 20 (9): 1132–1139. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02417.x . PMID   19656335. S2CID   25810191.
  96. Gentile, A.; Boca, S.; Giammusso, I. (1 November 2018). "'You play like a Woman!' Effects of gender stereotype threat on Women's performance in physical and sport activities: A meta-analysis". Psychology of Sport and Exercise. 39: 95–103. doi:10.1016/j.psychsport.2018.07.013. ISSN   1469-0292. S2CID   149490634.
  97. Lamont, R. A.; Swift, H. J.; Abrams, D. (March 2015). "A review and meta-analysis of age-based stereotype threat: negative stereotypes, not facts, do the damage". Psychology and Aging. 30 (1): 180–193. doi:10.1037/a0038586. PMC   4360754 . PMID   25621742.
  98. 1 2 3 Picho, K.; Rodriguez, A.; Finnie, L. (May 2013). "Exploring the moderating role of context on the mathematics performance of females under stereotype threat: a meta-analysis". The Journal of Social Psychology. 153 (3): 299–333. doi:10.1080/00224545.2012.737380. PMID   23724702. S2CID   45950675.
  99. Liu, S.; Liu, P.; Wang, M.; Zhang, B. (June 2021). "Effectiveness of stereotype threat interventions: A meta-analytic review". The Journal of Applied Psychology. 106 (6): 921–949. doi:10.1037/apl0000770. PMID   32772526. S2CID   221098319.
  100. VanLandingham, H.; Ellison, R. L.; Laique, A.; Cladek, A.; Khan, H.; Gonzalez, C.; Dunn, M. R. (February 2022). "A scoping review of stereotype threat for BIPOC: Cognitive effects and intervention strategies for the field of neuropsychology". The Clinical Neuropsychologist. 36 (2): 503–522. doi:10.1080/13854046.2021.1947388. PMID   34233577. S2CID   235769090.
  101. Kassin, Saul M.; Fein, Steven; Markus, Hazel Rose (2011). Social psychology (8th ed.). Belmont, California: Wadsworth / Cengage Learning. p. 172. ISBN   9780495812401.
  102. Brown, Rupert (2010). Prejudice: Its Social Psychology (2nd ed.). Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell. pp. 94–97. ISBN   9781405113069.
  103. Chen, Mark; Bargh, John A. (1997). "Nonconscious Behavioral Confirmation Processes: The Self-Fulfilling Consequences of Automatic Stereotype Activation" (PDF). Journal of Experimental Social Psychology . 33 (5): 541–560. doi:10.1006/jesp.1997.1329. Archived from the original (PDF) on 22 May 2013. Retrieved 5 April 2013.
  104. Word, Carl O.; Zanna, Mark P.; Cooper, Joel (1974). "The nonverbal mediation of self-fulfilling prophecies in interracial interaction". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 10 (2): 109–120. doi:10.1016/0022-1031(74)90059-6.
  105. Snyder, Mark; Tanke, Elizabeth D.; Berscheid, Ellen (1977). "Social perception and interpersonal behavior: On the self-fulfilling nature of social stereotypes" (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 35 (9): 656–666. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.35.9.656. Archived from the original (PDF) on 17 June 2012.
  106. Kellow, Thomas (February 2008). "The Effects of Stereotypes on the Achievement Gap: Reexamining the Academic Performance of African American High School Students". Journal of Black Psychology. 34: 94–120. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.553.9188 . doi:10.1177/0095798407310537. S2CID   145490359.
  107. Friedman, A. L.; Lyne, S. R. (2001). "The beancounter stereotype: Towards a general model of stereotype generation". Critical Perspectives on Accounting. 12 (4): 423–451. doi:10.1006/cpac.2000.0451.
  108. Jeacle, I.; Miley, F.; Read, A. (2012). "Jokes in popular culture: The characterisation of the accountant". Accounting, Auditing & Accountability Journal.
  109. Banaji, Mahzarin R. (2002). "The Social Psychology of Stereotypes". In Smelser, Neil; Baltes, Paul (eds.). International Encyclopedia of the Social and Behavioral Sciences. New York: Pergamon. pp. 15100–15104. doi:10.1016/B0-08-043076-7/01754-X. ISBN   9780080430768.
  110. Fiske, Susan T.; Lee, Tiane L. (2008). "Stereotypes and prejudice create workplace discrimination". In Brief, Arthur P (ed.). Diversity at Work. New York: Cambridge University Press. pp. 13–52. ISBN   9780521860307.
  111. Agerström, Jens; Rooth, Dan-Olof (2011). "The role of automatic obesity stereotypes in real hiring discrimination". Journal of Applied Psychology. 96 (4): 790–805. doi:10.1037/a0021594. PMID   21280934.
  112. Rudman, Laurie A.; Glick, Peter (2001). "Prescriptive Gender Stereotypes and Backlash toward Agentic Women" (PDF). Journal of Social Issues . 57 (4): 743–762. doi:10.1111/0022-4537.00239. hdl: 2027.42/146421 . S2CID   54219902. Archived from the original (PDF) on 6 November 2012.
  113. Yékú, James (9 September 2020). "Anti-Afropolitan ethics and the performative politics of online scambaiting". Social Dynamics. 46 (2): 240–258. doi:10.1080/02533952.2020.1813943. ISSN   0253-3952. S2CID   222232833.
  114. Sinclair, Stacey; Huntsinger, Jeff (2006). "The Interpersonal Basis of Self-Stereotyping". In Levin, Shana; Van Laar, Colette (eds.). Stigma and Group Inequality: Social Psychological Perspectives. Claremont Symposium on Applied Social Psychology. Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. p. 239. ISBN   9780805844153.
  115. Correll, Shelley J. (2001). "Gender and the career choice process: The role of biased self-assessments" (PDF). American Journal of Sociology. 106 (6): 1691–1730. doi:10.1086/321299. S2CID   142863258. Archived from the original (PDF) on 12 September 2011.
  116. Correll, Shelley J. (2004). "Constraints into Preferences: Gender, Status, and Emerging Career Aspirations" (PDF). American Sociological Review . 69 (1): 93–113. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.520.8370 . doi:10.1177/000312240406900106. S2CID   8735336.
  117. Sinclair, Stacey; Hardin, Curtis D.; Lowery, Brian S. (2006). "Self-Stereotyping in the Context of Multiple Social Identities" (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 90 (4): 529–542. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.90.4.529. PMID   16649853. Archived from the original (PDF) on 27 August 2014.
  118. Sachs, Nicole M.; Veysey, Bonita M.; Rivera, Luis M. (14 November 2017). "Implicit Social Cognitive Processes Underlying Victim Self and Identity: Evidence With College-Aged Adults". Journal of Interpersonal Violence. 36 (3–4): 1256–1282. doi:10.1177/0886260517741625. PMID   29294984. S2CID   206565963.
  119. Hayakawa, S. I. (1950). "Recognizing Stereotypes as Substitutes for Thought". ETC: A Review of General Semantics. 7 (3): 208–210. JSTOR   42581302.
  120. Taylor, Rhonda Harris; Patterson, Lotsee (December 2000). "Using information literacy to promote critical thinking". Teacher Librarian. 28 (2): 9–14. OCLC   425340563.
  121. "Lesson 2 – Stock Characters | BYU Theatre Education Database". tedb.byu.edu. Retrieved 30 March 2018.
  122. Fedorov, Alexander (2015). "Media Stereotypes Analysis in the Classroom at the Student Audience". European Journal of Contemporary Education. 12 (2): 158–162. doi: 10.13187/ejced.2015.12.158 .
  123. Lee, Kevin (January 2008). "'The Little State Department': Hollywood and the MPAA's Influence on U.S. Trade Relations". Northwestern Journal of International Law & Business. 28 (2).
  124. "Will the cliche of the 'Russian baddie' ever leave our screens?". The Guardian. 10 July 2017.
  125. "Russian film industry and Hollywood uneasy with one another." Fox News. 14 October 2014
  126. "5 Hollywood Villains That Prove Russian Stereotypes Are Hard to Kill". The Moscow Times. 9 August 2015.
  127. "Hollywood stereotypes: Why are Russians the bad guys?". BBC News. 5 November 2014.
  128. Berg, Charles (Summer 1990). "Stereotyping in films in general and of the Hispanic in Particular". The Howard Journal of Communications. 2 (3): 294–296. doi:10.1080/10646179009359721.
  129. Coltrane, Scott; Adams, Michele (April 1997). "Work–Family Imagery and Gender Stereotypes: Television and the Reproduction of Difference" . Journal of Vocational Behavior. 50 (2): 323–347. doi:10.1006/jvbe.1996.1575.
  130. Mou, Yi; Peng, Wei (January 2008). "Gender and Racial Stereotypes in Popular Video Games". Handbook of Research on Effective Electronic Gaming in Education. Hershey, Pa.: Information Science Reference. doi:10.4018/9781599048086.ch053. ISBN   9781599048086. OCLC   755871149. Book: doi : 10.4018/978-1-59904-808-6.
  131. Burgess, Melinda; Dill, Karen (15 September 2011). "Playing With Prejudice: The Prevalence and Consequences of Racial Stereotypes in Video Games". Media Psychology. 14 (3): 289–311. doi:10.1080/15213269.2011.596467. S2CID   1416833.
  132. Auracher, Jan; Hirose, Akiko (2017). "The Influence of Reader's Stereotypes on the Assessment of Fictional Characters". Comparative Literature Studies. 54 (4): 795–823. doi:10.5325/complitstudies.54.4.0795. JSTOR   10.5325/complitstudies.54.4.0795. S2CID   148778808.
  133. 1 2 Wartel, Jennifer (5 May 2021). "Stereotypes in Women's Sports and their Psychological Effects". Academic Festival.

Further reading