Trumai language

Last updated
Trumaí
ho kod ke
Native to Brazil
Regionupper Xingu River region of the Xingu Indigenous Park
Ethnicity120 Trumai people (2006) [1]
Native speakers
51 of varying fluency (2006) [1]
Language codes
ISO 639-3 tpy
Glottolog trum1247
ELP Trumai
This article contains IPA phonetic symbols. Without proper rendering support, you may see question marks, boxes, or other symbols instead of Unicode characters. For an introductory guide on IPA symbols, see Help:IPA.

Trumai is an endangered language isolate of Brazil. Most Trumai are fluent in languages of wider communication, and children are not learning it well. [2]

Contents

Background

Trumai is a language spoken by the indigenous community of the same name located in the Xingu reserve along the Upper Xingu River in central Brazil. Murphy and Quain [3] reported that there were only 25 people remaining in the Trumai community. Fortunately, this has since increased to 94 as of 1997, of which 51 people spoke the Trumai language. [4] In the International Encyclopedia of Linguistics, Grimes observes that there are 78 speakers as of 2003. [5] Due to the popularity of speaking Portuguese among the local population, [5] Trumai is considered an extremely endangered language [6] because the children are not learning to speak it as a first language. [2]

The Trumai people first entered the Upper Xingu region sometime in the early 19th century after being driven away from southeastern Brazil by the Xavante people. [7] The first contact the Trumai had with a white person was in 1884 when Karl von den Steinen explored the Upper Xingu region. [3] [6] He observed the differences between Trumai culture and other Xingu cultures due to the Trumai's relocation. [6] In the fifty years or so that followed Von den Steinem's first visit to the Trumai, there is little documentation of the community because researchers who visited the Xingu region preferred visiting and studying other indigenous cultures instead. [3]

In the time between the Trumai's first arrival in the upper Xingu and Von den Steinen's first contact with them, they were continuously being attacked by the native communities in the region, including the Suyá and Ikpeng. [7] Following a period of contacts from researchers, including Buell Quain in 1938, [3] the Trumai moved to a new territory again, this time because of a flu and measles epidemic. [7] After recovering from this, the subsequent population increase led to the emergence of more Trumai villages in the Upper Xingu region, while their former territories have since become occupied by other communities. [7]

Despite being surrounded by a variety of different languages that belong to the four major stocks of Brazilian indigenous languages (Tupi, Arawak, Cariban, and Ge), Trumai is an isolated language. [4] There is speculation that Trumai belongs to the Equatorial language stock, in which case it is still very far removed from other languages and families belonging here. [4] [5]

Initial research done on the Trumai was included in an overall study of the cultures of the Xingu region, which was performed through surveys focussing on "material culture". [3] Quain was the first researcher to focus on the Trumai culture specifically, however, this was an anthropological study, not linguistic. [3] In the preface to her thesis "A Reference Grammar of Trumaí", [4] Guirardello states that Monod-Becquelin was the first person to conduct descriptive studies on the Trumai language, in which she focused on aspects of the language such as ergativity [8] and phonological transcriptions. [9] Monod-Becquelin's early work was followed by Greenberg's research, which includes his aforementioned proposal that Trumai, instead of being a completely isolated language, belongs to the Equatorial stock. [4] [5]

Guirardello's "A Reference Grammar of Trumai" is the first proper description of Trumai grammar, [4] which Guirardello composed with the intention of it becoming an aid for future research papers on the language. [4] Since then, research on the Trumai language has increased, leading to studies of various aspects of the language. Monod-Becquelin has continued her research by investigating the use of transitive verbs in Trumai. [10] Guirardello's work has also included studies into Trumai's ergativity, focussing on the ergative-absolutive patterns in its morphology and the complexity of its syntax due to the nominative-accusative patterns also present. [11]

Trumai was one of the 24 indigenous languages studied in South America as part of a series of documentation projects conducted by DoBeS (Documenting Endangered Languages). [12] DoBeS is one of the many language documentation organizations operating in Brazil, and is funded by the Volkswagen Foundation. [12]

The Trumai culture has also been the topic of anthropological studies. The first was done by Buell Quain, who spent four months with the Trumai and gathered information on many aspects of the culture and community. [3] More recently, De Vienne has conducted ethnographic studies on Trumai focussing on language and communication in the community, such as joking [13] and ritual singing traditions. [14]

Distribution

Trumai is spoken in four villages, three of which are in Parque Indígena do Xingu. There are a few dozen fluent speakers among an ethnic population of about 100 people. [15] :1077

Language contact

Jolkesky (2016) notes that there are lexical similarities with the Macro-Mataguayo-Guaykuru, Tupi, and Mochika language families due to contact. [16]

Similarities with the Macro-Mataguayo-Guaykuru languages and Tupi-Guarani languages indicate that Trumai had originated in the Paraguay River basin. The Trumai had only arrived in the Upper Xingu basin via the Culuene River during the 19th century (Villas Bôas & Villas Bôas 1970:27 [17] ). [16] :426

Grammar

Phonology

Bilabial Dental Alveolar Palatal Velar Glottal
Plosive p t , d k ʔ
Ejective t̪ʼ
Affricate t͡s , t͡sʼ
Fricative f s , ɬ ʃ x h
Nasal m n
Approximant w ɾ , l j

This inventory is atypical of Amazonian languages in its ejective consonants, the lateral fricative /ɬ/, and the alveolar–dental distinction. Guirardello, who specializes on Trumai, has presented varied inventories of these phonemes: Guirardello (1999a) [4] lists /t̪ t̪' ts ts' s/ as dental, and /t t' d n l ɬ ɾ/ as alveolar; whereas Guirardello (1999b) lists only /t/ and /t'/ as alveolar. [18] Younger speakers do not make the ejective distinction.

Front Central Back
High i ɨ u
Mid e o
Low a

The vocalic inventory is / i , e , a , o , u / and ɨ . Syllable structure is maximally CVC, and stress always falls on the final syllable of a word.

Morphology

The morphological aspects of Trumai as covered in Guirardello's grammar of the language include the parts of speech: nouns, verbs, and auxiliaries. Under nouns, she investigates the effect of adjectives, plurality in the language, and count versus mass nouns, among others. Under verbs, she focuses on causality, negation, intensity, and imperativity in verb particles. And under auxiliaries, she discusses body posture, mood and aspect, and directional auxiliaries. Chapter 5 explores further analyses of each of these aspects of the parts of speech in terms of "Simple Declarative Clauses". [4]

Pronouns in Trumai are distinguished by person, gender, number, and listener inclusion/exclusion in the first-person plural pronoun. Like in English, gender is only seen in the third-person singular pronoun, while number is categorized as singular, dual, and plural (whereas English only has singular and plural). They are also affected by the type of Noun Phrase (henceforth "NP") they appear in (absolutive, ergative, or dative), which are distinguished by suffix insertion. The following are tables of the Trumai personal pronouns with examples included for the absolutive case: [4]

Absolutive
SingularDualPlural
1exclhaha aha wan
inclka aka wan
2hihi ahi wan
3mascineinak ainak wan
feminatl

ha

1

hu’tsa

see

chï(_in)

FOC/Tens

[huksitukuk

capivara

yi]-ki

YI-DAT

[yayanke

deer

tam]

COM

ha hu’tsa chï(_in) [huksitukuk yi]-ki [yayanke tam]

1 see FOC/Tens capivara YI-DAT deer COM

“I saw a capivara and a deer.” [4] Unknown glossing abbreviation(s) (help);

Ergative
SingularDualPlural
1exclhai-ts OR hai-kha ana-kha wan-ek
inclka ana-kka wan-ek
2hi-k OR ha-khi ana-khi wan-ek
3mascine-kinak ana-kinak wan-ek
feminatl-ek

[Karu],

Karu

[Kumaru],

Kumaru

[Atawaka]

Atawaka

hai-ts

1-ERG

amidoxos

call

ke.

KE

[Karu], [Kumaru], [Atawaka] hai-ts amidoxos ke.

Karu Kumaru Atawaka 1-ERG call KE

‘I called Karu, Kumaru, and Atawaka.’ [4] Unknown glossing abbreviation(s) (help);

Dative
SingularDualPlural
1exclhai-tlha ana-kiha wan-ki
inclka ana-kika wan-ki
2hi-tlhi ana-kihi wan-ki
3mascine-tlinak ana-kiinak wan-ki
feminatl-etl

kiki-k

man-ERG

atlat-ø

pan-ABS

kï̡tï

give

ha wan-ki.

1-DAT

kiki-k atlat-ø kï̡tï {ha wan-ki}.

man-ERG pan-ABS give 1-DAT

‘The man gave it to us.’ [4]

1st-person/exclusive pronouns are formed in ha, inclusive with ka, 2nd person with hi, and 3rd with in. Dual number is indicated by the suffix -a, and plural by -wan. Masculine and feminine are distinguished in the 3rd person. Alienable possession is indicated by the suffix -kte or -kate on the possessor (Kumaru-kte tahu 'Kumaru's spoon'), and inalienable possession by juxtaposition (dinoxo kuʃ 'the girl's head', ha kuʃ 'my head'). Suffixes are used to mark ergative (-ts for 1sg, otherwise -ek/-ak), dative, locative, allative, comitative, and instrumental case.

One interesting phenomena in Trumai morphology is the use of particles in the language. Guirardello first discusses them as a form of identifying tense in lieu of the tense-aspect-mood affixes used in English. These word formations are ka in, used for present or the recent past, and chï in, for past tense, but they are only used when the tense is not indicated via context. In a sentence, these particles appear as so:

ka_in:

[[Yaka]

Yaka

chumuchu]

lie.down

ka_in

FOC/Tens

tehnene-n.

floor-Loc

[[Yaka] chumuchu] ka_in tehnene-n.

Yaka lie.down FOC/Tens floor-Loc

‘Yaka lay.down on the floor.’

chï_in:

[ha

1

ayen]-atl

grandfather-DAT

chï_in

FOC/Tens

[hai]-ts

1-ERG

[oke

medicine

yi]

YI

kï̡tï.

give

[ha ayen]-atl chï_in [hai]-ts [oke yi] kï̡tï.

1 grandfather-DAT FOC/Tens 1-ERG medicine YI give

‘I gave medicine to my grandfather (emphasis).’ Unknown glossing abbreviation(s) (help);

As a whole, particles in Trumai are defined as a verb modifier that is not an adverb or auxiliary instead of being placed in a class of its own. There are four classes of particles in Trumai: Intensity, Negation, Causative, and Intensity. These modifiers fall under the 'particle' label because of their characteristics which differentiate them from auxiliaries. For example, while auxiliaries can only modify verbs, Intensity and Negation particles can also modify adverbs and quantifiers. [4]

The use of auxiliaries in Trumai is also very fascinating.[ editorializing ] In the language, they are defined as verb modifiers "associated with the domain of aspect, mood, and spatial orientation (this last domain is subdivided into body posture of the entity performing the event and directionality of the event being performed)". Here are some examples of each of these auxiliaries used in sentences:

Aspect/Mood:

iyi

IYI

sone-kma-n

drink-Perf-3Abs

de.

already

iyi sone-kma-n de.

IYI drink-Perf-3Abs already

‘He already finished drinking.’ OR ‘He already drank all.’ Unknown glossing abbreviation(s) (help);

Body Posture:

iyi

IYI

ma

eat

chumuchu-n.

lie-3Abs

iyi ma chumuchu-n.

IYI eat lie-3Abs

‘He is eating lying (on the floor).’ Unknown glossing abbreviation(s) (help);

Directionals:

ha

1

ka’chï

walk

lako.

Dir(down hill)

ha ka’chï lako.

1 walk {Dir(down hill)}

‘I go down walking.'

Guirardello makes a point of noting the significant differences between auxiliaries and verbs in Trumai: there is no independent argument structure, no lexical content, some experience phonological reduction, and they form a closed class. Their syntactic position is restricted to following a verb in the VP, which distinguishes them from adverbs, which hold a more flexible position in sentences. Apart from appearing inside the VP following the Verb, a property of some auxiliaries is that "they can bear the 3Abs enclitic –n/-e". [4]

Syntax

Ergative-Absolutive Language Structure

Trumai is a language with ergative-absolutive case assignment and therefore has three argument types: absolutive, ergative, and dative. The ergative-absolutive language system is described as that "in which a subject in an intransitive construction is realized in the same way as a patient or object in a transitive construction, and is thus distinguished from the subject or agent in the transitive". [19] It is "manifested through case-marking, verb-marking, and word order". [4]

Dative case is used for verbs such as 'eat', 'see', and 'talk with'. There are two verbs for 'kill', one, -fa, which takes a dative, and one, disi, which takes the ergative.[ clarification needed ][ Do the case roles for S reverse? ] Constituent order is basically ergative-absolutive-verb-dative (SV, SVB, AOV, AOVB). Ergative and dative arguments, which are marked by postpositions, may occur on the other side of the verb, but for an absolutive to do this, it needs to be marked with ke.[ clarification needed ][ postposition? ]

In noun phrases (including pronouns), the grammatical case is marked by an enclitic. For example, an ergative NP would end in either –ek or –k. Case marking using enclitics also occurs for Dative, Locative, and Genitive cases. In the case of personal pronouns, the first person singular pronoun also allows the enclitic –ts. The first person singular pronoun is also changed from ha in Absolutive case to hai for Ergative and Dative cases.

An interesting phenomenon in Trumai syntax is the NP morpheme iyi/yi because of its lack of a clear function. It always appears phrase-final, as iyi when only the noun itself is included in the phrase, but as yi when pluralizers or other lexical items are also included. However, despite this rule, there are still occasions where iyi appears in an NP with a lexical item. When it is present in Ergative and Dative sentences, the enclitics for each case are still added to the end of the NP:

Reduced form:

[inatl

3Pr(FEM)

yi]

run

pech

FOC/Tens

ka_in.

 

[inatl yi] pech ka_in.

3Pr(FEM) run FOC/Tens

‘She is running.’ Mismatch in the number of words between lines: 4 word(s) in line 1, 3 word(s) in line 2 (help);

No lexical item:

[iyi]

run-3.ABS

pech-e

FOC/Tens

ka_in.

 

[iyi] pech-e ka_in.

run-3.ABS FOC/Tens

‘She/he is running.’ Mismatch in the number of words between lines: 3 word(s) in line 1, 2 word(s) in line 2 (help);

Ergative/Dative:

[ni’dak

that.one

wan

PL

yi]-k

-ERG

chï_in

FOC/Tens

ha

1

disi.

beat

[ni’dak wan yi]-k chï_in ha disi.

that.one PL -ERG FOC/Tens 1 beat

‘They (these ones) beat me.’

For verb phrases in Trumai, case marking provides four verb categories: Intransitive, Transitive, Extended Intransitive, and Extended Transitive, where Intransitive verbs can have Absolutive case, Transtive verbs have Absolutive and Ergative case, and the Extended categories also have Dative case on top of this. Guirardello labels arguments as S, A, O, and DAT for case-marking instead of terms such as agent and patient.

Guirardello observed three types of clauses in Trumai sentences. Intransitive clauses attach the case marking /-ø/ to the S noun phrase or attach the third person enclitic –n/-e to the end of the verb phrase. Transitive clauses have A marked by –(V)k and O marked by –ø. Similarly to Intransitive clauses, the third person enclitic is added to the VP when A is absent. In Ditransitive clauses, the same marking occurs for A and O that we see in Transitive clauses. The DAT argument is marked by either –(V)tl, -ki, or –(V)s "depending on the characteristics of the head of the NP". No marking is attached to the VP when DAT is absent from the sentence. These clauses are proof of the Ergative-Absolutive argument of Trumai, as we see S and O undergo the same marking, while A is treated differently. In sentences, they would appear as follows:

Intransitive:

S

pet’ew-ø

frog-ABS

V

achïkida

jump

S V

pet’ew-ø achïkida

frog-ABS jump

‘The frog jumps.’ (256)

Transitive:

A

ine-k

3-ERG

V

Ø

break-3.ABS

 

mapa-n

 

A V

ine-k Ø mapa-n

3-ERG break-3.ABS

‘He broke it (a valuable pan).' Mismatch in the number of words between lines: 2 word(s) in line 1, 3 word(s) in line 2, 2 word(s) in line 3 (help);

Ditransitive:

A

kiki-k

1-ERG

O

atlat-ø

rice-ABS

V

kï̡tï

give

DAT

kasoro-s

dog-DAT

A O V DAT

kiki-k atlat-ø kï̡tï kasoro-s

1-ERG rice-ABS give dog-DAT

‘I gave rice to the dogs.'

The Absolutive argument is obligatory and therefore occurs in all clause types. Its case-marking is –ø or it has none at all and its syntactic placement is within the VP, before the verb (Guirardello is not clear as to whether it appears immediately before the verb or if other lexical items can be placed between them).

The Ergative argument is marked by –(V)k and only occurs in Transitive clauses, where it is the main argument of the verb and is therefore always required. Its syntactic placement is preceding the VP. While there are certain scenarios where it can be absent from a sentence that mainly occurs when there is discourse continuity. If it occurs without this, then the sentence has passive or middle voice.

The Dative argument is marked by –(V)tl,-ki, or –(V)s. Like the Ergative argument, it occurs before and outside of the VP. It is only obligatory in Extended clauses, where it is the main argument of the verb.

Trumai's causative construction uses the particle ka, which appears after the verb and can modify it. For Intransitive and Extended Transitive verbs, the causee is marked as Absolutive, while the causer is marked as Ergative. While this makes it similar to a simple Transitive clause, the difference is that the Absolutive enclitic is marked on the particle instead of the verb. For Transitive verbs, both the causee and causer are marked Ergative:

a.

hai-ts

1-ERG

Yakairu-ø

Yakairu-ABS

sa

dance

ka.

CAUS

hai-ts Yakairu-ø sa ka.

1-ERG Yakairu-ABS dance CAUS

‘I made Yakairu dance.’ [Intransitive verb]

b.

hai-ts

1-ERG

sa

dance

ka-n.

CAUS-3.ABS

hai-ts sa ka-n.

1-ERG dance CAUS-3.ABS

‘I made her dance.'

Semantics

Quantifiers

In Trumai, quantifiers appear before the noun they modify. For numerals, while the language includes the names for 1–10, most often only numbers 1–5 are spoken:

'1' mihin '2' huch '3' huch tahme

'4' pinepine-ktelen '5' inek'adkel-an

    Friend   friend-Gen   group               3Masc  hand finger-Loc

Other quantifier words (ex. 'few', 'many', etc.) change depending on whether they appear before count or mass nouns. They also vary from speaker to speaker, as in the case of "few" versus "a little", some speakers can distinguish the two based on how they appear before count vs. non-count words, while others cannot except for "high quantities". When they are distinguished, the quantifiers used by some speakers are:

Before count nounsBefore mass nouns
Many/a lota' dïpïx
Few/a littlea' dï tak (many-Neg)pïx tak (a lot-Neg)

While most speakers, who cannot distinguish between count and mass nouns for small quantities, say:

Before count nounsBefore mass nouns
Many/a lota' dïpïx
Few/a littlepïx tak

Aside from the above forms, there is an alternate form to describe small quantities: pa̡t 'few', which is meant to describe a small portion of an object. Here are some examples for each type of the above quantifiers:

Many/a lot: a'di ka_in k'ate yi.

(count) many Foc/Tens fish YI

'There are many fish.' (or: 'The fish are many/big in quantity.')

Many/a lot: pïx ka_in misu yi.

(mass) a.lot Foc/Tens water YI

'There is a lot of water.' (or: 'The water is big in quantity.')

Few/a little: a'di tak ka_in k'ate yi.

(some speakers) many Neg Foc/Tens fish YI

'There are a few fish.' (or: 'The fish are not many.')

Few/a little: pïx tak ka_in k'ate yi.

(most speakers) a.lot Neg Foc/Tens fish YI

'There are a few fish.' (or: 'The fish are not many.')

Few: atlat pa̡t.

         clay.pan  small

'small clay.pan.'

Just as is seen above where few/a little are formed as many/a lot plus a negation, there is no word for none, so negation is added in its place as well:

katnon take tak [kiki wan yi].

work Desid Neg man PL YI

'No man wants to work.' (lit: 'The men do not want to work.')

Quantifiers (numeral and otherwise) can also appear as a clause predicate or a focussed unit, in which they appear at the beginning of a sentence and are followed by a Focus/Tense particle.

Person plays a crucial role in what NP a quantifier is assigned to. Guirardello explains that in a sentence where there is both first- and third-person nouns/pronouns, it is clear that the quantifier is attached to the third-person. This is not as clear when there are two third-person NPs in a sentence. In order to understand which NP the quantifier is modifying in this scenario, one must look at what kinds of NPs are in the clause with the quantifier and if it is also being modified by a pluralizer.

Vocabulary

Loukotka (1968) lists the following basic vocabulary items for Trumai. [20]

glossTrumai
'one'mihin
'two'hursh
'three'hurstame
'head'yau-kut
'ear'yau-haptü
'tooth'yau-i
'hand'yau-kenap
'woman'ipae
'water'misu
'fire'
'stone'liki
'maize'hotet
'tapir'monotó

Related Research Articles

A grammatical case is a category of nouns and noun modifiers that corresponds to one or more potential grammatical functions for a nominal group in a wording. In various languages, nominal groups consisting of a noun and its modifiers belong to one of a few such categories. For instance, in English, one says I see them and they see me: the nominative pronouns I/they represent the perceiver and the accusative pronouns me/them represent the phenomenon perceived. Here, nominative and accusative are cases, that is, categories of pronouns corresponding to the functions they have in representation.

Mam is a Mayan language spoken by about half a million Mam people in the Guatemalan departments of Quetzaltenango, Huehuetenango, San Marcos, and Retalhuleu, and the Mexican states of Campeche and Chiapas. Thousands more make up a Mam diaspora throughout the United States and Mexico, with notable populations living in Oakland, California and Washington, D.C. The most extensive Mam grammar is Nora C. England's A grammar of Mam, a Mayan language (1983), which is based on the San Ildefonso Ixtahuacán dialect of Huehuetenango Department.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Hurrian language</span> Extinct ancient language of Mesopotamia

Hurrian is an extinct Hurro-Urartian language spoken by the Hurrians (Khurrites), a people who entered northern Mesopotamia around 2300 BC and had mostly vanished by 1000 BC. Hurrian was the language of the Mitanni kingdom in northern Mesopotamia and was likely spoken at least initially in Hurrian settlements in modern-day Syria.

The antipassive voice is a type of grammatical voice that either does not include the object or includes the object in an oblique case. This construction is similar to the passive voice, in that it decreases the verb's valency by one – the passive by deleting the agent and "promoting" the object to become the subject of the passive construction, the antipassive by deleting the object and "promoting" the agent to become the subject of the antipassive construction.

In linguistic typology, ergative–absolutive alignment is a type of morphosyntactic alignment in which the single argument ("subject") of an intransitive verb behaves like the object of a transitive verb, and differently from the agent ("subject") of a transitive verb. Examples include Basque, Georgian, Mayan, Tibetan, and certain Indo-European languages. It has also been attributed to the Semitic modern Aramaic languages. Ergative languages are classified into two groups: those that are morphologically ergative but syntactically behave as accusative and those that, on top of being ergative morphologically, also show ergativity in syntax. No language has been recorded in which both the morphological and syntactical ergative are present. Languages that belong to the former group are more numerous than those to the latter. Dyirbal is said to be the only representative of syntactic ergativity, yet it displays accusative alignment with certain pronouns.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Greenlandic language</span> Inuit language spoken in Greenland

Greenlandic is an Eskimo–Aleut language with about 57,000 speakers, mostly Greenlandic Inuit in Greenland. It is closely related to the Inuit languages in Canada such as Inuktitut. It is the most widely spoken Eskimo–Aleut language. In June 2009, the government of Greenland, the Naalakkersuisut, made Greenlandic the sole official language of the autonomous territory, to strengthen it in the face of competition from the colonial language, Danish. The main variety is Kalaallisut, or West Greenlandic. The second variety is Tunumiit oraasiat, or East Greenlandic. The language of the Inughuit of Greenland, Inuktun or Polar Eskimo, is a recent arrival and a dialect of Inuktitut.

Warrongo is an Australian Aboriginal language, one of the dozen languages of the Maric branch of the Pama–Nyungan family. It was formerly spoken by the Warrongo people in the area around Townsville, Queensland, Australia. Its last native speaker was Alf Palmer, who died in 1981.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Alyutor language</span> Chukotkan language of Kamchatka, Russia

Alyutor or Alutor is a language of Russia that belongs to the Chukotkan branch of the Chukotko-Kamchatkan languages, by the Alyutors. It is moribund, as only 25 speakers were reported in the 2010 Russian census.

Georgian grammar has many distinctive and extremely complex features, such as split ergativity and a polypersonal verb agreement system.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Hindustani grammar</span> Grammatical features of the Hindustani lingua franca

Hindustani, the lingua franca of Northern India and Pakistan, has two standardised registers: Hindi and Urdu. Grammatical differences between the two standards are minor but each uses its own script: Hindi uses Devanagari while Urdu uses an extended form of the Perso-Arabic script, typically in the Nastaʿlīq style.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Tsez language</span> Northeast Caucasian language

Tsez, also known as Dido, is a Northeast Caucasian language with about 15,000 speakers spoken by the Tsez, a Muslim people in the mountainous Tsunta District of southwestern Dagestan in Russia. The name is said to derive from the Tsez word for 'eagle', but this is most likely a folk etymology. The name Dido is derived from the Georgian word დიდი, meaning 'big'.

Canela is a dialect of the Canela-Krahô language, a Timbira variety of the Northern Jê language group spoken by the Apànjêkra (Apaniêkrá) and by the Mẽmõrtũmre in Maranhão, Brazil.

Cavineña is an indigenous language spoken on the Amazonian plains of northern Bolivia by over 1,000 Cavineño people. Although Cavineña is still spoken, it is an endangered language. Guillaume (2004) states that about 1200 people speak the language, out of a population of around 1700. Nearly all Cavineña are bilingual in Spanish.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Nias language</span> Austronesian language spoken in Indonesia

The Nias language is an Austronesian language spoken on Nias Island and the Batu Islands off the west coast of Sumatra in Indonesia. It is known as Li Niha by its native speakers. It belongs to the Northwest Sumatra–Barrier Islands subgroup which also includes Mentawai and the Batak languages. It had about 770,000 speakers in 2000. There are three main dialects: northern, central and southern. It is an open-syllable language, which means there are no syllable-final consonants.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Yukulta language</span> Extinct Australian Aboriginal language

The Yukulta language, also spelt Yugulda, Yokula, Yukala, Jugula, and Jakula, and also known as Ganggalidda, is a Tangkic language spoken in Queensland and Northern Territory, Australia. It was spoken by the Yukulta people, whose traditional lands lie on the southern coast of the Gulf of Carpentaria.

Mẽbêngôkre, sometimes referred to as Kayapó, is a Northern Jê language spoken by the Kayapó and the Xikrin people in the north of Mato Grosso and Pará in Brazil. There are around 8,600 native speakers since 2010 based on the 2015 Ethnologue 18th edition. Due to the number of speakers and the influence of Portuguese speakers, the language stands at a sixth level of endangerment; in which the materials for literacy and education in Mẽbêngôkre are very limited.

Mori Bawah, also known as Lower Mori or East Mori, is an Austronesian language of the Celebic branch. It is one of the principal languages of the Morowali Regency in Central Sulawesi.

Djaru (Tjaru) is a Pama–Nyungan language spoken in the south-eastern Kimberley region of Western Australia. As with most Pama-Nyungan languages, Djaru includes single, dual and plural pronoun numbers. Djaru also includes sign-language elements in its lexicon. Nouns in Djaru do not include gender classes, and apart from inflections, words are formed through roots, compounding or reduplication. Word order in Djaru is relatively free and has the ability to split up noun phrases. The Djaru language has a relatively small number of verbs, as compared to most languages, and thus utilizes a system of 'preverbs' and complex verbs to compensate. Djaru also has an avoidance language. Avoidance languages, sometimes known as 'mother-in-law languages', are special registers within a language that are spoken between certain family members – such registers are common throughout native Australian languages.

The grammar of the constructed Naʼvi language was created for the movie Avatar by Paul Frommer. It is a tripartite, primarily affixing agglutinative language.

In linguistic typology, nominative–absolutive alignment is a type of morphosyntactic alignment in which the sole argument of an intransitive verb shares some coding properties with the agent argument of a transitive verb and other coding properties with the patient argument of a transitive verb. It is typically observed in a subset of the clause types of a given language.

References

  1. 1 2 Trumaí at Ethnologue (18th ed., 2015) (subscription required)
  2. 1 2 Guirardello, Raquel. "Languages > Trumai". Provos Indigenas no Brasil. Instituto Socioambiental. Retrieved September 30, 2016.
  3. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Murphy, Robert F. (1955). The Trumai Indians of Central Brazil. J. J. Augustin.
  4. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 Guirardello, Raquel (1999). A reference grammar of Trumai (Doctor of Philosophy thesis). Rice University. hdl: 1911/19387 .
  5. 1 2 3 4 Frawley, William J. (2003). International Encyclopedia of Linguistics. Oxford University Press.
  6. 1 2 3 Aikhenvald, Alexandra Y. (2012). The Languages of the Amazon. Oxford University Press.
  7. 1 2 3 4 Guirardello, Raquel. "History of their occupation in the Xingu". Provos Indigenas no Brasil. Instituto Socioambiental. Retrieved September 30, 2016.
  8. Monod-Becquelin, A. (1976). "Classes verbales et construction ergative en trumai". Amerindia. 1: 117–141.
  9. Monod-Becquelin, A. (1977). "Les amant punis: conte Trumai (Haut-Xingu, Bresil)". Amerindia. 2: 163–173.
  10. Monod-Becquelin, A. and Becquey, C. (2013). "Transivite de l'action et ses expressions en trumai (langue isolee du Haut Xingu, Bresil)". Ateliers d'Anthropologie. 2: 1245–1436.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  11. Guirardello-Damian, Raquel (2010). "Ergativity in Trumai". Ergativity in Amazonia. Typological Studies in Language. 89: 203–234. doi:10.1075/tsl.89.09raq. ISBN   978-90-272-0670-1.
  12. 1 2 Franchetto, Bruna; Rice, Keren (September 2014). "Language Documentation in the Americas". Language Documentation and Conservation. 8: 251–261. hdl: 10125/24606 .
  13. De Vienne, E. (2012). "Make yourself uncomfortable". HAU: Journal of Ethnographic Theory. 2 (2): 163–187. doi:10.14318/hau2.2.010. S2CID   224807576.
  14. De Vienne, E. (2011). "Pourquoi chanter les ragots du passe?". Journal de la Société des Américanistes. 97: 291–319.
  15. Epps, Patience; Michael, Lev, eds. (2023). Amazonian Languages: Language Isolates. Volume II: Kanoé to Yurakaré. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter. ISBN   978-3-11-043273-2.
  16. 1 2 Jolkesky, Marcelo Pinho de Valhery (2016). Estudo arqueo-ecolinguístico das terras tropicais sul-americanas (Ph.D. dissertation) (2 ed.). Brasília: University of Brasília.
  17. Villas Bôas, O.; Villas Bôas, C. (1970). Xingu: Os Índios, Seus Mitos. São Paulo: Círculo do livro S.A.
  18. Guirardello, Raquel (1999). "Trumai". In Dixon, R.M.W.; Aikhenvald, Alexandra (eds.). The Amazonian Languages. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. p. 352. ISBN   9780521570213.
  19. Matthews, P. H. (2014). Oxford Concise Dictionary of Linguistics, 3rd Ed. Oxford University Press. p. 3. ISBN   9780199675128.
  20. Loukotka, Čestmír (1968). Classification of South American Indian languages . Los Angeles: UCLA Latin American Center.