Swedish phonology

Last updated

Swedish has a large vowel inventory, with nine vowels distinguished in quality and to some degree in quantity, making 18 vowel phonemes in most dialects. Another notable feature is the pitch accent, a development which it shares with Norwegian. Swedish pronunciation of most consonants is similar to that of other Germanic languages.

Contents

There are 18 consonant phonemes, of which /ɧ/ and /r/ show considerable variation depending on both social and dialectal context.

Finland Swedish has a slightly different phonology.

Vowels

The vowel phonemes of Central Standard Swedish in the Stockholm area. From Engstrand (1999:140) Swedish monophthongs chart.svg
The vowel phonemes of Central Standard Swedish in the Stockholm area. From Engstrand (1999 :140)
Front Central Back
unrounded rounded
short long shortlongshortlongshortlong
Close ɪ ʏ ʉː ʊ
Close-mid e øː ɵ
Open-mid ɛ ɛː œ ɔ
Open a ɑː

Swedish has nine vowels that, as in many other Germanic languages, exist in pairs of long and short versions. [1] The length covaries with the quality of the vowels, as shown in the table below (long vowels in the first column, short in the second), with short variants being more centered and lax. [1] The length is generally viewed as the primary distinction, with quality being secondary. [2] No short vowels appear in open stressed syllables. [3] The front vowels appear in rounded-unrounded pairs: /ʏ//ɪ/, /yː//iː/, /œ//ɛ/ and /øː//eː/.

VowelExampleVowelExample
/siːl/ sil ('sieve') ɪ /sɪl/ sill ('herring')
/heːl/ hel ('whole') ɛ /hɛl/ häll ('stone slab')
ɛː /hɛːl/ häl ('heel')
ɑː /mɑːt/ mat ('food') a /mat/ matt ('listless; matte')
/moːl/ mål ('goal') ɔ /mɔl/ moll ('minor [key]')
/buːt/ bot ('penance') ʊ /bʊt/ bott ('lived') (supine)
ʉː /fʉːl/ ful ('ugly') ɵ /fɵl/ full ('full')
/syːl/ syl ('awl') ʏ /sʏl/ syll ('sleeper (railroad tie)')
øː /nøːt/ nöt ('nut') œ /nœt/ nött ('worn')

Rounded vowels have two types of rounding:

Type of rounding is the primary way of distinguishing /ʉː,ɵ/ from /yː,œ/, especially in Central Standard Swedish.

/ɛː/, /ɛ/ (in stressed syllables), /øː/ (with a few exceptions) and /œ/ are lowered to [ æː ], [ æ ], [ œ̞ː ] and [ œ̞ ], respectively, when preceding /r/. [13] [14] [15]

The low allophones are becoming unmarked in younger speakers of Stockholm Swedish, so that läsa ('to read') and köpa ('to buy') are pronounced [ˈlæ̂ːsa] and [ˈɕœ̂ːpa] instead of standard [ˈlɛ̂ːsa] and [ˈɕø̂ːpa]. [15] These speakers often also pronounce pre-rhotic /øː/ and /œ/ even lower, i.e. [ ɶː ] and [ ɶ ]. [15] This is especially true for the long allophone. [15] Also, the [ ɶː ] allophone is sometimes difficult to distinguish from the long /ɑː/. [15]

In some pronunciations, traditionally characteristic of the varieties spoken around Gothenburg and in Östergötland, but today more common e.g. in Stockholm and especially in younger speakers, [œ] and [ɵ] merge, most commonly into [ɵ] (especially before [r] and the retroflex consonants). Words like fördömande ('judging', pronounced /fœrˈdœ̌mandɛ/ in Standard Swedish) and fördummande ('dumbing', pronounced /fœrˈdɵmandɛ/ in Standard Swedish) are then often pronounced similarly or identically, as [fɵˈɖɵmːandɛ]. [16] [17]

In Central Standard Swedish, unstressed /ɛ/ is slightly retracted [ɛ̠], but is still a front vowel rather than central [ə]. However, the latter pronunciation is commonly found in Southern Swedish. Therefore, begå 'to commit' is pronounced [bɛ̠ˈɡoː] in Central Standard Swedish and [bəˈɡoː] in Southern Swedish. Before /r/, southerners may use a back vowel [ ɔ ]. In Central Standard Swedish, a true schwa [ə] is commonly found as a vocalic release of word-final lenis stops, as in e.g. bädd[ˈbɛdːə] 'bed'. [18]

In many central and eastern areas (including Stockholm), the contrast between short /ɛ/ and /e/ is lost. [19] The loss of this contrast has the effect that hetta ('heat') and hätta ('cap') are pronounced the same.

In Central Standard Swedish, long /ɑː/ is weakly rounded [ ɒ̜ː ]. [1] [7] [20] The rounding is stronger in Gothenburg and weaker in most North Swedish dialects. [20]

One of the varieties of /iː/ is made with a constriction that is more forward than is usual. Peter Ladefoged and Ian Maddieson describe this vowel as being pronounced "by slightly lowering the body of the tongue while simultaneously raising the blade of the tongue (...) Acoustically this pronunciation is characterized by having a very high F3, and an F2 which is lower than that in /eː/." They suggest that this may be the usual Stockholm pronunciation of /iː/. [21]

There is some variation in the interpretations of vowel length's phonemicity. Elert (1964), [22] for example, treats vowel quantity as its own separate phoneme (a "prosodeme") so that long and short vowels are allophones of a single vowel phoneme.

Patterns of diphthongs of long vowels occur in three major dialect groups. In Central Standard Swedish, the high vowels /iː/, /yː/, /ʉː/ and /uː/ are realized as narrow closing diphthongs with fully close ending points: [ɪ̝iʏ̝yɵ̝˖ʉ̟ʊ̝u]. [23] According to Engstrand, the second element is so close as to become a palatal or bilabial fricative: [ɪ̝ʝʏ̝ʝʷɵ̝˖βʲʊ̝β]. [7] Elsewhere in the article, the broad transcription iː yː ʉː uː is used.

In Central Standard Swedish, /eː/, /øː/ and /oː/ are often realized as centering diphthongs [eə], [øə] and [oə].

In Southern Swedish dialects, particularly in Scania and Blekinge, the diphthongs are preceded by a rising of the tongue from a central position so that /ʉː/ and /ɑː/ are realized as [eʉ] and [aɑ] respectively. A third type of distinctive diphthongs occur in the dialects of Gotland. The pattern of diphthongs is more complex than those of southern and eastern Sweden; /eː/, /øː/ and /ʉː/ tend to rise while /ɛː/ and /oː/ fall; /uː/, /iː/, /yː/ and /ɑː/ are not diphthongized at all. [24]

Consonants

The table below shows the Swedish consonant phonemes in spoken Standard Swedish. [25]

Labial Dental/
Alveolar
Palatal Velar Glottal
Nasal m n ŋ
Plosive voiceless p t k
voiced b d ɡ
Fricative f s ɕ ɧ h
Approximant v l j
Rhotic r

/t,l/ are dental [, ], [26] but /n,d,s/ can be either dental [, , ] or alveolar [n, d, s]. [27] If /d/ is alveolar, then /n/ is also alveolar. [28] Dental realization of /n,d/ is the predominant one in Central Standard Swedish. [28]

Stops

PhonemeExample
p /puːl/ pol ('pole') (of axis)
b /buːk/ bok ('book')
t /tuːk/ tok ('fool')
d /duːp/ dop ('christening')
k /kuːn/ kon ('cone')
ɡ /ɡuːd/ god ('good')

Initial fortis stops (/p,t,k/) are aspirated in stressed position, but unaspirated when preceded by /s/ within the same morpheme. [7] Hence ko ('cow') is [kʰuː], but sko ('shoe') becomes [skuː]. Compare English [kʰuːɫ] ('cool') vs [skuːɫ] ('school'). In Finland Swedish, aspiration does not occur and initial lenis stops /b,d,ɡ/ are usually voiced throughout. [29] [30] Word-medial lenis stops are sometimes voiceless in Finland, a likely influence from Finnish. [30]

Preaspiration of medial [31] and final fortis stops, [32] including the devoicing of preceding sonorants, [33] is common, [34] [35] though its length and normativity varies from dialect to dialect, being optional (and idiolectal [36] ) in Central Standard Swedish but obligatory in, for example, the Swedish dialects of Gräsö, [37] Vemdalen and Arjeplog. [38] In Gräsö, preaspiration is blocked in certain environments (such as an /s/ following the fortis consonant [39] or a morpheme boundary between the vowel and the consonant [33] ), while it is a general feature of fortis medial consonants in Central Standard Swedish. [33] When not preaspirated, medial and final fortis stops are simply unaspirated. [40] In clusters of fortis stops, the second "presonorant" stop is unaspirated and the former patterns with other medial final stops (that is, it is either unaspirated or is preaspirated). [41]

The phonetic attributes of preaspiration also vary. In the Swedish of Stockholm, preaspiration is often realized as a fricative subject to the character of surrounding vowels or consonants so that it may be labial, velar, or dental; it may also surface as extra length of the preceding vowel. [42] In the province of Härjedalen, though, it resembles [h] or [x]. [42] The duration of preaspiration is highest in the dialects of Vemdalen and Arjeplog. [43] Helgason notes that preaspiration is longer after short vowels, in lexically stressed syllables, as well as in pre-pausal position. [31] [44]

Fricatives

PhonemeExample
f /fuːt/ fot ('foot')
s /suːt/ sot ('soot')
ɕ /ɕuːl/ kjol ('skirt')
ɧ /ɧuːk/ sjok ('chunk')
h /huːt/ hot ('threat')

/s/ is dental [ ] in Central Standard Swedish, [45] [46] but retracted alveolar [ ] in Blekinge, [47] Bohuslän, [47] Halland [47] and Scania. [47]

The Swedish fricatives /ɕ/ and /ɧ/ are often considered to be the most difficult aspects of Swedish pronunciation for foreign students. The combination of occasionally similar and rather unusual sounds as well as the large variety of partly overlapping allophones of /ɧ/ often presents difficulties for non-natives in telling the two apart. The existence of a third sibilant in the form of /s/ tends to confuse matters even more, and in some cases realizations that are labiodental can also be confused with /f/. The historic palatized kj and tj sound /ɕ/, is an affricate [t͡ɕ] or [t͡ʃ] in Finland Swedish. [29]

The Swedish phoneme /ɧ/ (the "sje-sound" or voiceless postalveolar-velar fricative) and its alleged coarticulation is a difficult and complex issue debated amongst phoneticians. [48] Though the acoustic properties of its [ɧ] allophones are fairly similar, the realizations can vary considerably according to geography, age, gender as well as social context and are notoriously difficult to describe and transcribe accurately. Most common are various sh-like sounds, with [ʂ] occurring mainly in northern Sweden and [ɕ] in Finland. A voiceless uvular fricative, [χ], can sometimes be used in the varieties influenced by major immigrant languages like Arabic and Kurdish. The different realizations can be divided roughly into the following categories: [49]

Sonorants

PhonemeExample
m /muːd/ mod ('courage')
n /nuːd/ nod ('node')
ŋ /lɔŋ/ lång ('long')
r /ruːv/ rov ('prey')
l /luːv/ lov ('tack')
v /voːt/ våt ('wet')
j /juːrd/ jord ('earth')

/r/ has distinct variations in Standard Swedish. For most speakers, the realization as an alveolar trill occurs only in contexts where emphatic stress is used.[ citation needed ] In Central Swedish, it is often pronounced as a fricative (transcribed as [ʐ]) [50] or approximant (transcribed as [ɹ]), [7] which is especially frequent in weakly articulated positions such as word-finally [29] and somewhat less frequent in stressed syllable onsets, in particular after other consonants. [50] It may also be an apico-alveolar tap. [29] One of the most distinct features of the southern varieties is the uvular realization of /r/, which may be a trill [ ʀ ], [51] a fricative [ ʁ ] or an approximant [ ʁ̞ ]. In Finland, /r/ is usually an apical trill [r], and may be an approximant [ɹ] postvocalically. [52]

Examples of retroflexion [53]
inputoutputgloss
Inflection/fœrt/[fœ̞ːʈ]fört'brought' sup
/fœrs/[fœ̞ːʂ]förs'is brought' pass
Derivation/fœrˈtɑːl/[fœ̞ˈʈʰɑːl]förtal'slander'
/fœrˈsɔrj/[fœ̞ˈʂɔrj]försorg'taking care'
Compounds/ˈfœ̂rˌtʉːr/[ˈfœ̞̂ːˌʈʰʉːr]förtur'priority'
/ˈfœ̂rˌsɑːl/[ˈfœ̞̂ːˌʂɑːl]försal'antechamber'
Across words/fœrˈtɵn/[fœ̞ˈʈʰɵnː]för tunn'too thin'
/fœrˈseːn/[fœ̞ˈʂeːn]för sen'too late'

In most varieties of Swedish that use an alveolar /r/ (in particular, the central and northern forms), the combination of /r/ with dental consonants (/t,d,n,l,s/) produces retroflex consonant realizations ([ʈ,ɖ,ɳ,ɭ,ʂ]), a recursive sandhi process called "retroflexion". [54] [55] Thus, /ˈkɑ̂ːrta/ ('map') is realized as [kʰɑ̂ːʈa], /nuːrd/ ('north') as [nuːɖ], /ˈvɛ̂ːnern/ ('Vänern') as [ˈvɛ̂ːnɛɳ], and /fɛrsk/ ('fresh') as [fæʂːk]. The process of retroflexion is not limited to just one dental, and e.g. först is pronounced [fœ̞ʂʈ]. [56] The combination of /r/ and /l/ does not uniformly cause retroflexion, so that it may also be pronounced with two separate consonants [rl], and even, occasionally in a few words and expressions, as a mere [l]. Thus sorl ('murmur') may be pronounced [soːɭ], but also [soːrl]. [57]

In Gothenburg and neighbouring areas (such as Mölndal and Kungälv) the retroflex consonants are substituted by alveolar ones, with their effects still remaining. For example: /kvɑːrn/ is [kvɑːn] not [kvɑːɳ], /hoːrd/ is [hoːd], not [hoːɖ]. However, /rs/, unlike what many other Swedes believe, is not [s] but [ʃ], i.e. /fɛrs/ is [fæʃː], not [fæsː].[ citation needed ]

As the adjacent table shows, this process is not limited by word boundaries, though there is still some sensitivity to the type of boundary between the /r/ and the dental in that retroflexion is less likely with boundaries higher up in the prosodic hierarchy. [58] In the southern varieties, which use a uvular /r/, [59] retroflex realizations do not occur. [56] For example, /ˈkɑ̂ːrta/ ('map') is realized as [ˈkʰɑ̌ʁta] (note that Tone 2 in Malmö sounds like Tone 1 in Stockholm), etc. [60] An /r/ spelled rr usually will not trigger retroflexion so that spärrnät/ˈspæ̂rˌnɛːt/ ('anti-sub net') is pronounced [ˈspæ̂rːˌnɛːt]. [61] Retroflexion also does not usually occur in Finland. [62] [63]

Variations of /l/ are not as common, though some phonetic variation exists, such as a retroflex flap [ɽ] that exists as an allophone in proximity to a labial or velar consonant (e.g. glad ('glad')) or after most long vowels. [64]

In casual speech, the nasals tend to assimilate to the place of articulation of a following obstruent so that, for example, han kom ('he came') is pronounced [haŋˈkʰɔmː]. [65]

/v/ and /j/ are pronounced with weak friction and function phonotactically with the sonorants. [56]

Prosody

Map of the major tonal dialects of Norwegian and Swedish, from Riad (2014).
* Dark areas have a low tone in accent 2, whereas the light areas have a high tone in accent 2.
* The isogloss marks the boundary between connective and non-connective dialects. East and north of it, all of the compounds get accent 2, whereas west and south of the isogloss, compounds vary in accent. Map of the major tonal dialects of Norwegian and Swedish.png
Map of the major tonal dialects of Norwegian and Swedish, from Riad (2014).
• Dark areas have a low tone in accent 2, whereas the light areas have a high tone in accent 2.
• The isogloss marks the boundary between connective and non-connective dialects. East and north of it, all of the compounds get accent 2, whereas west and south of the isogloss, compounds vary in accent.

Stress

In Swedish, stress is not fixed. Primary stress can fall on one of the last three syllables in a word’s stem. [66] [67] This can lead to surface contrasts based solely on difference in position of stress:

Primary stressed syllables are always metrically heavy, i.e. contain either a long vowel or a short vowel followed by a consonant. [67] In phonological analyses of Swedish, stressed syllables in underived forms are assumed to be associated with a basic moraic trochaic foot [μ μ]σ , [68] e.g. bˈil 'car' (stress marked as (ˈ)). More whole-word based analyses of metrical structure where affixes are included also assume other foot types, in particular, syllabic trochaic feet [σ σ]Ft, bˈil-ar 'cars'. [67] [69] Affixes affect stress to a considerable degree in the sense that inflectional suffixes can never receive primary stress (bˈil-ar-na 'the cars'), whereas many derivational suffixes can tent-ˈabel 'examinable'. Disyllabic words with accent 2 like ˈandˌe ‘spirit’, kvˈinnˌa ‘woman’, bˈilˌar 'cars' have secondary stress on the second syllable. In the Swedish Academy's lexicon , [70] these disyllables are transcribed with the stress pattern 3 2, e.g. kvin3a2 where (3) stands for primary stressed syllable with accent 2 and (2) represents a ‘secondary stressed’ syllable in words with accent 2). This secondary stress is assumed to have existed in Old Norse (see [71] and references therein). Compound words have primary stress on the first element and secondary stress on the last element bˈil-dels-butˌiken 'car-part shop' (secondary stress marked as (ˌ)). [66] [67]

Pitch accents

Stressed syllables carry one of two different tones, often described as pitch accents, or tonal word accents. [72] [73] [68] They are called acute and grave accent, accent 1 and accent 2. The actual realization of these two tones varies from dialect to dialect. [74] In the central Swedish dialect of Stockholm, accent 1 is characterized by a low tone at the beginning of the stressed syllable (fìsken 'the fish') and accent 2, by a high tone at the beginning of the stressed syllable (mátta 'mat'). [73] When the word is in a prominent/focused position, a high tone often occurs following the word accent (fìskén). In accent 2 words, this results in two high tones within the word (e.g. máttá), hence the term "two-peaked" for this dialect. In southern Swedish, a "one-peaked" dialect, accent 1 is realized as a high tone at the beginning of the stressed syllable (físken) and accent 2, by a low tone (màtta). [74] Generally, the grave accent is characterized by a later timing of the word accent pattern as compared with the acute accent. [73]

The phonemicity of this tonal system is demonstrated in the nearly 300 pairs of two-syllable words differentiated only by their use of either grave or acute accent. Outside of these pairs, the main tendency for tone is that the acute accent appears in monosyllables (since the grave accent cannot appear in monosyllabic words) while the grave accent appears in polysyllabic words. [75] Polysyllabic forms resulting from declension or derivation also tend to have a grave accent except when it is the definite article that is added. This tonal distinction has been present in Scandinavian dialects at least since Old Norse though a greater number of polysyllables now have an acute accent. These are mostly words that were monosyllabic in Old Norse, but have subsequently become disyllabic, as have many loanwords. [76] For example, Old Norse kømr ('comes') has become kommer in Swedish (with an acute accent). [75]

The distinction can be shown with the minimal pair anden 'the mallard' (tone 1) and anden 'the spirit' (tone 2).

In Central Swedish, this is a high, slightly falling tone followed by a low tone; that is, a single drop from high to low pitch spread over two syllables.
In Central Swedish, this is a mid falling tone followed by a high falling tone; that is, a double falling tone over two syllables.

The exact realization of the tones also depends on the syllable's position in an utterance. For instance, at the beginning of an utterance, the acute accent may have a rising rather than slightly falling pitch on the first syllable. Also, these are word tones that are spread across the syllables of the word. In trisyllabic words with the grave accent, the second fall in pitch is distributed across the second and third syllables:

The position of the tone is dependent upon stress: The first stressed syllable has a high or falling tone, as does the following syllable(s) in grave-accented words.

In most Finland-Swedish varieties, however, the distinction between grave and acute accent is missing.

A reasonably complete list of uncontroversial so-called minimal pairs can be seen below. [77] [ circular reference ] The two words in each pair are distinguished solely by having different tone (acute vs. grave). In those cases where both words are nouns it would have been possible to list the genitive forms of the words as well, thereby creating another word pair, but this has been avoided. A few word pairs where one of the words is a plural form with the suffix -or have been included. This is due to the fact that many Swedish-speakers in all parts of Sweden pronounce the suffix -or the same way as -er.[ citation needed ]

Translation acuteAcute accent (accent I)Grave accent (accent II)Translation grave
stern (of boat/ship)akterakteracts
the elmalmenallmänpublic, general
the AsA:naanasuspect
the mallardandenandenthe spirit
the reverse gear, the cratebackenbackenthe slope
the ball (dance event)balenbalenthe nest
the bulb (on horse)ballenballenthe dick (slang for penis)
the BsB:nabenaparting (hair)
bindsbinderbindorsanitary towels
the piecebitenbitenbitten
the bookbokenbokenoverripe, spoilt (of fruit)
the nestsbonabonapolish
the nests' (genitive of 'bona')bonasbonasbe polished (passive of 'bona')
the bristlesborstenborstenthe brush, the broom
the brace (sailing)brassenbrassenthe Brazilian
the lettersbrevenbrevvänpen pal
breaks (present tense of 'brista')bristerbristerflaws
the wellbrunnenbrunnenburnt (past participle of 'brinna')
the edges of forestsbrynenbrynenwhetstones
the edge of a forestbrynetbrynetthe whetstone
the cageburenburencarried (past participle of 'bära')
the pranksbusenbusenthe hooligan
the trolling spoonsdragendragendrawn (past participle of 'dra'), tipsy
the draught, the trolling spoondragetdragetdrawn (past participle of 'dra')
the speed, the energydrivetdrivetdrifted, driven (past participle of 'driva')
the EsE:naenaunite, unify
male nameEnarenarjunipers
the fallsfallenfallenfallen (past participle of 'falla')
the fallfalletfalletfallen (past participle of 'falla')
fastensfästerfesterparties
the fishfiskenfiskenacts of fishing
the FsF:enFNThe UN
the phone (in phonetics)fonenfånenthe idiot
the armfulsfångenfångenthe prisoner
the armfulfångetfångetcaught (past particple of 'fånga')
the rugfällenfällenplaces where trees have been felled
fells, cuts downfällerfällortraps (plural of the noun 'fälla')
the party, the feastfestenfästenplaces where something has been attached
the bow (on ship/boat)förenförenconditions of the ground for travelling (plural of 'före')
towards the bow (on ship/boat)förutförutbefore, earlier
marriesgiftergifterpoisons (plural of 'gift')
the poisongiftetgiftetthe marriage
the JsJ:naginatackle (sailing), take a shortcut
the deal (in card games)givengivengiven
the candlesljusengjusenthe osprey
the griffingripengripengrabbed, gripped (past participle of 'gripa')
the walkwaygångengångengone (past participle of 'gå')
the heathhedenhedenheathen (adjective)
has the time to do somethinghinnerhinnorcoatings
the cuts (made with a heavy object like an axe)huggenhuggenchopped (past participle of 'hugga')
the directionshållenhållenheld (past participle of 'hålla')
the directionhållethålletheld (past participle of 'hålla')
the HsH:nahånamock, taunt
higherhögrehögrethe man to the right (as in 'den högre')
the ideidenidenbears' dens for hibernation
the IsI:naInafemale name
ahead of, in front ofinförinförintroduces, introduce (present tense or imperative of 'införa')
the soundsljudenjudenthe Jew
Japanese syllabarykanakanaslide (noun/verb)
the caratkaratenkaratenthe karate
the catkattenkattena profanity (as in for example 'Katten också!')
the bangknallenknallenthe small hill, the pedlar
the knotknutenknutentied (past participle of 'knyta')
the bowler hatkubbenkubbenthe chopping block (for wood)
the litter (group of newborn animals)kullenkullenthe hill
the corpskårenkårenthe breeze
the lichenlavenlaventhe headframe
leads (present tense of 'leda')lederlederjoints (anatomy)
the military servicelumpenlumpencontemptible, lousy
the mothmalenmalenground, milled (past participle of 'mala')
the milkmjölkenmjölkenthe fish seed
the couragemodetmodetthe fashion
the mopmoppenmoppenthe moped
the namesnamnennamnenthe namesake
the normnormennorrmänNorwegians
the tacknubbennubbenthe shot (alcohol)
pinches (present tense of 'nypa')nypernyporGrips made with the thumb against one or more of the other fingers (plural noun)
name of a Norse GodOdenodenodes
the miteoretorättinjustice
the rabble (definite plural of 'pack')packenpackenthe bale
clownpajaspajasbe destroyed (passive of 'paja')
pantherpanterpanterdeposits
Persiansperserpärserordeals
PolandPolenpålenthe pole (thick wooden stick)
pollenpollenpållenthe horsey
radarradarradarpresent tense of 'rada', as in 'rada upp' (=list something)
gridrasterrasterbreaks (in school or at a workplace, i.e. for example coffee breaks)
ruleregelregellatch
travels (present tense of 'resa')reserresorjourneys, trips
the melee, the fightingrivetrivettorn
cylinder that rotates and is used for paintingrollerrollerroles
diamonds (in card games)ruterrutorsquares, (window) panes
the routeruttenruttenrotten
the ryerågenrågenthe overmeasure
the nymphsrånarånarob
the horizontal bars (gymnastics)räckenräckenrailings
the horizontal bar (gymnastics)räcketräcketthe railing
runsrännerrännorchutes
sabbathsabbatsabbatdestroyed, sabotaged (past participle of 'sabba')
cedarcedersedercustoms (traditions)
the CsC:nasenalate (plural of 'sen'), sinew
cidercidersidorpages
the viewsiktensiktensights (on rifles, plural of 'sikte')
the barks (dog sounds)skallenskallenthe skull
the spoonskedenskedenstages (of time)
the shiftskiftetskiftetthe change
the shiftsskiftenskiftenchanges
the ejaculationsskjutenskjutenshot (past participle of 'skjuta')
the speed, the ejaculationskjutetskjutetshot (past participle of 'skjuta')
the shotsskottenskottenthe Scotsman
the (rain) showerskurenskurencut (past participle of 'skära')
the gunnerskyttenskyttenacts of shooting
the battles, the hitsslagenslagenbeaten
the battle, the hitslagetslagetbeaten
the toilslitetslitetworn
the endsslutenslutenclosed (past participle of 'sluta')
the endslutetslutetclosed (past participle of 'sluta')
the (extended) familysläktensläktengenera (biology)
the copsnutensnutenpast participle of 'snyta' (=blow one's nose)
the zooszoonasonaexpiate
the stocks (cooking)spadenspadenthe spade
the spasspanaspanawatch, observe, search
bursts, cracks (present tense of the verb 'spricka')sprickersprickorcracks (plural of the noun 'spricka')
the stepsstegenstegenthe ladder
fights (present tense of 'strida')striderstriderfights, battles (plural of the noun 'strid')
the moving patches/bands (of something)stråkenstråkenthe bow (for a violin)
the stubblestubbenstubbenthe tree stump
the racksställenställenplaces (locations)
the rackställetställetthe place
the sucking devicesugensugensucked (past participle of 'suga'), in the mood for something
the urgesugetsugetsucked (past participle of 'suga'), in the mood for something
the seed, the grainsädensädenthings intended for sowing (plural of 'säde')
the cellcellensällenthe brute
the gripstagentagentaken
the griptagettagettaken
the tanktankentankenthe thought
tonertonertonertones
the trottraventraventhe pile, the stack
the plot (of land)tomtentomtenSanta Claus, the gnome
the inchtummentummenthe thumb
signteckentäckenbed covers
the point, the cuspuddenuddenthe headland
uphilluppföruppförpresent tense or imperative of 'uppföra' (=set up a theatre play, behave)
downhillutförutförpresent tense or imperative of 'utföra' (=carry out)
the hole in the icevakenvakenawake
the whalevalenvalenstiff, numb
the shrouds (sailing)vantenvantenthe mitten
the vasevasenvasenthe bundle of brushwood
the bayvikenvikenfolded (past participle of 'vika')
makes a whistling sound (of for example wind)vinervinerwines
the knobsvredenvredenthe rage, the wrath
the host/the worldvärden/världenvärdenvalues
male nameOskaråskarpresent tense of 'åska' (=thunder)
the gravelörenörenpennies (plural of the monetary unit 'öre' used when no numeral immediately precedes the word)
the gravelöretöretthe penny (1/100 of a Swedish krona)

Note that karaten/karaten is the only pair with more than two syllables (although we would get a second one if we used the definite forms of the pair perser/pärser, i.e. perserna/pärserna). The word pair länder ('countries', plural of land) and länder ('loins', plural of länd) could have been included, but this one is controversial. [78] [ circular reference ] For those speakers who have grave accent in the plural of länd, the definite plural forms will also constitute a three-syllable minimal pair: länderna (acute accent, 'the countries') vs. länderna (grave accent, 'the loins'). Although examples with more than two syllables are very few in Standard Swedish, it is possible to find other three-syllable pairs in regional dialects, such as Värmländska: hunnera (acute, 'the Huns') vs. hunnera (grave, 'the dogs'), ändera/ännera (acute, 'the mallards') vs. ändera/ännera (grave, 'the ends'), etc.

Prosody in Swedish often varies substantially between different dialects including the spoken varieties of Standard Swedish. As in most languages, stress can be applied to emphasize certain words in a sentence. To some degree prosody may indicate questions, although less so than in English.

Phonotactics

At a minimum, a stressed syllable must consist of either a long vowel or a short vowel and a long consonant. [79] Like many other Germanic languages, Swedish has a tendency for closed syllables with a relatively large number of consonant clusters in initial as well as final position. Though not as complex as that of most Slavic languages, examples of up to 7 consecutive consonants can occur when adding Swedish inflections to some foreign loanwords or names, and especially when combined with the tendency of Swedish to make long compound nouns. The syllable structure of Swedish can therefore be described with the following formula:

(C)(C)(C)V(C)(C)(C)

This means that a Swedish one-syllable morpheme can have up to three consonants preceding the vowel that forms the nucleus of the syllable, and three consonants following it. Examples: skrämts/skrɛmts/ (verb 'scare' past participle, passive voice) or sprängts/sprɛŋts/ (verb 'explode' past participle, passive voice). All but one of the consonant phonemes, /ŋ/, can occur at the beginning of a morpheme, though there are only 6 possible three-consonant combinations, all of which begin with /s/, and a total of 31 initial two-consonant combinations. All consonants except for /h/ and /ɕ/ can occur finally, and the total number of possible final two-consonant clusters is 62.

In some cases this can result in very complex combinations, such as in västkustskt /ˈvɛ̂stˌkɵstskt/ , consisting of västkust ('west coast') with the adjective suffix -sk and the neuter suffix -t. [80]

Central Standard Swedish and most other Swedish dialects feature a rare "complementary quantity" feature [81] wherein a phonologically short consonant follows a long vowel and a long consonant follows a short vowel; this is true only for stressed syllables and all segments are short in unstressed syllables. [34] [37] This arose from the historical shift away from a system with a four-way contrast (that is, VːCː, VC, VːC and VCː were all possible) inherited from Proto-Germanic to a three-way one (VC, VːC and VCː), and finally the present two-way one; certain Swedish dialects have not undergone these shifts and exhibit one of the other two phonotactic systems instead. [82] In literature on Swedish phonology, there are a number of ways to transcribe complementary relationship, including: [83]

With the conventional assumption that medial long consonants are ambisyllabic (that is, penna ('pen'), is syllabified as [ˈpɛ̂n.na]), all stressed syllables are thus "heavy". [83] In unstressed syllables, the distinction is lost between /u/ and /o/ or between /e//ɛ/. [29] With each successive post-stress syllable, the number of contrasting vowels decreases gradually with distance from the point of stress; at three syllables from stress, only [a] and [ɛ] occur. [80]

Sample

The sample text is a reading of The North Wind and the Sun. The transcriptions are based on the section on Swedish found in The Handbook on the International Phonetic Association, in which a man in his forties from Stockholm is recorded reading out the traditional fable in a manner typical of Central Standard Swedish as spoken in his area. The broad transcription is phonemic, while the narrow is phonetic. [86]

Broad transcription

/nuːrdanvɪndɛnɔsuːlɛntvɪstadɛɛnɡɔŋːɔmvɛmɑːvdɔmsɔmvɑːrstarkast||jɵstdoːkɔmɛnvandrarɛvɛːɡɛnfram|ɪnsveːptɛnvarmkapːa||dɔmkɔmdoːøːvɛrɛnsɔm|atdɛnsɔmfœrstkɵndɛfoːvandrarɛnattaɑːvsɛjkapːan|hanskɵlːɛanseːsvɑːrastarkarɛɛndɛnandra||doːbloːstɛnuːrdanvɪndɛnhoːrthannɔnsɪnkɵndɛ|mɛnjʉːhoːrdarɛhanbloːstɛdɛstʊtɛːtarɛsveːptɛvandrarɛnkapːanɔmsɛj|ɔtɪlslʉːtɡɑːvnuːrdanvɪndɛnɵpːfœrsøːkɛt||doːlɛːtsuːlɛnsiːnastroːlarɧiːnaheːltvarmtɔjeːnasttuːɡvandrarɛnɑːvsɛjkapːan|ɔsoːvɑːrnuːrdanvɪndɛntvɵŋɛnateːrɕɛnːaatsuːlɛnvɑːrdɛnstarkastɛɑːvdɔmtvoː/

Narrow transcription

[ˈnuːɖaɱˌvɪnˑdɛnɔˈsuːlɛnˈtv̥ɪsːtadɛɛŋˈɡɔŋːɔɱˈvɛmˑɑvˌdɔmsɔɱvɑˑˈstaɹːcast||ˈʝɵsˑtˈd̥oːkʰɔmɛɱˈvanːdɾaɾɛˈvɛːɡəɱfɾam|ˈɪnˌsv̥eə̯ptɛɱvaɹˑmˈcʰapːa||dɔmkʰɔmdoˑøə̯vɛˈɾɛnːsˈɔmˑatdɛnsɔmˈfɵʂːʈkʰɵnˑdɛfoˑˈvanːdɹ̝aɹɛnatˈtʰɑːɑˑvsɛjˈcʰapːan|hanˑskɵlˑɛˈanːˌseːsvɑˑˈstaɹːcaɾɛɛndɛnˈandɾa||doːˈbloə̯stɛˈnuːɖaɱˌvɪnˑdɛnsoˑˈhoːʈhanˈnɔnːˌsɪŋˈkʰɵnːdɛ|mɛɳʝʉˑˈhoːɖaɾɛhamˈbloə̯stɛ|dɛsˑtʊˈtʰɛːtaɾɛˈsv̥eə̯ptɛˈvanːdɹ̝aɹɛŋˈcʰapːanˈɔmˑsɛj|ɔtʰɪlˈslʉːtɡɑˑvˈnuːɖaɱˌvɪnˑdɛnˈɵpːfœ̞ˈʂøə̯cɛt||doˑlɛˑtˈsuːlɛnsiˑnaˈstɾoːlaɹˈɧiːnaheˑltˈvaɹːmt|ɔˈʝeːnasttʰuˑɡˈvanːdɹ̝aɹɛnˈɑːvsɛjˈcʰapːan|ɔsoˑvɑˑˈnuːɖaɱˌvɪnˑdɛnˈtvɵŋːɛnatˈeːɹˌɕɛnːaatˈsuːlɛnvɑˑdɛnˈstaɹːcastɛɑˑvdɔmˈtv̥oː]

Orthographic version

Nordanvinden och solen tvistade en gång om vem av dem som var starkast. Just då kom en vandrare vägen fram, insvept i en varm kappa. De kom då överens om att den som först kunde få vandraren att ta av sig kappan, han skulle anses vara starkare än den andra. Då blåste nordanvinden så hårt han någonsin kunde, men ju hårdare han blåste, desto tätare svepte vandraren kappan om sig, och till slut gav nordanvinden upp försöket. Då lät solen sina strålar skina helt varmt och genast tog vandraren av sig kappan, och så var nordanvinden tvungen att erkänna att solen var den starkaste av de två.

Notes

  1. 1 2 3 Andersson (2002), p. 272.
  2. Schaeffler (2005), p. 26; citing Elert (1964), Gårding (1974), and Bannert (1976).
  3. Schaeffler (2005), pp. 7–8.
  4. 1 2 3 Engstrand (1999), p. 140.
  5. Thorén & Petterson (1992), p. 15.
  6. 1 2 Ladefoged & Maddieson (1996), pp. 295–6.
  7. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Engstrand (1999), p. 141.
  8. Elmquist (1915), p. 31.
  9. Thorén & Petterson (1992), pp. 11–2, 14–5, 17–8.
  10. 1 2 Riad (2014), p. 27.
  11. Elmquist (1915), p. 33.
  12. Thorén & Petterson (1992), pp. 8–11, 13–4, 16–7.
  13. Eliasson (1986), p. 273.
  14. Thorén & Petterson (1992), pp. 13–5.
  15. 1 2 3 4 5 Riad (2014), p. 38.
  16. Engstrand (2004), pp. 115–6.
  17. Riad (2014), pp. 29, 38–9.
  18. Riad (2014), pp. 22, 48–9.
  19. Fant (1983), p. 2.
  20. 1 2 Riad (2014), pp. 35–6.
  21. Ladefoged & Maddieson (1996), p. 292. The symbols "i" and "e" used in the original citation were changed to /iː/ and /eː/ to keep this article consistent.
  22. Cited in Schaeffler (2005 , p. 8).
  23. McAllister, Lubker & Carlson (1974); cited in Ladefoged & Maddieson (1996, p. 295).
  24. Elert (2000), pp. 38–43.
  25. Table adapted from Engstrand (2004 , p. 167).
  26. Riad (2014), pp. 46, 67.
  27. Riad (2014), pp. 46, 58.
  28. 1 2 Riad (2014), p. 46.
  29. 1 2 3 4 5 Andersson (2002), p. 273.
  30. 1 2 Ringen & Suomi (2012).
  31. 1 2 Helgason (1998), p. 53.
  32. Ringen & Helgason (2004), p. 56.
  33. 1 2 3 Helgason (1999a), p. 80.
  34. 1 2 Tronnier (2002), p. 33.
  35. Helgason (1999b), p. 1851.
  36. Helgason (1999b), p. 1854.
  37. 1 2 Wretling, Strangert & Schaeffler (2002), p. 703; citing Helgason (1999a).
  38. Wretling, Strangert & Schaeffler (2002), p. 706.
  39. Helgason (1999b), p. 1853.
  40. Ringen & Helgason (2004), p. 59.
  41. Petrova et al. (2006), p. 20; citing Ringen & Helgason (2004).
  42. 1 2 Liberman (1978), pp. 64ff.
  43. Wretling, Strangert & Schaeffler (2002), p. 704.
  44. Helgason (1999b), pp. 1852–3.
  45. Engstrand (1999), pp. 140–1.
  46. Engstrand (2004), p. 167.
  47. 1 2 3 4 Adams (1975), p. 289.
  48. Ladefoged & Maddieson (1996), pp. 171–2, 329–30.
  49. Garlén (1988), pp. 71–2.
  50. 1 2 Elert (2000).
  51. Ladefoged & Maddieson (1996), pp. 225–6.
  52. Riad (2014), pp. 68, 75.
  53. Table modified from Hamann (2003 , p. 84), citing Eliasson (1986).
  54. Eliasson (1986), pp. 278–9.
  55. "Postalveolarization" and "supradentalization" are also common terms.
  56. 1 2 3 Andersson (2002), p. 274.
  57. Eliasson (1986), p. 279.
  58. Hamann (2003), p. 84; citing Eliasson (1986, p. 282).
  59. Those south of Kalmar, Jönköping and Falkenberg; a little north of these cities, a uvular rhotic appears in initial position and as a long consonant ( Andersson 2002 , p. 273).
  60. Garlén (1988), pp. 73–4.
  61. Eliasson (1986), p. 281.
  62. Riad (2014), p. 73.
  63. Reuter (1992), p. 108.
  64. Andersson (2002), pp. 273–4.
  65. Eliasson (1986), p. 276.
  66. 1 2 Bruce (1993).
  67. 1 2 3 4 Bruce & Hermans (1999).
  68. 1 2 Riad (2014).
  69. Frid (2001).
  70. Swedish Academy (1893-2023).
  71. Gårding (1973).
  72. Gårding (1974).
  73. 1 2 3 Bruce (1977).
  74. 1 2 Bruce (2010).
  75. 1 2 Liberman (1982), p. 13.
  76. Engstrand (2004), pp. 186–90.
  77. Translated from a Swedish-only Wikipedia article.
  78. From the Discussion section of the Swedish article.
  79. Schaeffler (2005), p. 7.
  80. 1 2 Garlén (1988), pp. 101–14.
  81. Schaeffler (2005), p. 9.
  82. Schaeffler (2005), p. 39.
  83. 1 2 Schaeffler (2005), p. 8; citing Elert (1964).
  84. E.g. Elert (1964 , p. 43).
  85. E.g. Eliasson & La Pelle (1973) and Riad (1992).
  86. Engstrand (1999), pp. 140–2.

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Diacritic</span> Modifier mark added to a letter

A diacritic is a glyph added to a letter or to a basic glyph. The term derives from the Ancient Greek διακριτικός, from διακρίνω. The word diacritic is a noun, though it is sometimes used in an attributive sense, whereas diacritical is only an adjective. Some diacritics, such as the acute ⟨ó⟩, grave ⟨ò⟩, and circumflex ⟨ô⟩, are often called accents. Diacritics may appear above or below a letter or in some other position such as within the letter or between two letters.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">International Phonetic Alphabet</span> System of phonetic notation

The International Phonetic Alphabet (IPA) is an alphabetic system of phonetic notation based primarily on the Latin script. It was devised by the International Phonetic Association in the late 19th century as a standard written representation for the sounds of speech. The IPA is used by lexicographers, foreign language students and teachers, linguists, speech–language pathologists, singers, actors, constructed language creators, and translators.

The circumflex is a diacritic in the Latin and Greek scripts that is also used in the written forms of many languages and in various romanization and transcription schemes. It received its English name from Latin: circumflexus "bent around"—a translation of the Ancient Greek: περισπωμένη.

A pitch-accent language is a type of language that, when spoken, has certain syllables in words or morphemes that are prominent, as indicated by a distinct contrasting pitch rather than by loudness or length, as in some other languages like English. Pitch-accent languages also contrast with fully tonal languages like Vietnamese, Thai and Standard Chinese, in which practically every syllable can have an independent tone. Some scholars have claimed that the term "pitch accent" is not coherently defined and that pitch-accent languages are just a sub-category of tonal languages in general.

English phonology is the system of speech sounds used in spoken English. Like many other languages, English has wide variation in pronunciation, both historically and from dialect to dialect. In general, however, the regional dialects of English share a largely similar phonological system. Among other things, most dialects have vowel reduction in unstressed syllables and a complex set of phonological features that distinguish fortis and lenis consonants.

The sound system of Norwegian resembles that of Swedish. There is considerable variation among the dialects, and all pronunciations are considered by official policy to be equally correct – there is no official spoken standard, although it can be said that Eastern Norwegian Bokmål speech has an unofficial spoken standard, called Urban East Norwegian or Standard East Norwegian, loosely based on the speech of the literate classes of the Oslo area. This variant is the most common one taught to foreign students.

Proto-Balto-Slavic is a reconstructed hypothetical proto-language descending from Proto-Indo-European (PIE). From Proto-Balto-Slavic, the later Balto-Slavic languages are thought to have developed, composed of the Baltic and Slavic sub-branches, and including modern Lithuanian, Polish, Russian and Serbo-Croatian, among others.

Stød is a suprasegmental unit of Danish phonology, which in its most common form is a kind of creaky voice (laryngealization), but it may also be realized as a glottal stop, especially in emphatic pronunciation. Some dialects of Southern Danish realize stød in a way that is more similar to the tonal word accents of Norwegian and Swedish. In much of Zealand it is regularly realized as reminiscent of a glottal stop. A probably unrelated glottal stop, with quite different distribution rules, occurs in Western Jutland and is known as the vestjysk stød. The noun stød itself does not have a stød.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Portuguese orthography</span> Alphabet and spelling

Portuguese orthography is based on the Latin alphabet and makes use of the acute accent, the circumflex accent, the grave accent, the tilde, and the cedilla to denote stress, vowel height, nasalization, and other sound changes. The diaeresis was abolished by the last Orthography Agreement. Accented letters and digraphs are not counted as separate characters for collation purposes.

Dutch phonology is similar to that of other West Germanic languages, especially Afrikaans and West Frisian.

The phonology of Turkish deals with current phonology and phonetics, particularly of Istanbul Turkish. A notable feature of the phonology of Turkish is a system of vowel harmony that causes vowels in most words to be either front or back and either rounded or unrounded. Velar stop consonants have palatal allophones before front vowels.

The phonology of Standard Chinese has historically derived from the Beijing dialect of Mandarin. However, pronunciation varies widely among speakers, who may introduce elements of their local varieties. Television and radio announcers are chosen for their ability to affect a standard accent. Elements of the sound system include not only the segments—e.g. vowels and consonants—of the language, but also the tones applied to each syllable. In addition to its four main tones, Standard Chinese has a neutral tone that appears on weak syllables.

Lithuanian has 11 vowels and 45 consonants, including 22 pairs of consonants distinguished by the presence or absence of palatalization. Most vowels come in pairs which are differentiated through length and degree of centralization.

Wahgi is a Trans–New Guinea language of the Chimbu–Wahgi branch spoken by approximately 100,000 people in the highlands of Papua New Guinea. Like other Chimbu languages, Wahgi has some unusual lateral consonants.

This article is about the phonology and phonetics of standard Slovene.

Proto-Slavic accent is the accentual system of Proto-Slavic and is closely related to the accentual system of some Baltic languages with which it shares many common innovations that occurred in the Proto-Balto-Slavic period. Deeper, it inherits from the Proto-Indo-European accent. In modern languages the prototypical accent is reflected in various ways, some preserving the Proto-Slavic situation to a greater degree than others.

This article describes the phonology of the Oromo language.

Kerkrade dialect is a Ripuarian dialect spoken in Kerkrade and its surroundings, including Herzogenrath in Germany. It is spoken in all social classes, but the variety spoken by younger people in Kerkrade is somewhat closer to Standard Dutch.

Amongst the Iranian languages, the phonology of Pashto is of middle complexity, but its morphology is very complex.

The phonology of the Maastrichtian dialect, especially with regards to vowels is quite extensive due to the dialect's tonal nature.

References

Further reading