Transitivity and valency |
---|
Transitivity |
Intransitive verb Transitive verb Ditransitive verb |
Valence increasing |
Causative Applicative Benefactive Dative shift |
Valence decreasing |
Passive Antipassive Impersonal passive |
Reflexives and reciprocals |
Reflexive pronoun Reflexive verb Reciprocal construction Reciprocal pronoun |
Linguisticsportal |
In linguistics, a causative (abbreviated CAUS) is a valency-increasing operation [1] that indicates that a subject either causes someone or something else to do or be something or causes a change in state of a non-volitional event. Normally, it brings in a new argument (the causer), A, into a transitive clause, with the original subject S becoming the object O.
All languages have ways to express causation but differ in the means. Most, if not all, languages have specific or lexical causative forms (such as English rise → raise, lie → lay, sit → set). Some languages also have morphological devices (such as inflection) that change verbs into their causative forms or change adjectives into verbs of becoming. Other languages employ periphrasis, with control verbs, idiomatic expressions or auxiliary verbs. There tends to be a link between how "compact" a causative device is and its semantic meaning. [2]
The normal English causative verb [3] or control verb used in periphrasis is make rather than cause. Linguistic terms are traditionally given names with a Romance root, which has led some to believe that cause is more prototypical. While cause is a causative, it carries some additional meaning (it implies direct causation) and is less common than make. Also, while most other English causative verbs require a to complement clause (as in "My mom caused me to eat broccoli"), make does not require one ("My mom made me eat broccoli"), at least when it is not being used in the passive voice. [5] : 36–7
Many authors have written extensively on causative constructions and have used a variety of terms, often to talk about the same things.
S, A, and O are terms used in morphosyntactic alignment to describe arguments in a sentence. The subject of an intransitive verb is S, the agent of a transitive verb is A, and the object of a transitive is O. These terms are technically not abbreviations (anymore) for "subject", "agent", and "object", though they can usually be thought of that way. P is often used instead of O in many works.
The term underlying is used to describe sentences, phrases, or words that correspond to their causative versions. Often, this underlying sentence may not be explicitly stated. For example, for the sentence "'John made Bill drive the truck'", the underlying sentence would be Bill drove the truck. This has also been called the base situation. [6]
A derived sentence would be the causativized variant of the underlying sentence.
The causer is the new argument in a causative expression that causes the action to be done. The causer is the new argument brought into a derived sentence. In the example sentence above, John is the causer.
The causee is the argument that actually does the action in a causativized sentence. It is usually present in both the underlying and derived sentences. Bill is the causee in the above example.
There are various ways of encoding causation, which form somewhat of a continuum of "compactness." [2] : 74–5
Lexical causatives are common in the world's languages. There are three kinds of lexical causatives, the unifying factor being that the idea of causation is part of the semantics of the verb itself. [1] : 177 (English, for example, employs all three of these kinds of lexical causatives.)
On the surface, lexical causatives look essentially the same as a regular transitive verb. There are a few reasons why this is not true. The first is that transitive verbs generally do not have an intransitive counterpart but lexical causatives do. The semantics of the verbs show the difference as well. A regular transitive verb implies a single event while a lexical causative implies a realization of an event: [8] : 511
Sentence (b) is judged ungrammatical because it goes against the successful event implied by the verb melt.
Some languages, including English, have ambitransitive verbs like break, burn or awake, which may either be intransitive or transitive ("The vase broke" vs. "I broke the vase.")
These are split into two varieties: agentive and patientive ambitransitives. Agentive ambitransitives (also called S=A ambitransitives) include verbs such as walk and knit because the S of the intransitive corresponds to the A of the transitive. For example:
This type of ambitransitive does not show a causative relationship.
For patientive ambitransitives (also called S=O ambitransitives), such as trip and spill, the S of the intransitive corresponds to the O of the transitive:
These are further divided into two more types, based on speakers' intuition. Some, like spill in (2), are primarily transitive and secondarily intransitive. Other verbs like this include smash and extend. Other verbs, such as trip in (3) go the other way: they are primarily intransitive and secondarily transitive.
Other examples of this type include explode, melt, dissolve, walk, and march. It is this type of ambitransitive verb that is considered a causative. [2] : 38 This is given some anecdotal evidence in that to translate (3b) above into languages with morphological causatives, a morpheme would need to be attached to the verb.
Lexical causatives are apparently constrained to involving only one agentive argument. Semantically, the causer is usually marked as the patient. In fact, it is unlikely whether any language has a lexical causative for verbs such as swim, sing, read, or kick. [7] : 3
English fell (as in "Paul felled the tree") can be thought of as a lexical causative of fall ("the tree fell"), exemplifying this category. [1] : 177 This is considered a lexical change because it is not at all productive. If it were productive, it would be an internal change morphological causative (below). [1]
English has verb pairs such as rise and raise, eat and feed, see and show where one is essentially the causative correspondent of the other. [1] : 177
These pairs are linked semantically by various means, usually involving translation. For example, burn as in "The grass burned" (intransitive) would translate as awa- in Yimas, while burn as in "I burned the grass" (transitive) would translate as ampu- in Yimas. [2] : 40
There are eight different morphological processes by which a causative may be marked, roughly organized by compactness: [2] : 34
Process | Basic Verb | Causative Form | Language |
---|---|---|---|
internal change | tìkti'be suitable' | táikyti'make suitable' | Lithuanian |
tone change | nɔ̂ (high falling) 'be awake' | nɔ̄ (low level) 'awaken, rouse' | Lahu |
consonant repetition | xarab'go bad' | xarrab'make go bad, ruin' | Gulf Arabic |
vowel lengthening | mar'die' | ma:r'kill' | Kashmiri |
reduplication | bengok'shout' | be-bengok'make shout' | Javanese |
prefix | gǝbba'enter' | a-gǝbba'insert' | Amharic |
suffix | -kam-'die' | -kam-isa-'kill' | Kʼicheʼ |
circumfix | -č'am-'eat' | -a-č'm-ev-'feed (make eat)' | Georgian |
Within morphological causatives, this degree of compactness bears an important variable when considering the semantics of the two processes. For example, mechanisms that do not change the length of the word (internal change, tone change) are shorter than those that lengthen it. Of those that lengthen it, shorter changes are more compact than longer.
Verbs can be classified into four categories, according to how susceptible they are to morphological causativization: [7] : 4–11
This hierarchy has some exceptions, but it does generally hold true. For example, given a text of Guarani, only about 16% of causatives apply to transitives. [7] : 5 For some languages, it may not apply to transitive verbs productively and may only apply to verbs that denote abstract action or consumption of food. Additionally, within Athabaskan family, all languages can causativize inactive intransitives, but not all of them can causativize active intransitives or even transitives. [7] : 5
A number of languages involve a form of analytic causative that involves two verbs in a single predicate, such as French, Spanish, Italian and Catalan. [2] : 35 For example, when French faire is used as a causative, the causee noun phrase cannot occur between it and the next verb. [10]
je
1SG.A
ferai
make+FUT+1SG
manger
eat+INF
les
the
gâteaux
cakes
à
PREP
Jean
Jean
"I will make Jean eat the cakes." [2] : 35
Unlike most other Romance languages, Portuguese uses a periphrastic construction like that of English, discussed below.
Kiowa uses a similar mechanism. Verbs can be compounded with the transitive verb ɔ́m to create a causative: [11]
bé-khó-ày-ɔ́m
2SG.A-now-start.off-CAUS+IMP
"Go ahead and run it [the tape recorder]!" (lit. "make it start off")
Some languages use a periphrastic (or analytic) construction to express causation and typically include two verbs and two clauses. English causatives prototypically use make (but other verbs such as cause, order, allow, force, compel can be used) in the main clause with the lexical verb in a subordinate clause, as in "I made him go." [2] : 35–7
Other languages, such as Persian, [12] have the opposite syntax: the causative is in a subordinating clause and the main verb is in the main clause, as in the following example from Macushi:
[imakui'pî
bad
kupî
do
Jesus-ya]
Jesus-ERG
emapu'tî
CAUS
yonpa-'pî
try-PAST
makui-ya
Satan-ERG
teuren
FRUSTRATION
"Satan unsuccessfully tried to make Jesus do bad." [14]
Canela-Krahô has a combination of the two in which the causee is marked twice, once in each clause:
Portuguese also has a periphrastic construction like that of English but unlike most other Romance languages:
Analytic causatives are sometimes not considered to be valency increasing devices, but they can semantically be interpreted as such [1] .: 181
A language may have one or more different formal mechanisms for expression of causation. For languages with only one, the semantic range is broad. For those with multiple, there is always a semantic difference between the two. [2] : 61 R. M. W. Dixon breaks down these semantic differences into 9 parameters, involving the verb itself, the causee, and the causer: [2] : 62–73
These parameters are not mutually exclusive. Many causative constructions involve the semantics of two or more parameters. However, the difference between the causatives in a language most likely will be distinguished by one of the parameters.
There is a strong correlation between the semantics of a causative and the mechanism by which it is expressed. Generally, if a causative is more "compact" than another, it usually implies a more direct causation.
For inanimate and unconscious objects, English analytic causatives (1–3) are therefore not completely synonymous with lexical causatives (4–6):
Analytic causatives (1–3) imply that no physical contact was involved and therefore was done by some sort of magical power or telekinesis. Lexical causatives (4–6) do not imply that meaning. [17] : 784
For animate and conscious objects, there is a different difference in meaning:
(1) makes sense only if they are animate and awake. Barring magic, (2) makes sense only if the object is inanimate or unconscious. [17] : 784
Generally, the larger the distance between the causer and the causee, the more finite the verb is. Consider the following examples from Spanish:
Montezuma
hizo
CAUS:3SG:PERF
comer
eat:INF
pan
bread
a
DAT
Cortés.
"Montezuma made Cortés eat bread."
Montezuma
Montezuma
hizo
CAUS:3SG:PERF
que
that
Cortés
Cortés
comiera
eat:3SG:SUB
pan.
bread
"Montezuma made Cortés eat bread."
The first example implies that Montezuma was physically there and was directly involved in making Cortés eat bread. The second example implies that Montezuma was not physically there and arranged for something to happen to make Cortés eat bread, perhaps by killing all of his cattle. That could approximate the English construction "Montezuma got Cortés to eat bread." Therefore, at least in Spanish, a conjugated verb implies a less direct causation. [1] : 185
Dixon examines this correlation cross-linguistically, and summarizes his findings in the following table. [2] : 76 In this table, L refers to lexical causatives, M1 refers to more compact morphological processes while M2 refers to less compact processes, CP refers to complex predicates (two verbs, one predicate), and P refers to periphrastic constructions. These processes are explained more clearly in the devices section above.
Parameter | Meaning | Mechanism | Language | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Causative type 1 | Causative type 2 | Causative type 1 | Causative type 2 | ||
1 | state | action | M1 | M2 | Amharic |
M | P | Bahasa Indonesia, Malay | |||
2 | intransitive | all transitive | M | P | Austronesian languages, Mayan languages, etc. |
intransitive and simple transitive | ditransitive | M | P | Basque, Abkhaz | |
3 | causee lacking control | causee having control | L | M | Japanese |
M1 | M2 | Creek | |||
4 | causee willing | causee unwilling | M1 | M2 | Swahili |
M | CP | Tangkhul Naga | |||
M | P | Swahili | |||
5 | causee partially affected | causee fully affected | M1 | M2 | Tariana |
6 | direct | indirect | M1 | M2 | Nivkh, Apalaí, Hindi, Jingpaw |
M | P | Buru, Chrau, Alamblak, Mixtec, Korean | |||
7 | intentional | accidental | M | CP | Kammu |
P | M plus P | Chrau | |||
8 | naturally | with effort | L | M | Fijian |
L | P | English | |||
M | P | Russian, Tariana |
Parameter 9, Involvement, cannot be included in the table because the only two languages with this distinction, Nomatsiguenga and Kamayurá, the morphemes are about the same length. [2] : 75 When a larger sample of languages show this distinction, perhaps this parameter can be included in the table.
The table shows that for each of eight semantic parameters outlined in the semantics section above, more compact causative processes show one distinction while less compact processes show the other distinction. For example, Parameter 6 distinguishes between more direct and less direct causation. In Hindi, M1, or the shorter morphological process, shows direct causation while M2, the longer morphological process, shows indirect causation.
Summarizing the table, Dixon has given two prototypes for causatives: [2] : 77
All eight of the components in each prototype are never attested in a single causative. However, a single process may have two or three components. Dixon admits to these being very tentative and in need for further investigation. [2] : 77–8
R.M.W. Dixon also outlines the syntactic possibilities of causatives in the world's languages.
Since intransitive verbs have low valency, virtually any type of causative construction can apply to them productively within a language. Some constructions are only allowed with intransitive verbs and some languages (such as Arabic, Blackfoot, and Gothic) only allow causatives of intransitive verbs, with some exceptions. [7] : 5 In all cases, the original subject of the underlying intransitive verb corresponds with the object of the derived transitive verb. All languages have this construction, though some allow a semantic difference if the original subject is marked differently (such as Japanese and Hungarian). [2] : 45
For split systems, causatives of intransitives may be treated differently. [2] : 45
The syntax of a causative construction is almost always the same as some other type of sentence, such as a sentence with a transitive verb. Tariana, however, is an exception to this rule. [2] : 45
In the causative of a transitive verb, the new causer always becomes the new A of the sentence. What happens to the causee and the original object depend on the language. Dixon shows that there are five main types of situations:
type | causer | original A (causee) | original O | languages |
---|---|---|---|---|
(i) | A | special marking | O | Nivkh, Telugu |
(ii) | A | retains A-marking | O | Kabardian, Trumai, Qiang |
(iii) | A | has O-marking | has O-marking | Hebrew, Tariana, Amharic, Sanskrit [1] : 180 |
(iv) | A | O | non-core | Javanese, Swahili, Kammu, Babungo |
(v) | A | non-core | O | many languages |
Within type (v) there are two main subtypes. Either the original A goes into the first empty slot in a hierarchy or it always takes a certain function. [2] : 54
For the first subtype, there is a hierarchy involved in the language:
French is a language that follows this hierarchy. When a causative is employed, the original A does not get marked the same for intransitives, transitives, and ditransitives. [2] : 54 In this first example, the verb in intransitive, and with the subject slot taken, the original A becomes a direct object:
je
1SG+NOM
ferai
make+FUT+1SG
courir
run+INF
Jean
Jean
"I will make Jean run."
The following example has a transitive verb. The subject and direct object slots are filled (with je and les gâteaux, respectively) so the original A becomes an indirect object:
je
1SG+NOM
ferai
make+FUT+1SG
manger
eat+INF
les
the
gâteaux
cakes
à
PREP
Jean
Jean
"I will make Jean eat the cakes."
This final French example has a ditransitive verb. The subject is je, the direct object is une lettre, and the indirect object is directeur, so the original A is marked as an oblique:
je
1SG+NOM
ferai
make+FUT+1SG
écrire
write+INF
une
a
lettre
letter
au
PREP+ART
directeur
headmaster
par
PREP
Jean
Jean
"I will make Jean write a letter to the headmaster"
While some writers have called this hierarchical causative construction the norm, [18] : 8 outside of Romance languages it is in fact rather rare. [2] : 54
Most other languages are of the second subtype of type (v), and the original A takes on a set case or marking, regardless whether the underlying verb is intransitive or transitive:
causer | original A (causee) | original O | languages |
---|---|---|---|
A | dative | O | Sanuma, Apalai, Kamaiurá, Turkish, Japanese |
A | instrumental | O | Hungarian, Kannada, Marathi |
A | locative | O | Some languages of Daghestan |
A | allative | O | West Greenlandic Eskimo |
A | adessive | O | The morphological causative in Finnish. |
A | possessive | O | Tsez |
The syntactic and morphological constraints of individual language generally restrict causatives of ditransitive verbs. The underlying phrase already contains an A, O, and indirect object, and so in order to accommodate a fourth argument, languages employ a variety of constructions. They tend to be idiosyncratic and are difficult to group together into types. Additionally, data is patchy for many languages since descriptions of languages seldom include information of causatives of ditransitives. [2] : 56–9
Some types of causative constructions essentially do not permit double causatives, e.g. it would be difficult to find a lexical double causative. Periphrastic causatives however, have the potential to always be applied iteratively (Mom made Dad make my brother make his friends leave the house.).
Many Indo-Aryan languages (such as Hindustani) have lexical double causatives.
For morphological causatives, some languages do not allow single morpheme to be applied twice on a single verb (Jarawara) while others do (Capanawa, Hungarian, Turkish, Kabardian, Karbi), though sometimes with an idiomatic meaning (Swahili's means force to do and Oromo's carries an intensive meaning). Other languages, such as Nivkh, have two different morphological mechanisms that can apply to a single verb. Still others have one morpheme that applies to intransitives and another to transitives (Apalai, Guarani). All of these examples apply to underlying intransitive verbs, yielding a ditransitive verb. So far, there are no reliable data for a morphological double causative of a transitive verb, resulting in a verb with four arguments. [2] : 59–61
Yokuts, an indigenous language spoken in California, has a morpheme, -lsaˑ, that indicates causation in addition to repetition. This is separate from the language's normal mechanisms of causation.
'utuˑ
play music
-lsaˑ
CAUS
-unhoˑ
AGT
"one who makes (people) play music repeatedly"
This implies a single act by the causer, but multiple acts by the causee. [19]
The causative voice is a grammatical voice promoting the oblique argument of a transitive verb to an actor argument. When the causative voice is applied to a verb, its valency increases by one. If, after the application of the grammatical voice, there are two actor arguments, one of them is obligatorily demoted to an oblique argument.
Japanese, Turkish and Mongolian are examples of languages with the causative voice. The following are examples from Japanese:
The causal or causative case (abbreviated CAUS) is a grammatical case that indicates that the marked noun is the cause or reason for something. It is found in the Dravidian languages Kannada [20] and Telugu, the Native South American language Quechua, and Northeast Caucasian Archi. It is also found in extinct Tocharian B, an Indo-European language.
The causal-final is a grammatical case in Hungarian (and Chuvash) expressing the meaning 'for the purpose of, for the reason that', [21] : 93 and denoting price asked of or paid for goods. [21] : 116 It is formed by adding the ending suffix -ért to the end of the noun, e.g. kenyér "bread" >kenyérért "for bread", e.g. elküldtem a boltba kenyérért "I sent him to the store for bread". [21] : 115 It is not affected by vowel harmony in Hungarian. [21] : 111
Shibatani [7] lists three criteria for entities and relations that must be encoded in linguistic expressions of causation:
This set of definitional prerequisites allows for a broad set of types of relationships based, at least, on the lexical verb, the semantics of the causer, the semantics of the causee and the semantics of the construction explicitly encoding the causal relationship. Many analysts (Comrie (1981), Song (1996), Dixon (2000) and others) have worked to tease apart what factors (semantic or otherwise) account for the distribution of causative constructions, as well as to document what patterns actually occur cross-linguistically.
Bernard Comrie [22] focuses on the typology of the syntax and semantics of causative constructions proper. Crucially, Comrie (and others to be discussed here) distinguish between the linguistic encoding of causal relations and other extra-linguistic concerns such as the nature of causation itself and questions of how humans perceive of causal relations. While certainly not irrelevant, these extra-linguistic questions will, for now, be left aside. Comrie usefully characterizes causative events in terms of two (or more) microevents perceived of composing a macroevent, and encoded in a single expression (of varying size and form). Formally, he categorizes causatives into 3 types, depending on the contiguity of the material encoding the causing event and that encoding the caused event. These are: 1) lexical causatives, in which the two events are expressed in a single lexical item, as in the well-discussed case of English kill; 2) morphological causatives, in which the causing event and the caused event are encoded in a single verbal complex via causative morphology, and, prototypically, morphological marking showing the status of affected arguments. Finally, Comrie discusses analytic causatives, in which the causing event and the caused event are encoded in separate clauses.
Comrie's work is also noteworthy for having brought the notion of syntactic hierarchy to bear on the typology of causative constructions. A hierarchy of grammatical relations had already been formulated to help explain possibilities for relative clause formation (first presented as Keenan and Comrie's (1972) NP accessibility hierarchy; see Croft 1990: 147), and Comrie argued that a similar hierarchy was in play, at least in some constructions, in the marking of the original A argument when a base transitive clause is causativized. The hierarchy is as follows:
Comrie's argument was, in short, that some causativized-transitive constructions mark the new A as belonging to the leftmost available slot in the above hierarchy. Dixon (2000) fleshes out a version this analysis in more detail.
Presenting a typology of causatives and causation based on a database of 600 languages, Song [23] is very critical of typological work that depends on statistical inference, citing data from the Niger-Congo family that contradicts some earlier claims that "languages within genera are generally fairly similar typologically". [23] Song therefore culls data from every language for which adequate documentation is available to him, and categorizes the various causative constructions gleaned therefrom into three classes: COMPACT, AND and PURP.
Song employs the following terminology:
The major differences between Song's analysis and Comrie (1981) and Dixon (2000), is that Song lumps the range of lexical and morphological causatives together under the label COMPACT, [23] : 20 in which [Vcause] can be "less than a free morpheme" (e.g., bound morpheme [prefix, suffix, infix, circumfix, reduplication], zero-derivation, suppletion); or "a free morpheme", [23] : 28 in which [Vcause] and [Veffect] form a single grammatical unit. Most of the examples given look like serial verb constructions, and no in-depth analysis is undertaken for some of the constructions in which [Vcause] and [Veffect] are less formally contiguous. Song notes this non-contiguity, but does not undertake to explain why it might be important.
The AND causative, for Song, is any construction with a separate [Scause] and [Seffect] i.e., in which "two clauses [are] involved". [23] : 35 This, in theory, could include larger, multi-clausal expressions of causal relations which many analysts probably would not label a 'causative construction', e.g.: 'It rained yesterday, so they stayed home', but the boundaries of the AND causative category are not discussed.
One of Song's major contributions to the literature[ according to whom? ] is fleshing out an analysis of his PURP causative. These are constructions which encode intended causation on the part of the causer, but which do not encode any outcome: i.e., the speaker encodes [Vcause] and causer intentionality, but remains agnostic as to whether [Veffect] was felicitously effected.
Leonard Talmy [24] conducts an in-depth investigation of different types of causal relations. Talmy refers to these as "lexicalization patterns," a term which remains unclear to me[ who? ], given that few of the examples given in his discussion are lexical items, and most interpretations of "different types of causation incorporated in the verb root" are in fact wholly dependent on other morphosyntactic material in the clause. Let us[ who? ] first examine his list of possible (semantic) causative types, [24] : 69–70 with examples:
One question remaining to be explored is how this set of divisions usefully differs from other analysts' typologies of the semantics of encoding causal relations. Some overlap in the types of semantic information in play is immediately apparent, however: in cases of instrument causation ('the hammer broke the cup'), we would certainly expect the 'causer' to be acting directly [Dixon's criterion 6] and to be involved in the activity [criterion 9]; likewise, we would expect instances of caused agency to include more information on causee control on willingness [criteria 3 & 4].
In Proto-Germanic, the parent language of Germanic languages such as English, causative verbs are formed by adding a suffix -j/ij- to the past-tense ablaut of a strong verb, with Verner's Law voicing applied. (All of those characteristics derive from the way that causative verbs are formed in Proto-Indo-European, with an accented -éy- suffix added to the o-grade of a non-derived verb.) Here are some examples:
In English, to sit/to seat", and in German, sitzen/setzen form pairs of resultative/causative.
English uses various causative mechanisms, with varying degrees of productivity. There are a large number of lexical causatives, such as kill, open and feed. [7] : 2
Additionally, there are several morphemes that can express causation. For example, -(i)fy can be thought of as a causative in that it is a derivation that turns an adjective or noun into a "verb of becoming":
en- can also be a causative. In English, adjectives (or stative verbs in other languages) can express the acquisition of a quality or changes of state with causatives, in the same way as with regular verbs. For example, if there is a stative verb to be large, the causative will mean to enlarge, to make grow. The reflexive form of the causative can then be used to mean to enlarge oneself, or even as a middle voice, to grow.
As far as lexical causatives are concerned, English has at least 49 causative verbs. Roughly half affect only sentient beings: allow, block, cause, enable, force, get, help, hinder, hold, impede, keep, leave, let, make, permit, prevent, protect, restrain, save, set, start, stimulate, stop. The others can affect either sentient or non-sentient beings: aid, bar, bribe, compel, constrain, convince, deter, discourage, dissuade, drive, have, hamper, impel, incite, induce, influence, inspire, lead, move, persuade, prompt, push, restrict, rouse, send, spur. [25]
In Sanskrit, there is a causative form of the verb (ṇijanta), which is used when the subject of a clause forces or makes the object perform an action. The causative suffix -ay is attached to the verbal root, which may cause vowel sandhi to take place:
In Persian, the causative form of the verb is formed by adding ân(i)dan to the present stem:
In Lithuanian, the causative form of the verb is made by adding suffix -(d)in- to the present stem:
The topic of causatives has not been studied much for Latin, mainly because of its lack of productive morphological causative. [6] : 2
Hindustani uses the infix -(l)ā- and -(l)vā- to make verbs causative.
The causative verbs are called prayōjaka kriẏā (প্রযোজক ক্রিয়া) in Bengali. In the simplest way, the causative form of a verb can be formed by adding the suffix "-nō"নো with the verbal noun form of the given verb.
From the verbal root (dhātuধাতু in Bengali) perspective, the formation of causatives is done by adding the suffix "-ā"-আ with the verb roots ending with a consonant, and the suffix "-ōẏā"ওয়া with those roots ending with a vowel. Thus, the verbal root transformations of the two previously mentioned verbs are:
These verb roots are thereafter inflected with tense, aspect and mood.
The Basque language has two ways to form causative verbs: by using a non-ergative transitive verb in the absolute form, or by the morphological causativization. The first method is only possible with a restricted set of verbs which excludes those whose subjects take the ergative case, such as the verb eztul egin (cough -- literally "make (a) cough"). [26]
Haurrak
child.ERG
katua
cat.ABS
hil
die
du
AUX:3SG.3SG
'The child killed the cat'
Haurrak
child.ERG
katua
cat.ABS
hilarazi
die.CAU
du
AUX:3SG.3SG
'The child caused the cat to die'
In addition to very productive morphological causatives, Turkish also has some lexical causatives: kır- "break", yırt- "split", dik- "plant", yak- "burn", sakla- "hide", aç- "open". [7] : 2
In most Semitic languages, there is a causative form of the verb. It is postulated that in Proto-Semitic, the causative verbal stem was formed by the š- prefix, which has become ʾa-, hi- or ī- in different languages.
Arabic also has a causative form (Form II) created by gemination of the central consonant of the triliteral root, as follows:
The ʾa- form (Form IV), while it is used in Modern Standard Arabic, is no longer productive in many of the colloquial varieties of Arabic, which uniformly prefer Form II.
Japanese has lexical forms and a morphological device to signify causation. Lexical forms come in pairs of intransitive and transitive verbs, where the causee is mostly inanimate.
However, both intransitive and transitive verbs can form the causative in a mostly regular pattern, now with the causee being mostly animate:
In the context of an intransitive verb, the syntax of Japanese causatives allows a two-way distinction in the causee's willingness to perform the action. If the new object is marked in the accusative case (o), it suggests that the causee did the action willingly, suggesting the agent allowed or requested the action rather than forcing or demanding it. However, if the object is marked in the dative case (ni), it expresses the idea that the causee was forced to perform the action. With a transitive verb, this contrast is not directly visible as a clause cannot contain two noun phrases marked as accusative. [2] : 45, 65–66 [27]
Khmer has six prefixes and one infix to derive the causative form of verbs, but they vary in frequency and productiveness. The consonantal prefix p- is one of them:
Causative forms are also found in the Uralic languages of Europe, such as Finnish:
The causative suffix is often used irregularly and/or because of historical reasons, as the following Finnish examples:
Hungarian marks the original subject of an intransitive differently in causative forms to convey direct causation. If the causee is marked by the accusative case, a more direct causation is implied than if the instrumental case is used. [2] : 45–6
In Māori, an Austronesian language, the whaka- prefix can be added to a verb:
In Philippine languages such as Tagalog and Ilokano, the pa- prefix is added to verbal forms and to adjectives to form causatives:
In Malay/Indonesian, causatives are formed from the prefix per- (it becomes memper- after actor focus/active prefix meng-, expected *memer- as in *memerhatikan found informally). While most languages uses their causative affix for derivational purposes, it has integrated to Malay verb inflection system.
In Guaraní, there are three causatives: one for transitive verbs and two for intransitive verbs. [28] In some texts, the first one is called "coactive." [29]
The -uka suffix (or one of its allomorphes: -yka, -ka) is added to transitive verbs: [28]
The mbo- prefix is added to intransitive oral verbs and is replaced by mo- for nasal verbs: [28] [29]
The guero- (rero- or just ro-) prefix can also be added to intransitive verbs. It has a comitative meaning and translates roughly as "to cause something or someone to participate in an action with the subject:" [31]
The same root (guata) can take both causatives but with different meanings.
Classical Nahuatl, in the Uto-Aztecan language family, has a well-developed morphological system of expressing causation by means of the suffix -tia:
Causativity is often used in honorific speech in Classical Nahuatl, and rather than simply "doing," the honored person "causes himself to do." [32]
Rice makes the following points about morphological causatives in Athabaskan languages: [33] : 212
The semantic factor of causee control, or the degree of control that that causee wields over the effecting of the caused microevent (also discussed as parameter #3 on Dixon's (2000:62) list) and which Rice (2001) finds to be a major factor in other Athabaskan causatives helps account for much of the distribution of the Hupa syntactic causative (below).
Golla, in his (1970) descriptive grammar of Hupa (summarized in Sapir and Golla (2001)), describes three classes of morphologically derived causatives:
While Golla does not generalize about the semantics of verb themes that are compatible with causative ƚ-, several preliminary generalizations can be made. Firstly, in the three cases described by Golla, O [the undergoer] is neither controlling nor agentive; O is largely patientive in all cases. Secondly, the causer appears to be acting directly on O. Thirdly, none of the examples given (including the examples above) involve the causativization of a base-transitive theme.
Mithun (2000) lists nine causatives for Central Alaskan Yup'ik and describes each in detail. [35] : 98–102 Here is a brief description of each:
Morpheme | Approximate meaning |
---|---|
-vkar-/-cete- | 'let, allow, permit, cause, compel' |
-te- | 'let, allow, cause, compel' |
-nar- | 'cause' |
-rqe- | 'intentionally or deliberately cause' |
-cetaar- | 'try to cause' |
-narqe- | 'tend to cause' |
-naite- | 'tend not to cause' |
-cir- | 'let, wait for, make' |
-(r/l)i - | 'become or cause to become' |
Kinyarwanda uses periphrastic causatives and morphological causatives.
The periphrastic causatives use the verbs -teer- and -tum-, which mean cause. With -teer-, the original S becomes the O of the main clause, leaving the original verb in the infinitive, just like in English: [36] : 160–1
Ábáana
children
b-a-gii-ye.
they-PST-go-ASP
"The children left."
Umugabo
man
y-a-tee-ye
he-PST-cause-ASP
ábáana
children
ku-geend-a.
INF-go-ASP
"The man caused the children to go.
With -túm-, the original S remains in the embedded clause and the original verb is still marked for person and tense: [36] : 161–2
N-a-andits-e
I-PST-write-ASP
amábárúwa
letters
meênshi.
many
"I wrote many letters.
Umukoôbwa
girl
y-a-tum-ye
she-PST-cause-ASP
n-á-andik-a
I-PST-write-ASP
amábárúwa
letters
meênshi.
many
"The girl caused me to write many letters."
Derivational causatives use the -iish- morpheme, which can be applied to intransitives (3) or transitives (4): [36] : 164
Ábáana
children
ba-rá-ryáam-ye.
they-PRES-sleep-ASP
"The children are sleeping."
Umugóre
woman
a-ryaam-iish-ije
she-sleep-CAUS-ASP
ábáana
children
"The woman is putting the children to sleep."
Ábáana
children
ba-ra-som-a
they-PRES-read-ASP
ibitabo.
books
"The children are reading the books."
Umugabo
man
a-ra-som-eesh-a
he-PRES-read-CAUS-ASP
ábáana
children
ibitabo.
books
"The man is making the children read the books."
The suffix -iish- implies an indirect causation (similar to English have in "I had him write a paper"), but other causatives imply a direct causation (similar to English make in "I made him write a paper"). [36] : 166
One of the more direct causation devices is the deletion of what is called a "neutral" morpheme -ik-, which indicates state or potentiality. Stems with the -ik- removed can take -iish, but the causation is then less direct: [36] : 166
-mének- | "be broken" | -mén- | "break" | -méneesh- | "have (something) broken" |
-sáduk- | "be cut" | -sátur- | "cut" | -sátuz- | "have (something) cut" |
Another direct causation maker is -y- which is used for some verbs: [36] : 167
Ámáazi
water
a-rá-shyúuh-a.
it-PRES-be warm-ASP
"The water is being warmed."
Umugóre
woman
a-rá-shyúush-y-a
she-PRES-warm-CAUS-ASP
ámáazi.
water
"The woman is warming the water."
Umugabo
man
a-rá-shyúuh-iish-a
he-PRES-warm-CAUS-ASP
umugóre
woman
ámáazi.
water.
"The man is having the woman warm the water.
In Esperanto, the suffix -ig- can be added to any kind of word:
Lexical semantics, as a subfield of linguistic semantics, is the study of word meanings. It includes the study of how words structure their meaning, how they act in grammar and compositionality, and the relationships between the distinct senses and uses of a word.
In general linguistics, a labile verb is a verb that undergoes causative alternation; that is, it can be used both transitively and intransitively, with the requirement that the direct object of its transitive use corresponds to the subject of its intransitive use, as in "I ring the bell" and "The bell rings." Labile verbs are a prominent feature of English, and also occur in many other languages. When causatively alternating verbs are used transitively they are called causatives since, in the transitive use of the verb, the subject is causing the action denoted by the intransitive version. When causatively alternating verbs are used intransitively, they are referred to as anticausatives or inchoatives because the intransitive variant describes a situation in which the theme participant undergoes a change of state, becoming, for example, "rung".
Kinyarwanda, Rwandan or Rwanda, officially known as Ikinyarwanda, is a Bantu language and the national language of Rwanda. It is a dialect of the Rwanda-Rundi language that is also spoken in adjacent parts of the Democratic Republic of the Congo and in Uganda, where the dialect is known as Rufumbira or Urufumbira. Kinyarwanda is universal among the native population of Rwanda and is mutually intelligible with Kirundi, the national language of neighbouring Burundi. Kinyabwishya and Kinyamulenge are mutually intelligible dialects spoken in the North Kivu and South Kivu provinces of neighbouring DR Congo.
In linguistic typology, nominative–accusative alignment is a type of morphosyntactic alignment in which subjects of intransitive verbs are treated like subjects of transitive verbs, and are distinguished from objects of transitive verbs in basic clause constructions. Nominative–accusative alignment can be coded by case-marking, verb agreement and/or word order. It has a wide global distribution and is the most common alignment system among the world's languages. Languages with nominative–accusative alignment are commonly called nominative–accusative languages.
Greenlandic is an Eskimo–Aleut language with about 57,000 speakers, mostly Greenlandic Inuit in Greenland. It is closely related to the Inuit languages in Canada such as Inuktitut. It is the most widely spoken Eskimo–Aleut language. In June 2009, the government of Greenland, the Naalakkersuisut, made Greenlandic the sole official language of the autonomous territory, to strengthen it in the face of competition from the colonial language, Danish. The main variety is Kalaallisut, or West Greenlandic. The second variety is Tunumiit oraasiat, or East Greenlandic. The language of the Inughuit of Greenland, Inuktun or Polar Eskimo, is a recent arrival and a dialect of Inuktitut.
In linguistic typology, tripartite alignment is a type of morphosyntactic alignment in which the main argument ('subject') of an intransitive verb, the agent argument ('subject') of a transitive verb, and the patient argument of a transitive verb are each treated distinctly in the grammatical system of a language. This is in contrast with nominative-accusative and ergative-absolutive alignment languages, in which the argument of an intransitive verb patterns with either the agent argument of the transitive or with the patient argument of the transitive. Thus, whereas in English, "she" in "she runs" patterns with "she" in "she finds it", and an ergative language would pattern "she" in "she runs" with "her" in "he likes her", a tripartite language would treat the "she" in "she runs" as morphologically and/or syntactically distinct from either argument in "he likes her".
In linguistics, valency or valence is the number and type of arguments and complements controlled by a predicate, content verbs being typical predicates. Valency is related, though not identical, to subcategorization and transitivity, which count only object arguments – valency counts all arguments, including the subject. The linguistic meaning of valency derives from the definition of valency in chemistry. Like valency found in chemistry, there is the binding of specific elements. In the grammatical theory of valency, the verbs organize sentences by binding the specific elements. Examples of elements that would be bound would be the complement and the actant. Although the term originates from valence in chemistry, linguistic valency has a close analogy in mathematics under the term arity.
An anticausative verb is an intransitive verb that shows an event affecting its subject, while giving no semantic or syntactic indication of the cause of the event. The single argument of the anticausative verb is a patient, that is, what undergoes an action. One can assume that there is a cause or an agent of causation, but the syntactic structure of the anticausative makes it unnatural or impossible to refer to it directly. Examples of anticausative verbs are break, sink, move, etc.
The Inuit languages, like other Eskimo–Aleut languages, exhibit a regular agglutinative and heavily suffixing morphology. The languages are rich in suffixes, making words very long and potentially unique. For example, in Nunavut Inuktitut:
Dameli (دَميلي), also Damia, Damɛ̃ḍī, Dāmia bāṣa or Gidoj, is an Indo-Aryan language of the Dardic subgroup spoken by approximately 5,000 people in the Domel Town, in the Chitral District of Khyber-Pakhtunkhwa province of Pakistan.
The applicative voice is a grammatical voice that promotes an oblique argument of a verb to the core object argument. It is generally considered a valency-increasing morpheme. The Applicative is often found in agglutinative languages, such as the Bantu languages and Austronesian languages. Other examples include Nuxalk, Ubykh, and Ainu.
Babine–Witsuwitʼen or Nadotʼen-Wetʼsuwetʼen is an Athabaskan language spoken in the Central Interior of British Columbia. Its closest relative is Carrier. Because of this linguistic relationship together with political and cultural ties, Babine–Witsuwitʼen is often referred to as Northern Carrier or Western Carrier. Specialist opinion is, however, that it should be considered a separate, though related, language.
Apurinã, or Ipurina, is a Southern Maipurean language spoken by the Apurinã people of the Amazon basin. It has an active–stative syntax. Apurinã is a Portuguese word used to describe the Popikariwakori people and their language. Apurinã indigenous communities are predominantly found along the Purus River, in the Northwestern Amazon region in Brazil, in the Amazonas state. Its population is currently spread over twenty-seven different indigenous lands along the Purus River. with an estimated total population of 9,500 people. It is predicted, however, that fewer than 30% of the Apurinã population can speak the language fluently. A definite number of speakers cannot be firmly determined because of the regional scattered presence of its people. The spread of Apurinã speakers to different regions was initially caused by conflict or disease, which has consequently led natives to lose the ability to speak the language for lack of practice and also because of interactions with other communities.
Adang is a Papuan language spoken on the island of Alor in Indonesia. The language is agglutinative. The Hamap dialect is sometimes treated as a separate language; on the other hand, Kabola, which is sociolinguistically distinct, is sometimes included. Adang, Hamap, and Kabola are considered a dialect chain. Adang is endangered as fewer speakers raise their children in Adang, instead opting for Indonesian.
The Nias language is an Austronesian language spoken on Nias Island and the Batu Islands off the west coast of Sumatra in Indonesia. It is known as Li Niha by its native speakers. It belongs to the Northwest Sumatra–Barrier Islands subgroup which also includes Mentawai and the Batak languages. It had about 770,000 speakers in 2000. There are three main dialects: northern, central and southern. It is an open-syllable language, which means there are no syllable-final consonants.
Ledo Kaili is the largest member of the Kaili languages, which are a dialect chain within the Kaili–Pamona language family. These languages are spoken in Central Sulawesi (Indonesia). Kaili with all of its dialects is one of the largest languages in Sulawesi. One third of the population of Sulawesi Tengah province were (1979) native speakers of a Kaili language. The object language of this article is the main dialect Ledo, which is spoken in the Donggala and Sigi districts (Kabupaten) in and around the provincial capital Palu.
Ramarama, also known as Karo, is a Tupian language of Brazil.
Tuparí is an indigenous language of Brazil. It is one of six Tupari languages of the Tupian language family. The Tuparí language, and its people, is located predominantly within the state of Rondônia, though speakers are also present in the state of Acre on the Terra Indıgena Rio Branco. There are roughly 350 speakers of this language, with the total number of members of this ethnic group being around 600.
Mekéns (Mekem), or Amniapé, is a nearly extinct Tupian language of the state of Rondônia, in the Amazon region of Brazil.
Tsʼixa is a critically endangered African language that belongs to the Kalahari Khoe branch of the Khoe-Kwadi language family. The Tsʼixa speech community consists of approximately 200 speakers who live in Botswana on the eastern edge of the Okavango Delta, in the small village of Mababe. They are a foraging society that consists of the ethnically diverse groups commonly subsumed under the names "San", "Bushmen" or "Basarwa". The most common term of self-reference within the community is Xuukhoe or 'people left behind', a rather broad ethnonym roughly equaling San, which is also used by Khwe-speakers in Botswana. Although the affiliation of Tsʼixa within the Khalari Khoe branch, as well as the genetic classification of the Khoisan languages in general, is still unclear, the Khoisan language scholar Tom Güldemann posits in a 2014 article the following genealogical relationships within Khoe-Kwadi, and argues for the status of Tsʼixa as a language in its own right. The language tree to the right presents a possible classification of Tsʼixa within Khoe-Kwadi:
...verbs like let, make, and have together with their complements are often called causitive verbs or constructions since one agent is (to one degree or another) 'causing' another to act.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: DOI inactive as of November 2024 (link)