Medumba phonology

Last updated

Medumba phonology is the way in which the Medumba language is pronounced. Medumba is a Bamileke language of Cameroon; the people who speak it originate from the Nde division of the West Region of the country. It deals with phonetics, phonotactics and their variation across different dialects of Medumba.

Contents

Segments

Initial research on the Medumba segment inventory was conducted by Voorhoeve in the early 1960s, and published in Voorhoeve (1965). [1] He identified 15 vowels and 40 consonants. Not described by Voorhoeve (1965) are the plain and pre-nasalized bilabial trills /ʙ/, /ᵐʙ/, which occur most often before central vowels /ʉ, ə/, which brings the total number of consonants to 42. [2] The following two subsections survey the vowel and consonant inventory.

Vowels

Medumba has a 12 simple vowels, and 5 complex vowels (diphthongs).

Simple vowels

Medumba has 12 phonemic vowels given in the following inventory:

Simple vowels
FrontCentralBack
Closed

(High)

+Advanced Tongue Rootiʉu
-Advanced Tongue Rootɪʊ
Closed

(Non-high)

Mideəo
Low-midɛɔ
Lowaɑ
(adapted from Voorhoeve 1965:320, fn. 6; Voorhoeve 1977:65)
Examples of simple vowels
PhonemeExample
WordIPAGloss
/i/fí[fídə́]'to be arrogant'
/ʉ/fʉ[fʉ́də́]'to fly'
/u/fu[fúbə́]'bedding'
/ɪ/fí[fɪ́də́]'to peel'
/ʊ/fu[fʊ́də́]'hunting, net'
/e/fè[fènə́]'to choke, suffocate'
/ə/fət[fə̀t]'wind'
/o/fo[fógə́]'widowship'
/a/fat[fàt]'head-protector pillow for load-carrying'
/ɑ/fɑ[fɑ́ʔɑ́]'kind of tree'
(adapted from Voorhoeve 1965:327;3.2.1)

Diphthong vowels

Medumba has five phonemic diphthongs.

Diphthongs in Medumba
V1
FrontCentralBack
iʉu
V2aiaʉa
ə
ɑʉɑ
(adapted from Voorhoeve 1965:320, fn. 6)
Examples of diphthongs in Medumba
/ia/fyaŋə

[fiaŋə]

'sort of tree'

/uɑ/c

[cuɑdə]

'to sow, plant'

/ʉa/fʉɑ

[fʉɑgə]

'to blow'

/ʉɑ/fʉɑ

[fʉɑgə]

'to be wild'

/iə/(a)nzwəʔə

/ᶮjʷiəʔə/[ⁿzʷəʔə]

'sort of dance'

(b)və

/iə/[və] L

'architecture'

(c)tsə'tsə

/ciəʔ-tə/[tsəʔtə]

'to collect'

cf.cə'

[cəʔ] H

'servant of chief'

(d)zə

/jiə/[zə] H

'relative pronoun'

cf.yən

[jen] H

'demonstrative pronoun'

(e)mfə

/ᵐfiə/[ᵐfə] H

'oath'

cf.mvə

/ᵐfə/[ᵐvə] H

'on'

(adapted from Voorhoeve 1965:325;3.1.6-7 and 327;3.2.2)

row 1 = orthography; row 2 = [IPA]; row 3 = 'gloss'

Diphthongs involve a combination of a closed (high) vowel (V1) /i,ʉ,u/ with a non-closed (non-high) vowel (V2) /a,ə,ɑ/, as follows:

  • front /i/ combines with front or centre /a/ and /ə/ to form /ia/ and
    • front /i/ does not combine with back /ɑ/, so */iɑ/ is not a possible diphthong
  • central /ʉ/ combines with front or back /a/ and /ɑ/ to form /ʉa/ and /ʉɑ/
    • central /ʉ/ does not combine with central /ə/; so */ʉə/ is not a possible diphthong
  • back /u/ combines with back /ɑ/ to form //
    • back /u/ does not combine non-back vowels, so */ua/ and *// are not possible diphthongs

Consonants

The canonical morpheme in Medumba is a single syllable, either an open CV syllable or a closed CVC syllable (Voorhoeve 1965:319). [1] This morpheme structure constraint has consequences for the consonant inventory. Indeed, a notable property of Medumba is that the number of contrastive consonants differs according to whether one considers consonants in onset position (i.e., consonants that begin a CV or CVC syllable) or consonants in coda position (i.e. consonants that end a CVC syllable). Below, the consonant inventory is introduced, and the distributional differences between coda (C2) and onset (C1) consonants are described.

Medumba has 42 consonants, of which 18 are simplex consonants and 24 are complex consonants.

Simplex consonants

There are 18 simplex consonants in Medumba (Voorhoeve 1965).

Medumba has 18 simple consonants, with three of them (placed in parentheses in the table below) being extremely rare.

Simplex consonants
labialalveolarpalatalvelarglottal
nasalmnɲŋ
stopvoicedbdɟ [lower-alpha 1] ɡ
voicelesst [lower-alpha 1] c [lower-alpha 1] k [lower-alpha 1] ʔ [lower-alpha 2]
trillʙ
fricativevoiced(v) [lower-alpha 1] (z) [lower-alpha 1]
voicelessf [lower-alpha 1] s [lower-alpha 1] (ʃ) [lower-alpha 1]
approximantwj
(adapted from Voorhoeve 1965:320, and Nganmou 1991:62)
  1. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 Occurs only in onset positions.
  2. Occurs only in coda positions.

Note the absence of the following segments:

  • there is a voiced bilabial stop /b/, but no counterpart voiceless bilabial stop */p/
  • there is a voiceless palatal fricative /ʃ/ (with limited distribution), but no counterpart voiced palatal fricative */ʒ/
    • but [ʒ] occurs as an allophone of /ɟ/
  • there are velar nasal and oral stops, but no counterpart velar fricatives */ɣ/ or */x/
    • but [ɣ] occurs as a release in the /ᵑg/~[ᵑgɣ] alternation, and [x] occurs as a release in the /ᵑk/~[ᵑkx] alternation

Complex consonants

There are 24 complex consonants found in Medumba (Voorhoeve 1965:326, section 3.1.9). Complex consonants only occur in onset position.

Complex consonants
labialalveolarpalatalvelar
labialized nasalɲʷŋʷ
stopvoicedɟʷ
voiceless
fricative
pre-nasalized trillᵐʙ
plosivevoicedᵐbⁿdᶮɟᵑg
voicelessⁿtᶮcᵑk
fricativeᵐfⁿs
pre-nasalized
labialized
plosivevoicedᵐbʷᶮɟʷᵑgʷ
voicelessᶮcʷᵑkʷ
fricativeⁿsʷ
(adapted from Voorhoeve 1965:326)

Note the following gaps in the inventory of complex consonants:

  • no labialized labial nasal or voiceless stop: */mʷ/, */pʷ/, */ᵐpʷ/
  • no labialized alveolar (nasal or oral) stop: */nʷ/, */dʷ/, */ⁿdʷ/, */tʷ/, */ⁿtʷ/
  • no labialized voiceless labial, palatal or velar fricative: */fʷ/, */ᵐfʷ/, */ʃʷ/, */ᶮʃʷ/, */xʷ/, */ᵑxʷ/

Final consonants

Of the 40 consonants found in Medumba, only 7 can be coda consonants: the 3 nasal stops /m,n,ŋ/, the 3 counterpart voiced oral stops /b,d,ɡ/, and the glottal stop /ʔ/.

Final consonants and their allophones
Place of Articulation
labialalveolarvelarglottal
Manner of

Articulation

nasal stopmnŋ
oral stopvoicedb ~ pd ~ lɡ ~ ʁ/k
voicelessʔ
(adapted from Voorhoeve 1965:328, sn. 3.3.3)
Final nasals

Final nasals include bilabial /m/, alveolar /n/, and velar /ŋ/.

Examples of final nasals
PhonemeExample
WordIPAGloss
/m/m[cʊm] L(H)'prune'
/n/n L[tɑn] L(H)'cricket'
/ŋ/ŋmɑŋgəm LHL[foŋ.mɑ.ᵑgəm] LHL(H)'sort of ant'
(adapted from Voorhoeve 1965:328;3.3.3)
Final stops

Final stops includes bilabial /b/ (with allophone [p]), alveolar /d/ (with allophones [l,t]), velar /g/ (with allophone [ʁ,k]), and glottal /ʔ/.

Examples of final stops
PhonemeAllophoneExample
WordIPAGloss
/b/[b]
[p]cùupnyɑm[cʊʊp.ɲɑm]'wild cat'
/d/[d]
[l,t]mfətni[ᵐfət.ni] HH'reconciliation ceremony'
/g/[g]
[ʁ,k]ciak[ciak.tə] HH'hairstyle,cap'
/ʔ/[ʔ]
(adapted from Voorhoeve 1965:328;3.3.3)

for examples of consonant allophones, see Danis, Barnes & O'Connor 2012

Initial consonants

The below table presents the inventory of onset consonants and their allophones. The only consonant excluded from onset position is the glottal stop /ʔ/. All other consonants occur in onset position, so there are 39 possible onset consonants. In onset position, nasals may be plain (C) or labialized (). All other consonant types (voiced plosives, voiceless plosives, fricative) occur as plain (C), labialized (), pre-nasalized (ⁿC), or pre-nasalized and labialized (ⁿCʷ). In addition, onset consonants display allophonic variation that is conditioned by the following vowel.

Onset consonants and their allophones
Place of articulation
labialalveolarpalatalvelar
Manner of

articulation

stopnasalCmnɲŋ
ɲʷŋʷ
oralvoicedCb~pd~lɟ~y/z/ʒɡ~ʁ
~bⱽ
[ clarification needed ][ what language is this? ]
ɟʷ~yʷ>/zʷ~w/v
ⁿCᵐbⁿdᶮɟ~ⁿz/ᶮʒᵑg~ᵑgˠ
ⁿCʷᵐbʷᶮɟʷ~ⁿzʷᵑgʷ
voicelessCt~c~ʦ/cʃk~kʰ/kx
~tsʷ/ʃ
ⁿCⁿt~ⁿtʰᶮc~ⁿts/ᶮcʃᵑk~ᵑkʰ/ᵑkx
ⁿCʷᶮcʷ~ᶮʃᵑkʷ
fricativeCfs
ⁿCᵐf~ᵐvⁿs~ⁿz
ⁿCʷⁿsʷ
(adapted from Voorhoeve 1965:326)
Initial labials

Initial labials include:

  • plain consonants (C:/m/, /b/ (with allophone [p]), and /f/ (with allophone [ᵐv])
  • labialized consonants (): /bʷ/ (with allophone [bⱽ][ clarification needed ])
  • pre-nasalized consonants (ⁿC): /ᵐb/ and /ᵐf/
  • pre-nasalized labialized consonants (ⁿCʷ): /ᵐbʷ/
Examples of labial onsets
Cnasal/m/[m]mʉ'

[mʉʔ] H

'lake'(V323;3.1.2)

stop/b/[b]bɑb L

[bɑb] L

'wing' (V323;3.1.2)

[p]pxxx

[pxxx] /__[+Closed.V]

'zzz'

fricative/f/[f]fàm

[fàm]

'deserted homestead (V323;3.1.2)'

[ᵐv]mvxxx

[ᵐvxx] /__[-Closed.V]

'zzz'

stop/bʷ/[bʷ]bwə'ə

[əʔə]~[əʔɑ] HH

'owl' (V332;4.1)

[bᵛ][ clarification needed ]bvə

[bᵛə] H

'there' (V324;3.1.4)

ᴺCstop/ᵐb/[ᵐb]mbà

[ᵐbaʔ] L(H)

'nut' (V324;3.1.3)

fricative/ᵐf/[ᵐf]mf

[ᵐfáŋ]

'wound' (V324;3.1.3)

ᴺCʷstop/ᵐbʷ/[ᵐbʷ]mbwə

[ᵐbʷə] H

'goat' (V325;3.1.5)

(adapted from Voorhoeve 1965)

row 1 = orthography; row 2 = [IPA]; row 3 = 'gloss'

Initial alveolars

Initial alveolars include:

  • plain consonants (C): /n/, /d/ (with allophone [l]), /t/ (with allophone [tʰ]), and /s/
  • labialized consonants (): /sʷ/
  • pre-nasalized consonants (ⁿC): /ⁿd/, /ⁿt/ (with allophone [ⁿtʰ]), and /ⁿs/ (with allophone [ⁿz])
  • pre-nasalized labialized consonants (ᴺCʷ): /ⁿsʷ/
Examples of alveolar onsets
Cnasal/n/[n]nà

[nɑ] L

'field' (V323;3.1.2)

stop,

voiced

/d/[d]dim

[dɪm] H

'tongue' (V323;3.1.2)

[l]lxxx

[lxxx] /__[-Closed.V]

'zzz'

stop,

voiceless

/t/[t]tu

[tu] tone?

'head'

[tʰ]tu

[tʰʊ] tone?

'to pierce' (Voorhoeve 1966:323)

fricative/s/[s]sògo

[sògó] LH

'to wash' (V323;3.1.2)

fricative/sʷ/[sʷ]swá

[á]

'broom, tail' (V324;3.1.4)

ᴺCstop,

voiced

/ⁿd/[ⁿd]ndəb L

[ⁿdəb] L(H)

'cotton' (V324;3.1.3)

stop,

voiceless

/ⁿt/[ⁿt]ntɑnə

[ⁿtɑnə] HH

'market, business' (V324;3.1.3)

[ⁿtʰ]ntxx

['ⁿtʰyyy]/__[+Closed]

'zzz'

fricative/ⁿs/[ⁿs]nsindɑ

[ⁿsí-ⁿdɑ] H!H

'floor' (V324;3.1.3)

[ⁿz]nzxxx

[ⁿzyyyy] /__[-Closed]

'zzz'

ᴺCʷfricative/ⁿsʷ/[ⁿsʷ]nswə

[ⁿsʷə] H

'new' (V325;3.1.5)

(adapted from Voorhoeve 1965)

row 1 = orthography; row 2 = [IPA]; row 3 = 'gloss'

Initial palatals

Initial palatals include:

  • plain consonants (C): /ɲ/, /ɟ/ (with allophones [j], [ʒ], [z]), /c/ (with allophones [ts] and [cʃ]), and /ʃ/
  • labialized consonants (): /ɲʷ/, /ɟʷ/ (with allophones [ʒʷ],[zʷ]), and /cʷ/ (with allophones [ʃ] and [tsʷ])
  • pre-nasalized consonants (ᴺC): /ᶮɟ/ (with allophones [ᶮʒ], [ⁿz]), and /ᶮc/ (with allophones [ᶮcʃ] and [ⁿts])
  • pre-nasalized labialized consonants ('ⁿCʷ): /ᶮcʷ/ with allophone [ᶮʃ]
Examples of palatal onsets
PhonemeAllophoneExample
OrthographyIPAGloss
/ɲ/[ɲ]nyàang[ɲaaŋ] LH'to dance' (V323;3.1.2)
/ɟ/[ɟ]jənə[ɟənə] HH'to see' (V323;3.1.2)
[j]
[ʒ]
[z]zə[zə] H __/i/'relative pronoun' (V325; 3.1.6)
/c/[c]tu[cu][ tone? ]'head' (ref)
[ts]tu[tsʰʊ][ tone? ]'to pierce' (Voorhoeve 1966:323)
[cʃ]
/ʃ/[ʃ]
/ɲʷ/[ɲʷ]nywìi['ɲʷiiʔ] LH'to spoil' (V324;3.1.4)
/ɟʷ/[ɟʷ]jwəde LH[ɟᵂədə] LH'to wet' (V324; 3.1.4)
[ʒʷ]
[zʷ]
/cʷ/[cʷ]cwi[ii] LH'to give a name' (V327,3.3.1)
[ʃ]shʉmə/cʷʉmə/, [ʃʉmə] HH __ʉ,u'to swing' (V325;3.1.8)
[tsʷ]
/ᶮɟ/[ᶮɟ]nyjiag[ᶮjiag] H'mane' (V324;3.1.3)
[ᶮʒ]
[ⁿz]
/ᶮc/[ᶮc]ntɑnə[ⁿtɑnə] HH'market, business' (V324;3.1.3)
[ᶮcʃ]
[ⁿts]
/ⁿsʷ/[ⁿsʷ]nyjwi[ᶮɟᵂi][ tone? ]'woman' (V325;3.1.5)
[ⁿzʷ]
/ᶮcʷ/[ᶮcʷ]
[ᶮʃ]
(adapted from Voorhoeve 1965)
Initial velars

Initial velars include:

  • plain consonants (C): /ŋ/, /g/ (with allophone [ʁ]), /k/ (with allophones [kʰ] and [kx])
  • labialized consonants (): /ŋʷ/ and /gʷ/ (with allophones [w] and [v])
  • pre-nasalized consonants (ᴺC): /ᵑg/ (with allophone [ᵑgˠ])and /ᵑk/ (with allophones [ᵑkʰ] and [ᵑkx])
  • pre-nasalized labialized consonants (ⁿCʷ): /ᵑgʷ/ and /ᵑkʷ/
Examples of velar onsets
PhonemeAllophoneExample
OrthographyIPAGloss
/ŋ/[ŋ]ŋà'ŋà'[ŋɑʔ-ŋɑʔ] LL'mosquito' (V323;3.1.2)
/g/[g]gubtə[gub-tə] HH'to linger on' (V323;3.1.2)
[ʁ]
/t/[k]
[kʰ]
[kx]
/ŋʷ/[ŋʷ]ŋwìnte LLH[ŋʷin-tə] LLH'to grow thin' (V324;3.1.4)
/gʷ/[gʷ]gwə[gᵂə] H'who?' (V324; 3.1.4)
[w]
[v]
/kʷ/[kʷ]
/ᵑg/[ᵑg]ŋgà[ᵑgɑ] L(H)'root, vein' (V324;3.1.3)
[ᵑgˠ]
/ᵑk/[ᵑk]ŋkɑnə[ᵑkɑnə] HH'market, business' (V324;3.1.3)
[ᵑkʰ]
[ᵑkx]
/ᵑgʷ/[ᵑgʷ]ŋgwàn[ᵑgʷan] L(H)'slave' (V325;3.1.5)
/kʷ/[ᵑkʷ]
(adapted from Voorhoeve 1965:323ff.)

Phonological processes affecting segments

Vowel insertion

Consonant-final words - which are generally CVC because of the size constraint that favours CV or CVC words - are often augmented by a final vowel. This process of vowel insertion happens in one of two contexts: (i) before a pause; (ii) at the end of a sentence. The quality of the inserted vowel is conditioned by the final consonant: if the final C is a glottal stop, then the inserted vowel is schwa; elsewhere, the inserted vowel is a copy of the stem vowel. Examples illustrating vowel insertion are given in (60).

(60) a. koo                          b.  cintEE        ko-o                             cin-te-e        love-FV                          xx-yy-FV        'to want, to love'               'to urinate'        (adapted from Voorhoeve 1965:332)

Consonant mutation

Consonants in onset position surface with different variants. This consonant allophone, a form of consonant mutation, is conditioned by the following vowel. There are seven conditioning contexts, as follows:

  1. the non-closed vowels
  2. the closed vowels
  3. the high front vowel /i/
  4. the high non-front vowels /ʉ/ and /u/
  5. the high central (non-front, non-back) vowel /ʉ/
  6. the high back vowel /u/
  7. the vowels /o/ and /ə/
Conditioning contexts for consonant mutation
ContextSegment SetClassEffectAlternation
1.__ V[-Closed]{b, d, ɟ, g}voiced stop"devoicing"/b/→[p]; /d/→[l]; /ɟ/→[y]; /ɟᵂ/→[yᵂ]; /g/→[ʁ]; /gᵂ/→[w]
{ᵐf, ⁿs}nasalized fricativevoicing/ᵐf/→[ᵐv]; /ⁿs/→[ⁿz] (except before /o/)
2.__ V[+Closed]{t, ⁿt, k, ᵑk}voiceless stopaspiration/t/→[tʰ]; /ⁿt/→[ⁿtʰ]; /k/→[kʰ/kˣ]; /ᵑk/→[ᵑkʰ/ᵑkˣ]
{b}bilabialdevoicing/b/→[p] (optional)
3.__ /i/{ɟ, ɟᵂ, ᶮɟ, ᶮɟᵂ, c, cᵂ}palatalfronting + spirantization/ɟ/→[z]; /ɟᵂ/→[zᵂ]; /ᶮɟ/→[ⁿz]; /ᶮɟᵂ/→[ⁿzᵂ]; /c/ →[ʦ]; /cᵂ/ → [ʦᵂ]; /ᶮcᵂ/ →[ ᶮʃ]?
4.__ /ʉ, u/{ɟ, ᶮɟ, c, cᵂ}palatalspirantization/ɟ/ →[ʒ]; /ᶮɟ/ → [ᶮʒ]; /c/ → [cʃ]; /cᵂ/ → [ʃ] (3.1.8, V1965)
{gᵂ}labialized voiced velar/gᵂ/→[v]
5.__ /ʉ/{g, ᵑg}simplex & nasalized voiced velarspirantization/g/ → [ɣ]; /ᵑg/ → [ᵑgˠ]
6.__ /u/{g}simplex voiced velarretraction + spirantization/g/ → [ʁ]
7.__ /o, ə/{bᵂ}labialized bilabialspirantization/bᵂ/ → [bᵛ]
(adapted from Voorhoeve 1965:xxx-zzz)

Tone

Medumba is famous for the extent to which tone shapes grammar. Although having only a two-tone contrast, namely High (H) and Low (L), surface tone melodies are conditioned by a variety of lexical, morphological and syntactic factors:

  1. lexically specified level Low (L) and High (H) tone
  2. morphologically derived falling (HL) and rising (HL) contour tones
  3. syntactically conditioned downstep, where H is produced at a lower pitch than a preceding H tone

Two tones: high versus low

Medumba is described as a two-level tone system with low (L) and high (H) tones; examples are given in Table 16. Observe that the L/H contrast is found with all Lexical (open) class categories; this includes verbs, nouns and prepositions. Likewise, Functional (closed-class) categories show an L/H contrasts; this includes verbal F-categories (C, T, and Aspect) and nominal F-categories (Dem, Det, Pl). [Describe examples; also give minimal pairs]

Tone contrasts with verbs

Verb stems come in two shapes, CV and CVC, with each one contrasting Low and High tone. See (1-5) for examples of High/Low tone contrast with CV stems, and (6-6) for examples of High/Low tone contrast with CVC stems.

High/Low contrast with CV verb stems
Low toneHigh toneSource
IPAorthographyglossIPAorthographygloss
bɑ L'ecaillier''be crazy'V1976:111
'stand up'lo'leave'V1976:123
nyì'defecate'nyi'press'V1976:125
'be strong, hard'ta1. trade

2. deny

3. defend oneself

V1976:127
zwì'laugh'zwi'kill'V1976:131
High/Low contrast with CVC verb stems
Low toneHigh toneSource
IPAorthographyglossIPAorthographygloss
fʉ̀əgə LH'be light'fʉəgə H'blow'V1976:117
kʉ̀a L'sharpen, limer'kʉa H'reclame'V1976:121
làdə LH'assemble'ladə'lick'V1976:122
lɑnə'cry, lament'lɑnə'be clean, clear, healthy'V1976:122
tagə'miss'tagə'gather with full hands'V1976:127
tamə LH'mix, assemble'tamə1. 'pull with thread'

2. 'sew'

3. 'withe'

V1976:127
tɔgə LH'spit'tɔgə'pass'V1976:128
vɔgə LH'wake up with a start'vɔgə'be short'V1976:129
[jɑʔɑ] LHyɑʔ'ɑ LH'cross'[jɑʔɑ] HHyɑʔɑ'give credit'V1976:130
yɔgə LH1. 'live'

2. 'devore'

yɔgə1. 'warm onsself up'

2. 'pass the day'

V1976:130
ywədə LH'soak, wet'ywədə1. 'be rested

2. 'be full (from eating'

V1976:130
(adapted from Voorhoeve 1976)

Tone contrasts with nouns

High/Low tone contrast with CV nouns stems
Low toneHigh toneSource
IPAorthographyglossIPAorthographygloss
L(H)[mbà]m-bà'nut' (c2)H(L)[mbá]m-ba'pot, marmite'V1976:111
L(L)[cɔ]'news, story' (c1/4)H(H)[cɔ]'theft' (c3)V1976:114
L(L)[ndɔ]ndɔ'long solid unit'H(L)[ndɔ]ndɔ1. horn "corne"

2. whistle

V1976:115
L(H)1. 'feather' (c3/5)

2.' leaf'

H(L)'dead body'V19`76:116
L(H)'star'H(L)sa'game'V1976:126
L(H)n-zà'miracle' (c2/4)H(L)n-za'hill'V1976:131
(adapted from Voorhoeve 1976)
High/Low tone contrast with CVC noun stems: (23-29)
Low toneHigh toneSource
IPAorthographyglossIPAorthographygloss
(23)L(L)[mbàn]m-bàn'rain' (c2)H(L)[mbán]m-ban'side, c?'V1976:111
(24)L(H)[bùʔ]bù''mushroom' (c3/5)H(L)[búʔ]bu''package' (c3/6)V1976:113
(25)L(H)[ɣəʔ]ghə''cheek' (c3/5)H(H)ghə''avarice' (c3)V1976:117
(26)L(L)n-tɑn'string' (c1)H(H)n-tɑn'trade, commerce' (c1)V1976:127
(27)L(H)ŋ-kùn'tail' (c2/4)H(L)ŋ-kun'rice, beans'V1976:121
(28)L(L)kɑb'fence' (c1/4)H(L)ŋ-kɑb'money' (c1)V1976:119
(29)L(L)kam'piece' (c3/5)H(L)ŋ-kam'noble' (c1/4)V1976:119
(30)L(H)cwed'the bush'L(H)ncwed'chiefancy
(adapted from Voorhoeve 1976)

In principle, given the possibility of a stem bearing associated with one of four tone melodies — namely L(L), L(H), H(L) and H(H) for nouns and L or H for verbs — one expects to find a four-way tone contrast for a given segmental base (either CV or CVC). No such examples are attested within a given word-class, but there is one instance elf a 4-way contrast across word-classes. In addition, there are a few three-way contrasts for a given noun base, and numerous many four-way tone contrasts with the same base, if one looks at tone melodies across word-classes.

  • CV tɔ, which has L(H), H(L) and H(H) stems
  • CVC m-vɛd, which has L(L)
  • CVC lɛn, to, and mvdd.

Examples are given in (1-3).

Tone contrasts across word classes: L versus H
Low toneHigh toneSource
IPAorthographyglossIPAorthographygloss
(1)L(L)H(L)'N: neck, throat' (c2)V1976:128
L(H)N: 'nombril' (c3)H(H)'N: hole' (c3/5)
Ltɔ-ɔ LH1. V: 'govern'

2. V: take/pay a debt

H
(2)L(L)m-vɛdN: 'rope' (c1/4)H(L)m-vɛd'N: oil' (c5)V1976:129
L-L(L)m-vɛd-m-vɛdN: 'mosquito' (c4)
L(H)H(H)m-vɛd'N: brother' (c1/4)
Lvɛd-ə LHV: 'tremble'H
(3)L(L)lɛnN: 'sign' (c2/4)H(L)lɛnN: 'name' (c3/5)V1976:123
L(H)lɛnN: 'mark, quality, sort' (c2)H(H)
Llɛn-ə LHV: know, recognizeH
(4)L(L)bàgN: 'side' (c?)H(L)V1976:111
L(H)bàgN: '1pl pronoun'H(H)
Lbàg-ə LHV: 'split'Hbag-ə HHV: 'lean'
(5)L(L)bàmN: 'belly" (c3/5)H(L)V1976:111
L(H)H(H)
Lbàm-ə LHV: 'wake up'HbamV: 'accept, believer, answer'
(6)L(L)H(L)
L(H)bu'N: 'mushroom' (c3/5)H(H)bu'N: package (c3/5)
LHbu'V: 'play, sound out'
(adapted from Voorhoeve 1976)
Noun classes
Low toneHigh toneSource
IPAorthographyglossIPAorthographygloss
(1)L(L)'sorcellery, magic (c3)L(H)ŋ-kà'rank' (c2)V1976:119
(2)L(L)[kəʔ]kə''tam-tam' (c3/5)H(L)ŋ-kə''ball'V1976:120
(3)L(H)'arrow" (c3/5)L(H)ŋ-kɔ'pilon' (c1/4)V1976:121
(4)Hshun'friendship (c1/4)H(H)n-shun'friend' (c1/4)V1976:126
(5)H(H)1. 'tree' (c3/5)

2. 'up above'

H(L)n-tʉ'heart' (c/24)V1976:129
(adapted from Voorhoeve 1976)
(3)  Low-tone                                     (4) High-tone          tʃə́ŋ                           [fù]                                                [tʃə́ŋ]                        'medicine'                                           'food'           (From Kouankem 2013:60; Mucha 2017: 8)

Tone contrasts with prepositions

(5)  Low-tone    (a)   mbàŋ       [ᵐbàŋ]      (b)   nùm     [nùm]       (c)   ɲàm     [ɲàm]                                                                                     'next to'                    'on'                      'behind'  (6) High-tone          mʙə́    [ᵐʙə́]                        'in front of'  (from Hawkes et al. 2015:122)

Tone contrasts with complementizers

(7)  Low-tone    (a)   ndà                        (8) High-tone         mbʉ                           [ⁿdà]                                           [ᵐbʉ́]                        'C'                                              'C'      (From)

Tone contrasts with demonstratives

(9)  Low-tone    (a)    s-ə̂n                     (10) High-tone         N yə́n                           [s-ə̂n]                                           [N  yə́n]                        AGR-this                                          N    Dem.Dist                                                                        'that N'      (Kouankem 2013:60)

Tone contrasts with plural-marking

(11)  Low-tone    (a)   bà   N                    (12) High-tone        ba N                         [bà]                                             [bá]                        'PL'                                            'PL'     (Kouankem 2013:62)

Falling and rising tones

In addition to level high and low tones, Medumba exhibits falling (HL) and rising (LH) contour tones. These contour tones are morphologically derived from floating H tones that occur as affixes preceding or following the stems they associate with. These floating tones make themselves known by docking to tone-bearing units (TBUs) associated with L-tone, thus forming a tone contour. [DESCRIBE EXAMPLES; add LH examples]

(13)  L-tone verb (put in sentence)       ghʉ̀        [ɣʉ̀]        do       'do’   (14)  Derived HL-tone verb (put in sentence)       nghʉ̀        [N-ɣʉ̀]        N-do       'do, consecutive’  (identify source)

Downstep

Medumba shows downstep, where H is produced at a lower pitch than an immediately preceding H tone; downstep is represented as (ꜜ). Downstep is viewed as resulting from a floating Low tone that shifts the pitch level of a following High tone one step lower than the preceding High tone. [3] Downstep is syntactically conditioned in that it occurs at phrasal boundaries:

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Taa language</span> Tuu language of southwestern Botswana and eastern Namibia

TaaTAH, also known as ǃXóõKOH, is a Tuu language notable for its large number of phonemes, perhaps the largest in the world. It is also notable for having perhaps the heaviest functional load of click consonants, with one count finding that 82% of basic vocabulary items started with a click. Most speakers live in Botswana, but a few hundred live in Namibia. The people call themselves ǃXoon or ʼNǀohan, depending on the dialect they speak. The Tuu languages are one of the three traditional language families that make up the Khoisan languages. In 2011, there were around 2,500 speakers of Taa.

The phonology of Portuguese varies among dialects, in extreme cases leading to some difficulties in mutual intelligibility. This article on phonology focuses on the pronunciations that are generally regarded as standard. Since Portuguese is a pluricentric language, and differences between European Portuguese (EP), Brazilian Portuguese (BP), and Angolan Portuguese (AP) can be considerable, varieties are distinguished whenever necessary.

ǂʼAmkoeAM-koy, formerly called by the dialectal name ǂHoan, is a severely endangered Kxʼa language of Botswana. West ǂʼAmkoe dialect, along with Taa and Gǀui, form the core of the Kalahari Basin sprachbund, and share a number of characteristic features, including the largest consonant inventories in the world. ǂʼAmkoe was shown to be related to the Juu languages by Honken and Heine (2010), and these have since been classified together in the Kxʼa language family.

Bernese German, like other High Alemannic varieties, has a two-way contrast in plosives and fricatives that is not based on voicing, but on length. The absence of voice in plosives and fricatives is typical for all High German varieties, but many of them have no two-way contrast due to general lenition.

The phonology of the Persian language varies between regional dialects, standard varieties, and even from older variates of Persian. Persian is a pluricentric language and countries that have Persian as an official language have separate standard varieties, namely: Standard Dari (Afghanistan), Standard Iranian Persian (Iran) and Standard Tajik (Tajikistan). The most significant differences between standard varieties of Persian are their vowel systems. Standard varieties of Persian have anywhere from 6 to 8 vowel distinctions, and similar vowels may be pronounced differently between standards. However, there are not many notable differences when comparing consonants, as all standard varieties a similar amount of consonant sounds. Though, colloquial varieties generally have more differences than their standard counterparts. Most dialects feature contrastive stress and syllable-final consonant clusters. Linguists tend to focus on Iranian Persian, so this article may contain less adequate information regarding other varieties.

The Sikkimese language, also called Sikkimese, Bhutia, or Drenjongké, Dranjoke, Denjongka, Denzongpeke and Denzongke, belongs to the Tibeto-Burman languages. It is spoken by the Bhutia in Sikkim, India, and in parts of Koshi, Nepal. It is the official language in Sikkim. The Sikkimese people refer to their own language as Drendzongké and their homeland as Drendzong. Up until 1975, Sikkimese was not a written language. After gaining Indian statehood, the language was introduced as a school subject in Sikkim and the written language was developed.

Iau or Turu is a Lakes Plain language of West Papua, Indonesia, spoken by about 2,100 people, native speakers of this language are the Turu people (Iau). Most speakers are monolingual, and their number is growing. Other peoples in the western Lakes Plain area speak basic Iau. Iau is heavily tonal, with 11 tones on nouns and 19 simple and compound tones on verbs.

Unlike many languages, Icelandic has only very minor dialectal differences in sounds. The language has both monophthongs and diphthongs, and many consonants can be voiced or unvoiced.

The phonology of Sesotho and those of the other Sotho–Tswana languages are radically different from those of "older" or more "stereotypical" Bantu languages. Modern Sesotho in particular has very mixed origins inheriting many words and idioms from non-Sotho–Tswana languages.

Taos is a Tanoan language spoken by several hundred people in New Mexico, in the United States. The main description of its phonology was contributed by George L. Trager in a (pre-generative) structuralist framework. Earlier considerations of the phonetics-phonology were by John P. Harrington and Jaime de Angulo. Trager's first account was in Trager (1946) based on fieldwork 1935-1937, which was then substantially revised in Trager (1948). The description below takes Trager (1946) as the main point of departure and notes where this differs from the analysis of Trager (1948). Harrington's description is more similar to Trager (1946). Certain comments from a generative perspective are noted in a comparative work Hale (1967).

The most thorough treatment of the Kiowa sound system is by Laurel Watkins in a generative framework. A consideration of prosodic phenomena with acoustic analysis is in Sivertsen (1956). Earlier discussions of phonemics are Trager (1960), Merrifield (1959), Wonderly et al. (1954), and Harrington (1928).

This article is about the sound system of the Navajo language. The phonology of Navajo is intimately connected to its morphology. For example, the entire range of contrastive consonants is found only at the beginning of word stems. In stem-final position and in prefixes, the number of contrasts is drastically reduced. Similarly, vowel contrasts found outside of the stem are significantly neutralized. For details about the morphology of Navajo, see Navajo grammar.

Izi is an Igboid language spoken in Ebonyi state in Nigeria. It forms a dialect cluster with the closely related languages Ikwo, Ezza, and Mgbo.

The phonology of Burmese is fairly typical of a Southeast Asian language, involving phonemic tone or register, a contrast between major and minor syllables, and strict limitations on consonant clusters.

Kensiu (Kensiw) is an Austroasiatic language of the Jahaic subbranch. It is spoken by a small community of 300 people in Yala Province in southern Thailand and also reportedly by a community of approximately 300 speakers in Western Malaysia in Perak and Kedah states. Speakers of this language are Negritos who are known as the Mani people or Maniq of Thailand.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Babanki language</span> Grassfields Bantoid language of Cameroon

Babanki, or Kejom, is a Bantoid language that is spoken by the Babanki people of the Western Highlands of Cameroon.

Avava (Navava), also known as Katbol, Tembimbe-Katbol, or Bangsa’ is an Oceanic language of central Malekula, Vanuatu. It has nasalized fricatives and a bilabial trill.

The phonology of Old Saxon mirrors that of the other ancient Germanic languages, and also, to a lesser extent, that of modern West Germanic languages such as English, Dutch, Frisian, German, and Low German.

Gumuz is a dialect cluster spoken along the border of Ethiopia and Sudan. It has been tentatively classified within the Nilo-Saharan family. Most Ethiopian speakers live in Kamashi Zone and Metekel Zone of the Benishangul-Gumuz Region, although a group of 1,000 reportedly live outside the town of Welkite. The Sudanese speakers live in the area east of Er Roseires, around Famaka and Fazoglo on the Blue Nile, extending north along the border. Dimmendaal et al. (2019) suspect that the poorly attested varieties spoken along the river constitute a distinct language, Kadallu.

References

  1. 1 2 Voorhoeve, Jan (1965). "The Structure of the Morpheme in Bamileke (Bangangté dialect)". Lingua. 13: 319–334. doi:10.1016/0024-3841(64)90034-8.
  2. Olson, Kenneth S.; Meynadier, Yohann (2015). "On Medumba Bilabial Trills and Vowels" (PDF). Proceedings of the International Phonetic Association.
  3. Voorhoeve, Jan (1971). "Tonology of the Bamileke Noun". Journal of African Languages. 10: 44–53.