Biology and political orientation

Last updated

A number of studies have found that human biology can be linked with political orientation. [1] This means that an individual's biology may predispose them to a particular political orientation and ideology or, conversely, that subscription to certain ideologies may predispose them to measurable biological and health outcomes.

Contents

Studies have found that subjects with right-wing (or conservative in the United States) political views have larger amygdalae, [2] report larger social networks and greater happiness than liberals, are more prone to express disgust to moral infringements and are more sensitive to perceived threats. [3] [4] [5] Those with left-wing (or liberal in the United States) political views have a larger volume of grey matter in the anterior cingulate cortex [2] and are more likely to report greater relationship dissatisfaction and emotional distress than conservatives, to show more openness to experience as well as greater tolerance for uncertainty and disorder. [6] [7]

Genetic factors account for at least some of the variation of political views. [8] [9] From the perspective of evolutionary psychology, conflicts regarding redistribution of wealth may have been common in the ancestral environment and humans may have developed psychological mechanisms for judging their own chances of succeeding in such conflicts. These mechanisms may affect political views. However, many of the studies linking biology to politics remain controversial and unreplicated. [10] [11]

Brain studies

In the 2011 study by neuroscientist Ryota Kanai (n=90 students), the subjects who expressed conservative views (right) tended to have a larger amygdala than those who expressed liberal views (left). Amygdalae and Political Orientation.svg
In the 2011 study by neuroscientist Ryota Kanai (n=90 students), the subjects who expressed conservative views (right) tended to have a larger amygdala than those who expressed liberal views (left).

A 2011 study by cognitive neuroscientist Ryota Kanai at University College London found structural brain differences between subjects of different political orientation in a convenience sample of students at the same college. [2] The researchers performed MRI scans on the brains of 90 volunteer students who had indicated their political orientation on a five-point scale ranging from "very liberal" to "very conservative". [2] [12]

Students who reported more conservative political views were found to have larger amygdala, [2] a structure in the temporal lobes whose primary function is in the formation, consolidation and processing of memory, as well as positive and negative conditioning (emotional learning). [13] The amygdala is responsible for important roles in social interaction, such as the recognition of emotional cues in facial expressions and the monitoring of personal space, [14] [15] with larger amygdalae correlating with larger and more complex social networks. [16] [17] It is also postulated to play a role in threat detection, including modulation of fear and aggression to perceived threats. [18] [19] [20] Conservative students were also found to have greater volume of gray matter in the left insula and the right entorhinal cortex. [2] There is evidence that conservatives are more sensitive to disgust [3] and one role of the insula is in the modulation of social emotions, such as the feeling of disgust to specific sights, smells and norm violations. [21] [22] [23]

Students who reported more liberal political views were found to have a larger volume of grey matter in the anterior cingulate cortex, [2] a structure of the brain associated with emotional awareness and the emotional processing of pain. [24] [25] The anterior cingulate cortex becomes active in situations of uncertainty, [26] and is postulated to play a role in error detection, such as the monitoring and processing of conflicting stimuli or information. [27]

The authors concluded, "Although our data do not determine whether these regions play a causal role in the formation of political attitudes, they converge with previous work to suggest a possible link between brain structure and psychological mechanisms that mediate political attitudes." [2] In an interview with LiveScience, Ryota Kanai said, "It's very unlikely that actual political orientation is directly encoded in these brain regions", and that "more work is needed to determine how these brain structures mediate the formation of political attitude." [1] [12] [28] [29] Kanai and colleagues added that it is necessary to conduct a longitudinal study to determine whether the changes in brain structure that we observed lead to changes in political behavior or whether political attitudes and behavior instead result in changes of brain structure.

Functional differences

Psychometrics

Various studies suggest measurable differences in the psychological traits of liberals and conservatives. Conservatives are more likely to report larger social networks, greater happiness and self-esteem than liberals, are more reactive to perceived threats and more likely to interpret ambiguous facial expressions as threatening. [5] [6] [4] [7] Liberals are more likely to report greater emotional distress, relationship dissatisfaction and experiential hardship than conservatives, and show more openness to experience as well as greater tolerance for uncertainty and disorder. [6] [7] [5]

Behavioral studies

A study by David Amodio et al. at New York University and the University of California, Los Angeles, found differences in how self-described liberal and conservative research participants responded to changes in patterns. [30] Participants were asked to tap a keyboard when the letter "M" appeared on a computer monitor and to refrain from tapping when they saw a "W". The letter "M" appeared four times more frequently than "W", conditioning participants to press the keyboard when a letter appears. Liberal participants made fewer mistakes than conservatives during testing and their electroencephalograph readings showed more activity in the anterior cingulate cortex, the part of the brain that deals with conflicting information, during the experiment, suggesting that they were better able to detect conflicts in established patterns. Amodio warned against concluding that a particular political orientation is superior. He said: "The tendency of conservatives to block distracting information could be a good thing depending on the situation." [31] [32]

A 2017 study both replicated the original study and also found that conservatives performed better in a task in which choosing the simple strategy was the more optimal solution; while both liberals and conservatives started the task attempting the more complex but less effective strategy, conservatives switched to the simple strategy more quickly than liberals. [33] Amodio's original study was also replicated by Weissflog et al. (2013) [34] and Inzlicht et al. (2009). [35] Conversely, Kremláček et al. (2019) [36] and Wendell (2016) [37] did not replicate Amodio's results. Both studies also argued that Weissflog and Inzlicht's results were not as concordant with the Amodio's findings as originally claimed.

A study of subjects' reported level of disgust linked to various scenarios showed that people who scored highly on the "disgust sensitivity" scale held more politically conservative views, [3] which some researchers believe could be partially explained by personality traits. [38] However, the findings of a 2019 study suggest that sensitivity to disgust among conservatives varies according to the elicitors used, and that using an elicitor-unspecific scale caused the differences in sensitivity to disappear between those of different political orientations. [39]

A 2018 study in the United States looking at levels of cognitive reflection (the tendency to favour analytic reasoning over instinctive or "gut" responses) found that those who voted for Donald Trump in the 2016 US presidential election had lower levels of cognitive reflection than Hillary Clinton voters or third-party voters. However, this effect was mostly driven by Democrats who voted for Trump, while amongst Republicans, Clinton and Trump voters had more similar levels of cognitive reflection. Republicans who voted for third-party candidates or those who identified as libertarian had the highest levels of cognitive reflection. [40]

Physiology

People with right-wing views had greater skin conductance response, indicating greater sympathetic nervous system response, to threatening images than those with left-wing views in one study. [41] [1] There was no difference for positive or neutral images. Holding right-wing views was also associated with a stronger startle reflex as measured by strength of eyeblink in response to unexpected noise. Subsequent studies with substantially greater statistical power have failed to replicate these effects. [42] [43]

In an fMRI study published in Social Neuroscience , three different patterns of brain activation were found to correlate with individualism, conservatism, and radicalism. [44] In general, fMRI responses in several portions of the brain have been linked to viewing of the faces of well-known politicians. [45] Others believe that determining political affiliation from fMRI data is overreaching. [46]

Genetic studies

Heritability

Heritability compares differences in genetic factors in individuals to the total variance of observable characteristics ("phenotypes") in a population, to determine the heritability coefficient. Factors including genetics, environment and random chance can all contribute to the variation in individuals' phenotypes. [47]

The use of twin studies assumes the elimination of non-genetic differences by finding the statistical differences between monozygotic (identical) twins, which have almost the same genes, and dizygotic (fraternal) twins. [48] The similarity of the environment in which twins are reared has been questioned. [49] [50]

A twin study in 2005 by Alford et al. examined the attitudes regarding 28 different political issues such as capitalism, unions, X-rated movies, abortion, school prayer, divorce, property taxes, and the draft. Twins were asked if they agreed or disagreed or were uncertain about each issue. Genetic factors accounted for 53% of the variance of an overall score. However, self-identification as Republican and Democrat had a much lower heritability of 14%. It is worthwhile to note that identical twins correlated in opinion at a rate of 0.66 while fraternal twins correlated in opinion by 0.44. This likely occurs because identical twins share 100% of their DNA while fraternal twins share on average only 50% of their DNA. [51] [52] However, Jonathan Kaplan argued that the role of individual genes is often extremely small due to many human physical traits being polygenic and may be overstated, [53] observing that the study by Alford et al. made a case for the role of the 5-HTTLPR region being involved in numerous psychological and personality traits, yet Border et al. (2019) found that multiple associations with 5-HTTLPR were spurious and underpowered. [54]

Jost et al. wrote in a 2011 review that "Many studies involving quite diverse samples and methods suggest that political and religious views reflect a reasonably strong genetic basis, but this does not mean that ideological proclivities are unaffected by personal experiences or environmental factors." [1]

Gene association studies

In 2014, a study was performed on genomic data from 12,000 twin pairs from Australia, the USA, Denmark, Sweden and Hungary to examine genetic influences on political ideology. The study's genome-wide association analysis did not provide any definitive evidence of a specific genetic marker related to ideology. The authors remarked that, as with any complex trait, a single gene or small group of genes would not influence ideology directly but that there would likely be "thousands of genetic variants of very small effects and constellations of genes interacting with each other and the environment to influence behavior, indirectly". [55]

"A Genome-Wide Analysis of Liberal and Conservative Political Attitudes" by Peter K. Hatemi et al. traces DNA research involving 13,000 subjects. The study identifies several genes potentially[ qualify evidence ] connected with political ideology. [56]

Evolutionary psychology

From an evolutionary psychology perspective, conflicts regarding redistribution of wealth may have been a recurrent issue in the ancestral environment. Humans may therefore have developed psychological mechanisms for judging their chance of succeeding in such conflicts which will affect their political views. For males, physical strength may have been an important factor in deciding the outcome of such conflicts. Therefore, a prediction is that males that have high physical strength and low socioeconomic status (SES) will support redistribution while males that have both high SES and high physical strength will oppose redistribution. Cross-cultural research found this to be the case; for females, their physical strength had no influence on their political views which was as expected since females rarely have physical strength above that of the average male. [57] A study on political attitudes among Hollywood actors found that, while the actors were generally more left-leaning, male actors with great physical strength were more likely to support the Republican stance on foreign issues and foreign military interventions. [58]

An alternative evolutionary explanation for political diversity is that it is a polymorphism, like those of gender and blood type, resulting from frequency-dependent selection. Tim Dean has suggested that we live in such a moral ecosystem whereby the advantage gained by having any one particular moral strategy diminishes as it becomes very common, causing evolution to produce individuals with a diversity of moral strategies. [59] Alford et al. posit that political variation could offer groups different strategies of solving problems, thus variation is maintained by virtue of being adaptive at the group level. [60]

See also

Related Research Articles

A political spectrum is a system to characterize and classify different political positions in relation to one another. These positions sit upon one or more geometric axes that represent independent political dimensions. The expressions political compass and political map are used to refer to the political spectrum as well, especially to popular two-dimensional models of it.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Anterior cingulate cortex</span> Brain region

In the human brain, the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) is the frontal part of the cingulate cortex that resembles a "collar" surrounding the frontal part of the corpus callosum. It consists of Brodmann areas 24, 32, and 33.

Aging of the brain is a process of transformation of the brain in older age, including changes all individuals experience and those of illness. Usually this refers to humans.

Social dominance orientation (SDO) is a personality trait measuring an individual's support for social hierarchy and the extent to which they desire their in-group be superior to out-groups. SDO is conceptualized under social dominance theory as a measure of individual differences in levels of group-based discrimination; that is, it is a measure of an individual's preference for hierarchy within any social system and the domination over lower-status groups. It is a predisposition toward anti-egalitarianism within and between groups.

In psychology, right-wing authoritarianism (RWA) is a set of attitudes, describing somebody who is highly submissive to their authority figures, acts aggressively in the name of said authorities, and is conformist in thought and behavior. The prevalence of this attitude in a population varies from culture to culture, as a person's upbringing and education play a strong role in determining whether somebody develops this sort of worldview.

Affective neuroscience is the study of how the brain processes emotions. This field combines neuroscience with the psychological study of personality, emotion, and mood. The basis of emotions and what emotions are remains an issue of debate within the field of affective neuroscience.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Posterior cingulate cortex</span> Caudal part of the cingulate cortex of the brain

The posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) is the caudal part of the cingulate cortex, located posterior to the anterior cingulate cortex. This is the upper part of the "limbic lobe". The cingulate cortex is made up of an area around the midline of the brain. Surrounding areas include the retrosplenial cortex and the precuneus.

The study of height and intelligence examines correlations between human height and human intelligence. Some epidemiological research on the subject has shown that there is a small but statistically significant positive correlation between height and intelligence after controlling for socioeconomic class and parental education. The cited study, however, does not draw any conclusions about height and intelligence, but rather suggests "a continuing effect of post-natal growth on childhood cognition beyond the age of 9 years." This correlation arises in both the developed and developing world and persists across age groups. An individual's taller stature has been attributed to higher economic status, which often translates to a higher quality of nutrition. This correlation, however, can be inverted to characterize one's socioeconomic status as a consequence of stature, where shorter stature can attract discrimination that affects many factors, among them employment, and treatment by educators. One such theory argues that since height strongly correlates with white and gray matter volume, it may act as a biomarker for cerebral development which itself mediates intelligence. Competing explanations include that certain genetic factors may influence both height and intelligence, or that both height and intelligence may be affected in similar ways by adverse environmental exposures during development. Measurements of the total surface area and mean thickness of the cortical grey matter using a magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) revealed that the height of individuals had a positive correlation with the total cortical surface area. This supports the idea that genes that influence height also influence total surface area of the brain, which in turn influences intelligence, resulting in the correlation. Other explanations further qualify the positive correlation between height and intelligence, suggesting that because the correlation becomes weaker with higher socioeconomic class and education level, environmental factors could partially override any genetic factors affecting both characteristics.

Neuroticism is a personality trait associated with negative emotions. It is one of the Big Five traits. Individuals with high scores on neuroticism are more likely than average to experience such feelings as anxiety, worry, fear, anger, frustration, envy, jealousy, pessimism, guilt, depressed mood, and loneliness. Such people are thought to respond worse to stressors and are more likely to interpret ordinary situations, such as minor frustrations, as appearing hopelessly difficult. Their behavioral responses may include procrastination, substance use, and other maladaptive behaviors, which may temporarily aid in relieving negative emotions and generating positive ones.

The simulation theory of empathy holds that humans anticipate and make sense of the behavior of others by activating mental processes that, if they culminated in action, would produce similar behavior. This includes intentional behavior as well as the expression of emotions. The theory says that children use their own emotions to predict what others will do; we project our own mental states onto others.

Genopolitics is the study of the genetic basis of political behavior and attitudes. It combines behavior genetics, psychology, and political science and it is closely related to the emerging fields of neuropolitics and political physiology.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Disgust</span> Basic emotion

Disgust is an emotional response of rejection or revulsion to something potentially contagious or something considered offensive, distasteful or unpleasant. In The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals, Charles Darwin wrote that disgust is a sensation that refers to something revolting. Disgust is experienced primarily in relation to the sense of taste, and secondarily to anything which causes a similar feeling by sense of smell, touch, or vision. Musically sensitive people may even be disgusted by the cacophony of inharmonious sounds. Research has continually proven a relationship between disgust and anxiety disorders such as arachnophobia, blood-injection-injury type phobias, and contamination fear related obsessive–compulsive disorder.

Cultural neuroscience is a field of research that focuses on the interrelation between a human's cultural environment and neurobiological systems. The field particularly incorporates ideas and perspectives from related domains like anthropology, psychology, and cognitive neuroscience to study sociocultural influences on human behaviors. Such impacts on behavior are often measured using various neuroimaging methods, through which cross-cultural variability in neural activity can be examined.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Brain activity and meditation</span>

Meditation and its effect on brain activity and the central nervous system became a focus of collaborative research in neuroscience, psychology and neurobiology during the latter half of the 20th century. Research on meditation sought to define and characterize various practices. The effects of meditation on the brain can be broken up into two categories: state changes and trait changes, respectively alterations in brain activities during the act of meditating and changes that are the outcome of long-term practice.

Neuropolitics is a science which investigates the interplay between the brain and politics. It combines work from a variety of scientific fields which includes neuroscience, political science, psychology, behavioral genetics, primatology, and ethology. Often, neuropolitics research borrow methods from cognitive neuroscience to investigate classic questions from political science such as how people make political decisions, form political / ideological attitudes, evaluate political candidates, and interact in political coalitions. However, another line of research considers the role that evolving political competition has had on the development of the brain in humans and other species. The research in neuropolitics often intersects with work in genopolitics, political psychology, political physiology, sociobiology, neuroeconomics, and neurolaw.

The interdisciplinary study of biology and political science is the application of theories and methods from the field of biology toward the scientific understanding of political behavior. The field is sometimes called biopolitics, a term that will be used in this article as a synonym although it has other, less related meanings. More generally, the field has also been called "politics and the life sciences".

Moral foundations theory is a social psychological theory intended to explain the origins of and variation in human moral reasoning on the basis of innate, modular foundations. It was first proposed by the psychologists Jonathan Haidt, Craig Joseph, and Jesse Graham, building on the work of cultural anthropologist Richard Shweder. More recently, Mohammad Atari, Jesse Graham, and Jonathan Haidt have revised some aspects of the theory and developed new measurement tools. The theory has been developed by a diverse group of collaborators and popularized in Haidt's book The Righteous Mind. The theory proposes that morality is "more than one thing", first arguing for five foundations, and later expanding for six foundations :

Neuroimaging intelligence testing concerns the use of neuroimaging techniques to evaluate human intelligence. Neuroimaging technology has advanced such that scientists hope to use neuroimaging increasingly for investigations of brain function related to IQ.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Mechanisms of mindfulness meditation</span>

Mindfulness has been defined in modern psychological terms as "paying attention to relevant aspects of experience in a nonjudgmental manner", and maintaining attention on present moment experience with an attitude of openness and acceptance. Meditation is a platform used to achieve mindfulness. Both practices, mindfulness and meditation, have been "directly inspired from the Buddhist tradition" and have been widely promoted by Jon Kabat-Zinn. Mindfulness meditation has been shown to have a positive impact on several psychiatric problems such as depression and therefore has formed the basis of mindfulness programs such as mindfulness-based cognitive therapy, mindfulness-based stress reduction and mindfulness-based pain management. The applications of mindfulness meditation are well established, however the mechanisms that underlie this practice are yet to be fully understood. Many tests and studies on soldiers with PTSD have shown tremendous positive results in decreasing stress levels and being able to cope with problems of the past, paving the way for more tests and studies to normalize and accept mindful based meditation and research, not only for soldiers with PTSD, but numerous mental inabilities or disabilities.

Personality neuroscience uses neuroscientific methods to study the neurobiological mechanisms underlying individual differences in stable psychological attributes. Specifically, personality neuroscience aims to investigate the relationships between inter-individual variation in brain structures as well as functions and behavioral measures of persistent psychological traits, broadly defined as "predispositions and average tendencies to be in particular states", including but are not limited to personality traits, sociobehavioral tendencies, and psychopathological risk factors. Personality neuroscience is considered as an interdisciplinary field integrating research questions and methodologies from social psychology, personality psychology, and neuroscience. It is closely related to other interdisciplinary fields, such as social, cognitive, and affective neuroscience.

References

  1. 1 2 3 4 Jost, John T.; Amodio, David M. (13 November 2011). "Political ideology as motivated social cognition: Behavioral and neuroscientific evidence" (PDF). Motivation and Emotion. 36 (1): 55–64. doi:10.1007/s11031-011-9260-7. S2CID   10675844.
  2. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Kanai, R; Feilden, T; Firth, C; Rees, G (2011). "Political Orientations Are Correlated with Brain Structure in Young Adults". Current Biology. 21 (8): 677–80. Bibcode:2011CBio...21..677K. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2011.03.017. PMC   3092984 . PMID   21474316.
  3. 1 2 3 Y. Inbar; et al. (2008). "Conservatives are more easily disgusted than liberals" (PDF). Cognition and Emotion. 23 (4): 714–725. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.372.3053 . doi:10.1080/02699930802110007. S2CID   7411404.
  4. 1 2 J. Jost; et al. (2006). "The end of the end of ideology" (PDF). American Psychologist. 61 (7): 651–670. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.61.7.651. PMID   17032067.
  5. 1 2 3 Napier J; Jost JT (2008). "Why Are Conservatives Happier Than Liberals?". Psychological Science. 19 (6): 562–72. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02124.x.
  6. 1 2 3 J. Vigil; et al. (2010). "Political leanings vary with facial expression processing and psychosocial functioning". Group Processes & Intergroup Relations. 13 (5): 547–558. doi:10.1177/1368430209356930. S2CID   59571553.
  7. 1 2 3 J. Jost; et al. (2003). "Political conservatism as motivated social cognition" (PDF). Psychological Bulletin. 129 (3): 339–375. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.129.3.339. PMID   12784934. S2CID   1778256.
  8. Dolan, Eric W. (2024-04-10). "Genetic variations help explain the link between cognitive ability and liberalism". PsyPost - Psychology News. Retrieved 2024-04-12.
  9. Kleppesto, Thomas Haarklau; Czajkowski, Nikolai Olavi; Sheehy-Skeffington, Jennifer; Vassend, Olav; Roysamb, Espen; Eftedal, Nikolai Haahjem; Kunst, Jonas R.; Ystrom, Eivind; Thomsen, Lotte (2024-02-22). "The genetic underpinnings of right-wing authoritarianism and social dominance orientation explain political attitudes beyond Big Five personality". Journal of Personality. doi:10.1111/jopy.12921. hdl: 10852/111202 . ISSN   0022-3506. PMID   38386613.
  10. Buchen, Lizzie (2012). "Biology and ideology: The anatomy of politics". Nature. 490 (7421): 466–468. Bibcode:2012Natur.490..466B. doi: 10.1038/490466a . PMID   23099382.
  11. Davies, James (1983). "The Proper Biological Study of Politics". Political Psychology. 4 (4): 731–743. doi:10.2307/3791065. JSTOR   3791065.
  12. 1 2 Ninh, Amie (April 8, 2011). "Liberal vs. Conservative: Does the Difference Lie in the Brain?". Time .
  13. Carlson, Neil R. (12 January 2012). Physiology of Behavior. Pearson. p. 364. ISBN   978-0205239399.
  14. Bzdok D, Langner R, Caspers S, Kurth F, Habel U, Zilles K, Laird A, Eickhoff SB (January 2011). "ALE meta-analysis on facial judgments of trustworthiness and attractiveness". Brain Structure & Function. 215 (3–4): 209–23. doi:10.1007/s00429-010-0287-4. PMC   4020344 . PMID   20978908.
  15. Kennedy DP, Gläscher J, Tyszka JM, Adolphs R (October 2009). "Personal space regulation by the human amygdala". Nature Neuroscience. 12 (10): 1226–7. doi:10.1038/nn.2381. PMC   2753689 . PMID   19718035.
  16. Bickart KC, Wright CI, Dautoff RJ, Dickerson BC, Barrett LF (February 2011). "Amygdala volume and social network size in humans". Nature Neuroscience. 14 (2): 163–4. doi:10.1038/nn.2724. PMC   3079404 . PMID   21186358.
  17. Szalavitz, Maia (28 December 2010). "How to Win Friends: Have a Big Amygdala?". Time. Archived from the original on 17 July 2011. Retrieved 30 December 2010.
  18. T.L. Brink. (2008) Psychology: A Student Friendly Approach. "Unit 4: The Nervous System". pp 61.
  19. Feinstein JS, Adolphs R, Damasio A, Tranel D (January 2011). "The human amygdala and the induction and experience of fear". Current Biology. 21 (1): 34–8. Bibcode:2011CBio...21...34F. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2010.11.042. PMC   3030206 . PMID   21167712.
  20. Staut CC, Naidich TP (April 1998). "Urbach-Wiethe disease (Lipoid proteinosis)". Pediatric Neurosurgery. 28 (4): 212–4. doi:10.1159/000028653. PMID   9732251. S2CID   46862405.
  21. Sanfey AG, Rilling JK, Aronson JA, Nystrom LE, Cohen JD (June 2003). "The neural basis of economic decision-making in the Ultimatum Game". Science. 300 (5626): 1755–8. Bibcode:2003Sci...300.1755S. doi:10.1126/science.1082976. PMID   12805551. S2CID   7111382.
  22. B. Wicker; et al. (2003). "Both of us disgusted in My insula: The common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust". Neuron. 40 (3): 655–664. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00679-2 . PMID   14642287.
  23. Wright P, He G, Shapira NA, Goodman WK, Liu Y (October 2004). "Disgust and the insula: fMRI responses to pictures of mutilation and contamination". NeuroReport. 15 (15): 2347–51. doi:10.1097/00001756-200410250-00009. PMID   15640753. S2CID   6864309.
  24. Lane RD, Reiman EM, Axelrod B, Yun LS, Holmes A, Schwartz GE (July 1998). "Neural correlates of levels of emotional awareness. Evidence of an interaction between emotion and attention in the anterior cingulate cortex". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 10 (4): 525–35. doi:10.1162/089892998562924. PMID   9712681. S2CID   27743177.
  25. Price DD (June 2000). "Psychological and neural mechanisms of the affective dimension of pain". Science. 288 (5472): 1769–72. Bibcode:2000Sci...288.1769P. doi:10.1126/science.288.5472.1769. PMID   10846154. S2CID   15250446.
  26. H. Critchley; et al. (2001). "Neural activity in the human brain relating to uncertainty and arousal during anticipation". Neuron. 29 (2): 537–545. doi:10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00225-2. hdl: 21.11116/0000-0001-A313-1 . PMID   11239442. S2CID   10995076.
  27. Bush G, Luu P, Posner MI (June 2000). "Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 4 (6): 215–222. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01483-2. PMID   10827444. S2CID   16451230.
  28. "Politics on the Brain: Scans Show Whether You Lean Left or Right". LiveScience. 7 April 2011. Retrieved September 25, 2012.
  29. Kattalia, Kathryn (April 8, 2011). "The liberal brain? Scans show liberals and conservatives have different brain structures". New York Daily News . Archived from the original on July 10, 2012. Retrieved September 25, 2012.
  30. David M Amodio, John T Jost, Sarah L Master & Cindy M Yee, Neurocognitive correlates of liberalism and conservatism, Nature Neuroscience . Cited by 69 other studies
  31. "Brains of Liberals, Conservatives May Work Differently". Psych Central. 2007-10-20. Archived from the original on 2016-10-13.
  32. "Study finds left-wing brain, right-wing brain". Los Angeles Times . 2007-09-10.
  33. Bernabel, R. T., & Oliveira, A. (2017). Conservatism and liberalism predict performance in two nonideological cognitive tasks. Politics and the Life Sciences, 36(2), 49-59.
  34. Weissflog, M., Choma, B. L., Dywan, J., van Noordt, S. J., & Segalowitz, S. J. (2013). The political (and physiological) divide: Political orientation, performance monitoring, and the anterior cingulate response. Social neuroscience, 8(5), 434-447.
  35. Inzlicht, Michael, Ian McGregor, Jacob B. Hirsh, and Kyle Nash. "Neural markers of religious conviction." Psychological science 20, no. 3 (2009): 385-392.
  36. Kremláček, Jan, Daniel Musil, Jana Langrová, and Martin Palecek. "Neural correlates of liberalism and conservatism in a post-Communist country." Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 13 (2019): 119.
  37. Wendell, D. G. (2016). Loyola eCommons Neural Correlates of Political Attitudes: Emotion and Ideology in the Brain. Available online at: http://ecommons.luc.edu/luc_diss/2299, Loyola University Chicago, pages 133-178 Accessed 27/07/2023
  38. Xu, Xiaowen, Annika K. Karinen, Hanah A. Chapman, Jordan B. Peterson, and Jason E. Plaks. "An orderly personality partially explains the link between trait disgust and political conservatism." Cognition and Emotion (2019).
  39. Elad-Strenger, Julia, Jutta Proch, and Thomas Kessler. "Is Disgust a "Conservative" Emotion?." Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin (2019): 0146167219880191.
  40. Pennycook, Gordon, and David G. Rand. "Cognitive reflection and the 2016 US Presidential election." Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 45, no. 2 (2019): 224-239.
  41. Oxley, D. R.; Smith, K. B.; Alford, J. R.; Hibbing, M. V.; Miller, J. L.; Scalora, M.; Hatemi, P. K.; Hibbing, J. R. (19 September 2008). "Political Attitudes Vary with Physiological Traits". Science. 321 (5896): 1667–1670. Bibcode:2008Sci...321.1667O. doi:10.1126/science.1157627. PMID   18801995. S2CID   7215269.
  42. Bakker, Bert N.; Schumacher, Gijs; Gothreau, Claire; Arceneaux, Kevin (June 2020). "Conservatives and liberals have similar physiological responses to threats". Nature Human Behaviour. 4 (6): 613–621. doi:10.1038/s41562-020-0823-z. PMC   7306406 . PMID   32042109.
  43. Fournier, Patrick; Soroka, Stuart; Nir, Lilach (August 2020). "Negativity Biases and Political Ideology: A Comparative Test across 17 Countries". American Political Science Review. 114 (3): 775–791. doi:10.1017/S0003055420000131. S2CID   225441710.
  44. Zamboni G, Gozzi M, Krueger F, Duhamel JR, Sirigu A, Grafman J (2009). "Individualism, conservatism, and radicalism as criteria for processing political beliefs: a parametric fMRI study". Social Neuroscience. 4 (5): 367–83. doi:10.1080/17470910902860308. PMID   19562629. S2CID   15652205. Zamboni G, Gozzi M, Krueger F, Duhamel JR, Sirigu A, Jordan Grafman. National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD, USA
  45. Kristine Knudson; et al. (March 2006). "Politics on the Brain: An fMRI Investigation". Soc Neurosci. 1 (1): 25–40. doi:10.1080/17470910600670603. PMC   1828689 . PMID   17372621.
  46. Aue T, Lavelle LA, Cacioppo JT (July 2009). "Great expectations: what can fMRI research tell us about psychological phenomena?" (PDF). International Journal of Psychophysiology. 73 (1): 10–6. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.12.017. PMID   19232374.
  47. Raj, A; van Oudenaarden, A (2008). "Nature, nurture, or chance: stochastic gene expression and its consequences". Cell. 135 (2): 216–26. doi:10.1016/j.cell.2008.09.050. PMC   3118044 . PMID   18957198.
  48. Martin, Nicholas; Boomsma, Dorret; Machin, Geoffrey (17 December 1997). "A twin-pronged attack on complex traits" (PDF). Nature Genetics . 17 (4): 387–92. doi:10.1038/ng1297-387. PMID   9398838. S2CID   2028886.
  49. Beckwith, Jon; Morris, Corey A. (December 2008). "Twin Studies of Political Behavior: Untenable Assumptions?". Perspectives on Politics. 6 (4): 785–91. doi:10.1017/S1537592708081917. S2CID   55630117.
  50. Fiske, Susan T.; Gilbert, Daniel T.; Lindzey, Gardner (15 February 2010). Handbook of Social Psychology (PDF) (5th ed.). John Wiley & Sons. p. 372.
  51. Carey, Benedict (June 21, 2005). "Some Politics May Be Etched in the Genes". The New York Times . Retrieved September 25, 2012.
  52. Alford, J. R.; Funk, C. L.; Hibbing, J. R. (2005). "Are Political Orientations Genetically Transmitted?". American Political Science Review. 99 (2): 153–167. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.622.476 . doi:10.1017/S0003055405051579. S2CID   3820911.
  53. Kincaid, Harold, and Jeroen Van Bouwel, eds. The Oxford Handbook of Philosophy of Political Science. Oxford University Press, 2023, pg 19-32
  54. Border, Richard, Emma C. Johnson, Luke M. Evans, Andrew Smolen, Noah Berley, Patrick F. Sullivan, and Matthew C. Keller. "No support for historical candidate gene or candidate gene-by-interaction hypotheses for major depression across multiple large samples." American Journal of Psychiatry 176, no. 5 (2019): 376-387.
  55. Hatemi, PK; Medland, SE; Klemmensen, R; Oskarrson, S; Littvay, L; Dawes, C; Verhulst, B; McDermott, R; Nørgaard, AS; Klofstad, C; Christensen, K; Johannesson, M; Magnusson, PKE; Eaves, LJ; Martin, NG (2014). "Genetic Influences on Political Ideologies: Twin Analyses of 19 Measures of Political Ideologies from Five Democracies and Genome-Wide Findings from Three Populations". Behav Genet. 44 (3): 282–294. doi:10.1007/s10519-014-9648-8. PMC   4038932 . PMID   24569950.
  56. Hatemi, P. K.; Gillespie, N. A.; Eaves, L. J.; Maher, B. S.; Webb, B. T.; Heath, A. C.; Medland, S. E.; Smyth, D. C.; Beeby, H. N.; Gordon, S. D.; Montgomery, G. W.; Zhu, G.; Byrne, E. M.; Martin, N. G. (2011). "A Genome-Wide Analysis of Liberal and Conservative Political Attitudes". The Journal of Politics. 73: 271–285. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.662.2987 . doi:10.1017/S0022381610001015.
  57. Michael Bang Petersen. The evolutionary psychology of Mass Politics. In Roberts, S. C. (2011). Roberts, S. Craig (ed.). Applied Evolutionary Psychology. Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199586073.001.0001. ISBN   9780199586073.
  58. "Strong men more likely to vote Conservative". The Telegraph. April 11, 2012. Retrieved September 25, 2012.
  59. Dean, T. (2012). "Evolution and Moral Diversity". The Baltic International Yearbook of Cognition, Logic and Communication. 7. doi: 10.4148/biyclc.v7i0.1775 .
  60. Alford, John R., Carolyn L. Funk, and John R. Hibbing. "Are political orientations genetically transmitted?." American political science review 99, no. 2 (2005): 153-167.

Further reading