Sino-Tibetan languages

Last updated
Sino-Tibetan
Trans-Himalayan
Geographic
distribution
Central Asia, East Asia, South Asia and Southeast Asia
Native speakers
(undated figure of 1.4 billion)
Linguistic classification One of the world's primary language families
Proto-language Proto-Sino-Tibetan
SubdivisionsSome 40 well-established subgroups, of which those with the most speakers are:
Language codes
ISO 639-2 / 5 sit
Linguasphere 79- (phylozone)
Glottolog sino1245
Sino-Tibetan Languages Map.png
Groupings of Sino-Tibetan languages

Sino-Tibetan (sometimes referred to as Trans-Himalayan) [1] [2] is a family of more than 400 languages, second only to Indo-European in number of native speakers. [3] Around 1.4 billion people speak a Sino-Tibetan language. [4] The vast majority of these are the 1.3 billion native speakers of Sinitic languages. Other Sino-Tibetan languages with large numbers of speakers include Burmese (33 million) and the Tibetic languages (6 million). Four United Nations member states (China, Singapore, Myanmar, and Bhutan) have a Sino-Tibetan language as their main native language. Other languages of the family are spoken in the Himalayas, the Southeast Asian Massif, and the eastern edge of the Tibetan Plateau. Most of these have small speech communities in remote mountain areas, and as such are poorly documented.

Contents

Several low-level subgroups have been securely reconstructed, but reconstruction of a proto-language for the family as a whole is still at an early stage, so the higher-level structure of Sino-Tibetan remains unclear. Although the family is traditionally presented as divided into Sinitic (i.e. Chinese languages) and Tibeto-Burman branches, a common origin of the non-Sinitic languages has never been demonstrated. The Kra–Dai and Hmong–Mien languages are generally included within Sino-Tibetan by Chinese linguists but have been excluded by the international community since the 1940s. Several links to other language families have been proposed, but none have broad acceptance.

History

A genetic relationship between Chinese, Tibetan, Burmese, and other languages was first proposed in the early 19th century and is now broadly accepted. The initial focus on languages of civilizations with long literary traditions has been broadened to include less widely spoken languages, some of which have only recently, or never, been written. However, the reconstruction of the family is much less developed than for families such as Indo-European or Austroasiatic. Difficulties have included the great diversity of the languages, the lack of inflection in many of them, and the effects of language contact. In addition, many of the smaller languages are spoken in mountainous areas that are difficult to reach and are often also sensitive border zones. [5] There is no consensus regarding the date and location of their origin. [6]

Early work

During the 18th century, several scholars noticed parallels between Tibetan and Burmese, both languages with extensive literary traditions. Early in the following century, Brian Houghton Hodgson and others noted that many non-literary languages of the highlands of northeast India and Southeast Asia were also related to these. The name "Tibeto-Burman" was first applied to this group in 1856 by James Richardson Logan, who added Karen in 1858. [7] [8] The third volume of the Linguistic Survey of India , edited by Sten Konow, was devoted to the Tibeto-Burman languages of British India. [9]

Studies of the "Indo-Chinese" languages of Southeast Asia from the mid-19th century by Logan and others revealed that they comprised four families: Tibeto-Burman, Tai, Mon–Khmer and Malayo-Polynesian. Julius Klaproth had noted in 1823 that Burmese, Tibetan, and Chinese all shared common basic vocabulary but that Thai, Mon, and Vietnamese were quite different. [10] [11] Ernst Kuhn envisaged a group with two branches, Chinese-Siamese and Tibeto-Burman. [lower-alpha 1] August Conrady called this group Indo-Chinese in his influential 1896 classification, though he had doubts about Karen. Conrady's terminology was widely used, but there was uncertainty regarding his exclusion of Vietnamese. Franz Nikolaus Finck in 1909 placed Karen as a third branch of Chinese-Siamese. [12] [13]

Jean Przyluski introduced the French term sino-tibétain as the title of his chapter on the group in Meillet and Cohen's Les langues du monde in 1924. [14] [15] He divided them into three groups: Tibeto-Burman, Chinese and Tai, [14] and was uncertain about the affinity of Karen and Hmong–Mien. [16] The English translation "Sino-Tibetan" first appeared in a short note by Przyluski and Luce in 1931. [17]

Shafer and Benedict

In 1935, the anthropologist Alfred Kroeber started the Sino-Tibetan Philology Project, funded by the Works Project Administration and based at the University of California, Berkeley. [18] The project was supervised by Robert Shafer until late 1938, and then by Paul K. Benedict. Under their direction, the staff of 30 non-linguists collated all the available documentation of Sino-Tibetan languages. The result was eight copies of a 15-volume typescript entitled Sino-Tibetan Linguistics. [9] [lower-alpha 2] This work was never published, but furnished the data for a series of papers by Shafer, as well as Shafer's five-volume Introduction to Sino-Tibetan and Benedict's Sino-Tibetan, a Conspectus. [20] [21]

Benedict completed the manuscript of his work in 1941, but it was not published until 1972. [22] Instead of building the entire family tree, he set out to reconstruct a Proto-Tibeto-Burman language by comparing five major languages, with occasional comparisons with other languages. [23] He reconstructed a two-way distinction on initial consonants based on voicing, with aspiration conditioned by pre-initial consonants that had been retained in Tibetic but lost in many other languages. [24] Thus, Benedict reconstructed the following initials: [25]

TB Tibetan Jingpho Burmese Garo Mizo S'gaw Karen Old Chinese [lower-alpha 3]
*kk(h)k(h) ~ gk(h)k(h) ~ gk(h)k(h)*k(h)
*ggg ~ k(h)kg ~ k(h)kk(h)*gh
ŋŋŋŋŋy
*tt(h)t(h) ~ dt(h)t(h) ~ dt(h)t(h)*t(h)
*ddd ~ t(h)td ~ t(h)dd*dh
*nnnnnnn*n ~ *ń
*pp(h)p(h) ~ bp(h)p(h) ~ bp(h)p(h)*p(h)
*bbb ~ p(h)pb ~ p(h)bb*bh
*mmmmmmm*m
*tsts(h)ts ~ dzts(h)s ~ tś(h)ss(h)*ts(h)
*dzdzdz ~ ts ~ śtstś(h)fs(h) ?
*ssssththθ*s
*zzz ~ śssfθ ?
*rrrrrrγ*l
*lllllll*l
*hhhhh*x
*wwwwww*gjw
*yyyytś ~ dźzy*dj ~ *zj

Although the initial consonants of cognates tend to have the same place and manner of articulation, voicing and aspiration are often unpredictable. [26] This irregularity was attacked by Roy Andrew Miller, [27] though Benedict's supporters attribute it to the effects of prefixes that have been lost and are often unrecoverable. [28] The issue remains unsolved today. [26] It was cited together with the lack of reconstructable shared morphology, and evidence that much shared lexical material has been borrowed from Chinese into Tibeto-Burman, by Christopher Beckwith, one of the few scholars still arguing that Chinese is not related to Tibeto-Burman. [29] [30]

Benedict also reconstructed, at least for Tibeto-Burman, prefixes such as the causative s-, the intransitive m-, and r-, b-g- and d- of uncertain function, as well as suffixes -s, -t and -n. [31]

Study of literary languages

Ancient Chinese text on bamboo strips Kongzi Shilun Manuscript from Shanghai Museum 1.jpg
Ancient Chinese text on bamboo strips

Old Chinese is by far the oldest recorded Sino-Tibetan language, with inscriptions dating from around 1250 BC and a huge body of literature from the first millennium BC. However, the Chinese script is logographic and does not represent sounds systematically; it is therefore difficult to reconstruct the phonology of the language from the written records. Scholars have sought to reconstruct the phonology of Old Chinese by comparing the obscure descriptions of the sounds of Middle Chinese in medieval dictionaries with phonetic elements in Chinese characters and the rhyming patterns of early poetry. The first complete reconstruction, the Grammata Serica Recensa of Bernard Karlgren, was used by Benedict and Shafer. [32]

Karlgren's reconstruction was somewhat unwieldy, with many sounds having a highly non-uniform distribution. Later scholars have revised it by drawing on a range of other sources. [33] Some proposals were based on cognates in other Sino-Tibetan languages, though workers have also found solely Chinese evidence for them. [34] For example, recent reconstructions of Old Chinese have reduced Karlgren's 15 vowels to a six-vowel system originally suggested by Nicholas Bodman. [35] Similarly, Karlgren's *l has been recast as *r, with a different initial interpreted as *l, matching Tibeto-Burman cognates, but also supported by Chinese transcriptions of foreign names. [36] A growing number of scholars believe that Old Chinese did not use tones and that the tones of Middle Chinese developed from final consonants. One of these, *-s, is believed to be a suffix, with cognates in other Sino-Tibetan languages. [37]

Old Tibetan text found at Turfan Turfan fragment tibt.jpg
Old Tibetan text found at Turfan

Tibetic has extensive written records from the adoption of writing by the Tibetan Empire in the mid-7th century. The earliest records of Burmese (such as the 12th-century Myazedi inscription) are more limited, but later an extensive literature developed. Both languages are recorded in alphabetic scripts ultimately derived from the Brahmi script of Ancient India. Most comparative work has used the conservative written forms of these languages, following the dictionaries of Jäschke (Tibetan) and Judson (Burmese), though both contain entries from a wide range of periods. [38]

There are also extensive records in Tangut, the language of the Western Xia (1038–1227). Tangut is recorded in a Chinese-inspired logographic script, whose interpretation presents many difficulties, even though multilingual dictionaries have been found. [39] [40]

Gong Hwang-cherng has compared Old Chinese, Tibetic, Burmese, and Tangut to establish sound correspondences between those languages. [23] [41] He found that Tibetic and Burmese /a/ correspond to two Old Chinese vowels, *a and *ə. [42] While this has been considered evidence for a separate Tibeto-Burman subgroup, Hill (2014) finds that Burmese has distinct correspondences for Old Chinese rhymes -ay : *-aj and -i : *-əj, and hence argues that the development *ə > *a occurred independently in Tibetan and Burmese. [43]

Fieldwork

The descriptions of non-literary languages used by Shafer and Benedict were often produced by missionaries and colonial administrators of varying linguistic skills. [44] [45] Most of the smaller Sino-Tibetan languages are spoken in inaccessible mountainous areas, many of which are politically or militarily sensitive and thus closed to investigators. Until the 1980s, the best-studied areas were Nepal and northern Thailand. [46] In the 1980s and 1990s, new surveys were published from the Himalayas and southwestern China. Of particular interest was the increasing literature on the Qiangic languages of western Sichuan and adjacent areas. [47] [48]

Distribution

Distribution of the larger branches of Sino-Tibetan, with proportion of first-language speakers:
.mw-parser-output .col-begin{border-collapse:collapse;padding:0;color:inherit;width:100%;border:0;margin:0}.mw-parser-output .col-begin-small{font-size:90%}.mw-parser-output .col-break{vertical-align:top;text-align:left}.mw-parser-output .col-break-2{width:50%}.mw-parser-output .col-break-3{width:33.3%}.mw-parser-output .col-break-4{width:25%}.mw-parser-output .col-break-5{width:20%}@media(max-width:720px){.mw-parser-output .col-begin,.mw-parser-output .col-begin>tbody,.mw-parser-output .col-begin>tbody>tr,.mw-parser-output .col-begin>tbody>tr>td{display:block!important;width:100%!important}.mw-parser-output .col-break{padding-left:0!important}}
.mw-parser-output .legend{page-break-inside:avoid;break-inside:avoid-column}.mw-parser-output .legend-color{display:inline-block;min-width:1.25em;height:1.25em;line-height:1.25;margin:1px 0;text-align:center;border:1px solid black;background-color:transparent;color:black}.mw-parser-output .legend-text{}
Sinitic (94.3%)
Lolo-Burmese (3.4%)
Tibetic (0.4%)
Karenic (0.3%)
others (1.6%) Sino-Tibetan language groups.svg
Distribution of the larger branches of Sino-Tibetan, with proportion of first-language speakers:

Most of the current spread of Sino-Tibetan languages is the result of historical expansions of the three groups with the most speakers – Chinese, Burmese and Tibetic – replacing an unknown number of earlier languages. These groups also have the longest literary traditions of the family. The remaining languages are spoken in mountainous areas, along the southern slopes of the Himalayas, the Southeast Asian Massif and the eastern edge of the Tibetan Plateau.

Contemporary languages

The branch with the largest number of speakers by far is the Sinitic languages, with 1.3 billion speakers, most of whom live in the eastern half of China. [50] The first records of Chinese are oracle bone inscriptions from c.1250 BC, when Old Chinese was spoken around the middle reaches of the Yellow River. [51] Chinese has since expanded throughout China, forming a family whose diversity has been compared with the Romance languages. Diversity is greater in the rugged terrain of southeast China than in the North China Plain. [52]

Burmese is the national language of Myanmar, and the first language of some 33 million people. [53] Burmese speakers first entered the northern Irrawaddy basin from what is now western Yunnan in the early ninth century, in conjunction with an invasion by Nanzhao that shattered the Pyu city-states. [54] Other Burmish languages are still spoken in Dehong Prefecture in the far west of Yunnan. [55] By the 11th century, their Pagan Kingdom had expanded over the whole basin. [54] The oldest texts, such as the Myazedi inscription, date from the early 12th century. [55] The closely related Loloish languages are spoken by 9 million people in the mountains of western Sichuan, Yunnan, and nearby areas in northern Myanmar, Thailand, Laos, and Vietnam. [56] [49]

The Tibetic languages are spoken by some 6 million people on the Tibetan Plateau and neighbouring areas in the Himalayas and western Sichuan. [57] They are descended from Old Tibetan, which was originally spoken in the Yarlung Valley before it was spread by the expansion of the Tibetan Empire in the seventh century. [58] Although the empire collapsed in the ninth century, Classical Tibetan remained influential as the liturgical language of Tibetan Buddhism. [59]

The remaining languages are spoken in upland areas. Southernmost are the Karen languages, spoken by 4 million people in the hill country along the Myanmar–Thailand border, with the greatest diversity in the Karen Hills, which are believed to be the homeland of the group. [60] The highlands stretching from northeast India to northern Myanmar contain over 100 highly diverse Sino-Tibetan languages. Other Sino-Tibetan languages are found along the southern slopes of the Himalayas and the eastern edge of the Tibetan plateau. [61] The 22 official languages listed in the Eighth Schedule to the Constitution of India include only two Sino-Tibetan languages, namely Meitei (officially called Manipuri) and Bodo.

Homeland

There has been a range of proposals for the Sino-Tibetan urheimat, reflecting the uncertainty about the classification of the family and its time depth. [62] Three major hypotheses for the place and time of Sino-Tibetan unity have been presented: [63]

Zhang et al. (2019) performed a computational phylogenetic analysis of 109 Sino-Tibetan languages to suggest a Sino-Tibetan homeland in northern China near the Yellow River basin. The study further suggests that there was an initial major split between the Sinitic and Tibeto-Burman languages approximately 4,200 to 7,800 years ago (with an average of 5,900 years ago), associated with the Yangshao and/or Majiayao cultures. [63] Sagart et al. (2019) performed another phylogenetic analysis based on different data and methods to arrive at the same conclusions to the homeland and divergence model but proposed an earlier root age of approximately 7,200 years ago, associating its origin with millet farmers of the late Cishan culture and early Yangshao culture. [70]

The origin and spread of the Sino-Tibetan language family.png
Hypothesised homeland and dispersal according to Sagart et al. (2019) [70]
Homeland and dispersal of the Sino-Tibetan languages (2).svg
Hypothesised homeland and dispersal according to van Driem (2005) [71]
Homeland and dispersal of the Sino-Tibetan languages.svg
Hypothesised homeland and dispersal according to Blench (2009) [72] [73]

Classification

Several low-level branches of the family, particularly Lolo-Burmese, have been securely reconstructed, but in the absence of a secure reconstruction of a Sino-Tibetan proto-language, the higher-level structure of the family remains unclear. [74] [75] Thus, a conservative classification of Sino-Tibetan/Tibeto-Burman would posit several dozen small coordinate families and isolates; attempts at subgrouping are either geographic conveniences or hypotheses for further research.[ citation needed ]

Li (1937)

In a survey in the 1937 Chinese Yearbook, Li Fang-Kuei described the family as consisting of four branches: [76] [77]

Tai and Miao–Yao were included because they shared isolating typology, tone systems and some vocabulary with Chinese. At the time, tone was considered so fundamental to language that tonal typology could be used as the basis for classification. In the Western scholarly community, these languages are no longer included in Sino-Tibetan, with the similarities attributed to diffusion across the Mainland Southeast Asia linguistic area, especially since Benedict (1942). [77] The exclusions of Vietnamese by Kuhn and of Tai and Miao–Yao by Benedict were vindicated in 1954 when André-Georges Haudricourt demonstrated that the tones of Vietnamese were reflexes of final consonants from Proto-Mon–Khmer. [78]

Many Chinese linguists continue to follow Li's classification. [lower-alpha 4] [77] However, this arrangement remains problematic. For example, there is disagreement over whether to include the entire Kra–Dai family or just Kam–Tai (Zhuang–Dong excludes the Kra languages), because the Chinese cognates that form the basis of the putative relationship are not found in all branches of the family and have not been reconstructed for the family as a whole. In addition, Kam–Tai itself no longer appears to be a valid node within Kra–Dai.

Benedict (1942)

Benedict overtly excluded Vietnamese (placing it in Mon–Khmer) as well as Hmong–Mien and Kra–Dai (placing them in Austro-Tai). He otherwise retained the outlines of Conrady's Indo-Chinese classification, though putting Karen in an intermediate position: [79] [80]

  • Sino-Tibetan
    • Chinese
    • Tibeto-Karen
      • Karen
      • Tibeto-Burman

Shafer (1955)

Shafer criticized the division of the family into Tibeto-Burman and Sino-Daic branches, which he attributed to the different groups of languages studied by Konow and other scholars in British India on the one hand and by Henri Maspero and other French linguists on the other. [81] He proposed a detailed classification, with six top-level divisions: [82] [83] [lower-alpha 5]

  • Sino-Tibetan
    • Sinitic
    • Daic
    • Bodic
    • Burmic
    • Baric
    • Karenic

Shafer was sceptical of the inclusion of Daic, but after meeting Maspero in Paris decided to retain it pending a definitive resolution of the question. [84] [85]

Matisoff (1978, 2015)

James Matisoff abandoned Benedict's Tibeto-Karen hypothesis:

  • Sino-Tibetan
    • Chinese
    • Tibeto-Burman

Some more-recent Western scholars, such as Bradley (1997) and La Polla (2003), have retained Matisoff's two primary branches, though differing in the details of Tibeto-Burman. However, Jacques (2006) notes, "comparative work has never been able to put forth evidence for common innovations to all the Tibeto-Burman languages (the Sino-Tibetan languages to the exclusion of Chinese)" [lower-alpha 6] and that "it no longer seems justified to treat Chinese as the first branching of the Sino-Tibetan family," [lower-alpha 7] because the morphological divide between Chinese and Tibeto-Burman has been bridged by recent reconstructions of Old Chinese.

The internal structure of Sino-Tibetan has been tentatively revised as the following Stammbaum by Matisoff in the final print release of the Sino-Tibetan Etymological Dictionary and Thesaurus (STEDT) in 2015. [86] Matisoff acknowledges that the position of Chinese within the family remains an open question. [87]

Starostin (1996)

Sergei Starostin proposed that both the Kiranti languages and Chinese are divergent from a "core" Tibeto-Burman of at least Bodish, Lolo-Burmese, Tamangic, Jinghpaw, Kukish, and Karen (other families were not analysed) in a hypothesis called Sino-Kiranti. The proposal takes two forms: that Sinitic and Kiranti are themselves a valid node or that the two are not demonstrably close so that Sino-Tibetan has three primary branches:

  • Sino-Tibetan (version 1)
    • Sino-Kiranti
    • Tibeto-Burman
  • Sino-Tibetan (version 2)
    • Chinese
    • Kiranti
    • Tibeto-Burman

Van Driem (1997, 2001)

George van Driem, like Shafer, rejects a primary split between Chinese and the rest, suggesting that Chinese owes its traditional privileged place in Sino-Tibetan to historical, typological, and cultural, rather than linguistic, criteria. He calls the entire family "Tibeto-Burman", a name he says has historical primacy, [88] but other linguists who reject a privileged position for Chinese nevertheless continue to call the resulting family "Sino-Tibetan".

Like Matisoff, van Driem acknowledges that the relationships of the "Kuki–Naga" languages (Kuki, Mizo, Meitei, etc.), both amongst each other and to the other languages of the family, remain unclear. However, rather than placing them in a geographic grouping, as Matisoff does, van Driem leaves them unclassified. He has proposed several hypotheses, including the reclassification of Chinese to a Sino-Bodic subgroup:

Van Driem points to two main pieces of evidence establishing a special relationship between Sinitic and Bodic and thus placing Chinese within the Tibeto-Burman family. First, there are some parallels between the morphology of Old Chinese and the modern Bodic languages. Second, there is a body of lexical cognates between the Chinese and Bodic languages, represented by the Kirantic language Limbu. [89]

In response, Matisoff notes that the existence of shared lexical material only serves to establish an absolute relationship between two language families, not their relative relationship to one another. Although some cognate sets presented by van Driem are confined to Chinese and Bodic, many others are found in Sino-Tibetan languages generally and thus do not serve as evidence for a special relationship between Chinese and Bodic. [90]

Van Driem's "fallen leaves" model (2001, 2014)

Van Driem has also proposed a "fallen leaves" model that lists dozens of well-established low-level groups while remaining agnostic about intermediate groupings of these. [91] In the most recent version (van Driem 2014), 42 groups are identified (with individual languages highlighted in italics): [92]

He also suggested (van Driem 2007) that the Sino-Tibetan language family be renamed "Trans-Himalayan", which he considers to be more neutral. [93]

Orlandi (2021) also considers the van Driem's Trans-Himalayan fallen leaves model to be more plausible than the bifurcate classification of Sino-Tibetan being split into Sinitic and Tibeto-Burman. [94]

Blench and Post (2014)

Roger Blench and Mark W. Post have criticized the applicability of conventional Sino-Tibetan classification schemes to minor languages lacking an extensive written history (unlike Chinese, Tibetic, and Burmese). They find that the evidence for the subclassification or even ST affiliation in all of several minor languages of northeastern India, in particular, is either poor or absent altogether.

While relatively little has been known about the languages of this region up to and including the present time, this has not stopped scholars from proposing that these languages either constitute or fall within some other Tibeto-Burman subgroup. However, in the absence of any sort of systematic comparison – whether the data are thought reliable or not – such "subgroupings" are essentially vacuous. The use of pseudo-genetic labels such as "Himalayish" and "Kamarupan" inevitably gives an impression of coherence which is at best misleading.

In their view, many such languages would for now be best considered unclassified, or "internal isolates" within the family. They propose a provisional classification of the remaining languages:

Following that, because they propose that the three best-known branches may be much closer related to each other than they are to "minor" Sino-Tibetan languages, Blench and Post argue that "Sino-Tibetan" or "Tibeto-Burman" are inappropriate names for a family whose earliest divergences led to different languages altogether. They support the proposed name "Trans-Himalayan".

Menghan Zhang, Shi Yan, et al. (2019)

A team of researchers led by Pan Wuyun and Jin Li proposed the following phylogenetic tree in 2019, based on lexical items: [95]

  • Sino-Tibetan
    • Sinitic
    • Tibeto-Burman
        • Karenic
        • Kuki-Chin–Naga
        • Sal
            • Digarish
            • Tani
              • Himalayish
              • Nungish
              • Kinauri
                  • Gurung-Tamang
                  • Bodish
                    • Naic
                    • Ersuish, Qiangic, Rgyalrongic
                  • Lolo-Burmese

Typology

Word order

Except for the Chinese, Bai, Karenic, and Mruic languages, the usual word order in Sino-Tibetan languages is object–verb. [96] However, Chinese and Bai differ from almost all other subject–verb–object languages in the world in placing relative clauses before the nouns they modify. [97] Most scholars believe SOV to be the original order, with Chinese, Karen, and Bai having acquired SVO order due to the influence of neighbouring languages in the Mainland Southeast Asia linguistic area. [98] [99] This has been criticized as being insufficiently corroborated by Djamouri et al. 2007, who instead reconstruct a VO order for Proto-Sino-Tibetan. [100]

Phonology

Contrastive tones are a feature found across the family although absent in some languages like Purik. [101] Phonation contrasts are also present among many, notably in the Lolo-Burmese group. [102] While Benedict contended that Proto-Tibeto-Burman would have a two-tone system, Matisoff refrained from reconstructing it since tones in individual languages may have developed independently through the process of tonogenesis. [103]

Morphology

The structure of words

Sino-Tibetan is structurally one of the most diverse language families in the world, including all of the gradation of morphological complexity from isolating (Lolo-Burmese, Tujia) to polysynthetic (Gyalrongic, Kiranti) languages. [70] While Sinitic languages are normally taken to be a prototypical example of the isolating morphological type, southern Chinese languages express this trait far more strongly than northern Chinese languages do. [104]

Voice and Voicing alternation

Initial consonant alternations related to transitivity are pervasive in Sino-Tibetan; while devoicing (or aspiration) of the initial is associated with a transitive/causative verb, voicing is linked to its intransitive/anticausative counterpart. [105] [106] This is argued to reflect morphological derivations that existed in earlier stages of the family. Even in Chinese, one would find semantically-related pairs of verbs such as 見 'to see' (MC: kenH) and 現 'to appear' (ɣenH), which are respectively reconstructed as *[k]ˤen-s and *N-[k]ˤen-s in the Baxter-Sagart system of Old Chinese. [106] [107]

Ergativity

In morphosyntactic alignment, many Tibeto-Burman languages have ergative and/or anti-ergative (an argument that is not an actor) case marking. However, the anti-ergative case markings can not be reconstructed at higher levels in the family and are thought to be innovations. [108]

Person indexation

Many Sino-Tibetan languages exhibit a system of person indexation. [109] Notably, Gyalrongic and Kiranti have an inverse marker prefixed to a transitive verb when the agent is lower than the patient in a certain person hierarchy. [110]

Hodgson had in 1849 noted a dichotomy between "pronominalized" (inflecting) languages, stretching across the Himalayas from Himachal Pradesh to eastern Nepal, and "non-pronominalized" (isolating) languages. Konow (1909) explained the pronominalized languages as due to a Munda substratum, with the idea that Indo-Chinese languages were essentially isolating as well as tonal. Maspero later attributed the putative substratum to Indo-Aryan. It was not until Benedict that the inflectional systems of these languages were recognized as (partially) native to the family. Scholars disagree over the extent to which the agreement system in the various languages can be reconstructed for the proto-language. [111] [112]

Evidentiality, mirativity, and egophoricity

Although not very common in some families and linguistic areas like Standard Average European, fairly complex systems of evidentiality (grammatical marking of information source) are found in many Tibeto-Burman languages. [113] The family has also contributed to the study of mirativity [114] [115] and egophoricity, [116] which are relatively new concepts in linguistic typology.

Vocabulary

Sino-Tibetan numerals
gloss Old Chinese [117] Old Tibetan [118] Old Burmese [118] Jingpho [119] Garo [119] Limbu [120] Kanauri [121] Tujia [122]
"one" *ʔjitacsaid
*tjek "single"gcigtacthik
"two" *njijsgnyisnhacgininɛtchinišne⁵⁵
"three" *sumgsumsumḥmə̀sūmgittamsumsisumso⁵⁵
"four" *sjijsbzhiliymə̀lībrilisipə:ze⁵⁵
"five" *ŋaʔlngaṅāḥmə̀ŋāboŋanasiṅaũ⁵⁵
"six" *C-rjukdrugkhrokkrúʔdoktuksițukwo²¹
"seven" *tsʰjitkhu-nacsə̀nìtsininusištišne²¹
"eight" *pretbrgyadrhacmə̀tshátchetyɛtchirəyje²¹
"nine" *kjuʔdgukuiḥcə̀khùsku [123] skusguikɨe⁵⁵
"ten" *gjəpkip [124] gip
bcuchayshīchikuŋsəy

Proposed external relationships

Beyond the traditionally recognized families of Southeast Asia, some possible broader relationships have been suggested.

Austronesian

Laurent Sagart proposes a "Sino-Austronesian" family with Sino-Tibetan and Austronesian (including Kra–Dai as a subbranch) as primary branches. Stanley Starosta has extended this proposal with a further branch called "Yangzian" joining Hmong–Mien and Austroasiatic. The proposal has been largely rejected by other linguists who argue that the similarities between Austronesian and Sino-Tibetan more likely arose from contact rather than being genetic. [125] [126] [127]

Dené–Yeniseian

As noted by Tailleur [128] and Werner, [129] some of the earliest proposals of genetic relations of Yeniseian, by M.A. Castrén (1856), James Byrne (1892), and G.J. Ramstedt (1907), suggested that Yeniseian was a northern relative of the Sino–Tibetan languages. These ideas were followed much later by Kai Donner [130] and Karl Bouda. [131] A 2008 study found further evidence for a possible relation between Yeniseian and Sino–Tibetan, citing several possible cognates. [132] Gao Jingyi (2014) identified twelve Sinitic and Yeniseian shared etymologies that belonged to the basic vocabulary, and argued that these Sino-Yeniseian etymologies could not be loans from either language into the other. [133]

The "Sino-Caucasian" hypothesis of Sergei Starostin posits that the Yeniseian languages form a clade with Sino-Tibetan, which he called Sino-Yeniseian. The Sino-Caucasian hypothesis has been expanded by others to "Dené–Caucasian" to include the Na-Dené languages of North America, Burushaski, Basque and, occasionally, Etruscan. A narrower binary Dené–Yeniseian family has recently been well received. The validity of the rest of the family, however, is viewed as doubtful or rejected by nearly all historical linguists. [134] [135] [136]

A link between the Na–Dené languages and Sino-Tibetan languages, known as Sino–Dené had also been proposed by Edward Sapir. Around 1920 Sapir became convinced that Na-Dené was more closely related to Sino-Tibetan than to other American families. [137] Edward Vadja's Dené–Yeniseian proposal renewed interest among linguists such as Geoffrey Caveney (2014) to look into support for the Sino–Dené hypothesis. Caveney considered a link between Sino-Tibetan, Na-Dené, and Yeniseian to be plausible but did not support the hypothesis that Sino-Tibetan and Na-Dené were related to the Caucasian languages (Sino–Caucasian and Dené–Caucasian). [138]

A 2023 analysis by David Bradley using the standard techniques of comparative linguistics supports a distant genetic link between the Sino-Tibetan, Na-Dené, and Yeniseian language families. Bradley argues that any similarities Sino-Tibetan shares with other language families of the East Asia area such as Hmong-Mien, Altaic (which is a sprachbund), Austroasiatic, Kra–Dai, Austronesian came through contact; but as there has been no recent contact between the Sino-Tibetan, Na-Dené, and Yeniseian language families, any similarities these groups share must be residual. [139]

Indo-European

August Conrad proposed the Sino-Tibetan-Indo-European language family.[ citation needed ] This hypothesis holds that there is a genetic relationship between the Sino-Tibetan language family and the Indo-European language family. The earliest comparative linguistic study of Chinese and Indo-European languages was by the 18th-century Nordic scholar Olaus Rudbeck. He compared the vocabulary of Gothic and Chinese and guessed that the two may be of the same origin. In the second half of the 19th century, Kong Haogu, Shigude, Ijosser, etc. successively proposed that Chinese and European languages are homologous. Among them, Kong Haogu, through the comparison of Chinese and Indo-European domestic animal vocabulary, first proposed an Indo-Chinese language macrofamily (including Chinese, Tibetan, Burmese, and Indo-European languages).

In the 20th century, R. Shafer put forward the conjecture of a Eurasial language super-family and listed hundreds of similar words between Tibeto-Burman and Indo-European languages. [140] [141]

Notes

  1. Kuhn (1889), p. 189: "wir das Tibetisch-Barmanische einerseits, das Chinesisch-Siamesische anderseits als deutlich geschiedene und doch wieder verwandte Gruppen einer einheitlichen Sprachfamilie anzuerkennen haben." (also quoted in van Driem (2001), p. 264.)
  2. The volumes were: 1. Introduction and bibliography, 2. Bhotish, 3. West Himalayish, 4. West Central Himalayish, 5. East Himalayish, 6. Digarish, 7. Nungish, 8. Dzorgaish, 9. Hruso, 10. Dhimalish, 11. Baric, 12. Burmish–Lolish, 13. Kachinish, 14. Kukish, 15. Mruish. [19]
  3. Karlgren's reconstruction, with aspiration as 'h' and 'i̯' as 'j' to aid comparison.
  4. See, for example, the "Sino-Tibetan" (汉藏语系 Hàn-Zàng yǔxì) entry in the "languages" (語言文字, Yǔyán-Wénzì) volume of the Encyclopedia of China (1988).
  5. For Shafer, the suffix "-ic" denoted a primary division of the family, whereas the suffix "-ish" denoted a sub-division of one of those.
  6. les travaux de comparatisme n'ont jamais pu mettre en évidence l'existence d'innovations communes à toutes les langues « tibéto-birmanes » (les langues sino-tibétaines à l'exclusion du chinois)
  7. il ne semble plus justifié de traiter le chinois comme le premier embranchement primaire de la famille sino-tibétaine

Related Research Articles

The Tani language, often referred to as Tani languages, encompasses a group of closely related languages spoken by the Tani people in the northeastern region of India, primarily in the state of Arunachal Pradesh and Assam. These languages belong to the Sino-Tibetan family and include several major dialects such as Nyishi, Galo, Apatani, Adi, Tagin, and Mising.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Qiangic languages</span> Language family

Qiangic is a group of related languages within the Sino-Tibetan language family. They are spoken mainly in Southwest China, including Sichuan and northern Yunnan. Most Qiangic languages are distributed in the prefectures of Ngawa, Garzê, Ya'an and Liangshan in Sichuan with some in Northern Yunnan as well.

The Sal languages, also known as the Brahmaputran languages, are a branch of Tibeto-Burman languages spoken in northeast India, as well as parts of Bangladesh, Myanmar (Burma), and China.

Bodish, named for the Tibetan ethnonym Bod, is a proposed grouping consisting of the Tibetic languages and associated Sino-Tibetan languages spoken in Tibet, North India, Nepal, Bhutan, and North Pakistan. It has not been demonstrated that all these languages form a clade, characterized by shared innovations, within Sino-Tibetan.

The Tibeto-Kanauri languages, also called Bodic, Bodish–Himalayish, and Western Tibeto-Burman, are a proposed intermediate level of classification of the Sino-Tibetan languages, centered on the Tibetic languages and the Kinnauri dialect cluster. The conception of the relationship, or if it is even a valid group, varies between researchers.

The West Himalayish languages, also known as Almora and Kanauric, are a family of Sino-Tibetan languages centered in Himachal Pradesh, Uttarakhand and across the border into Nepal. LaPolla (2003) proposes that the West Himalayish languages may be part of a larger "Rung" group.

The Nung or Nungish languages are a poorly described family of uncertain affiliation within the Sino-Tibetan languages spoken in Yunnan, China and Burma. They include:

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Lolo-Burmese languages</span> Sino-Tibetan language group of Southeast Asia

The Lolo-Burmese languages of Burma and Southern China form a coherent branch of the Sino-Tibetan family.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Tibeto-Burman languages</span> Group of the Sino-Tibetan language family

The Tibeto-Burman languages are the non-Sinitic members of the Sino-Tibetan language family, over 400 of which are spoken throughout the Southeast Asian Massif ("Zomia") as well as parts of East Asia and South Asia. Around 60 million people speak Tibeto-Burman languages. The name derives from the most widely spoken of these languages, Burmese and the Tibetic languages, which also have extensive literary traditions, dating from the 12th and 7th centuries respectively. Most of the other languages are spoken by much smaller communities, and many of them have not been described in detail.

Sino-Austronesian or Sino-Tibetan-Austronesian is a proposed language family suggested by Laurent Sagart in 1990. Using reconstructions of Old Chinese, Sagart argued that the Austronesian languages are related to the Sinitic languages phonologically, lexically and morphologically. Sagart later accepted the Sino-Tibetan languages as a valid group and extended his proposal to include the rest of Sino-Tibetan. He also placed the Tai–Kadai languages within the Austronesian family as a sister branch of Malayo-Polynesian. The proposal has been largely rejected by other linguists who argue that the similarities between Austronesian and Sino-Tibetan more likely arose from contact rather than being genetic.

There have been various classification schemes for Southeast Asian languages.

Proto-Tibeto-Burman is the reconstructed ancestor of the Tibeto-Burman languages, that is, the Sino-Tibetan languages, except for Chinese. An initial reconstruction was produced by Paul K. Benedict and since refined by James Matisoff. Several other researchers argue that the Tibeto-Burman languages sans Chinese do not constitute a monophyletic group within Sino-Tibetan, and therefore that Proto-Tibeto-Burman was the same language as Proto-Sino-Tibetan.

Sak is a Sino-Tibetan language of the Sal branch spoken in Bangladesh and Myanmar by the Chak people.

The Luish, Asakian, or Sak languages are a group of Sino-Tibetan languages belonging to the Sal branch. They are spoken in Burma and Bangladesh, and consist of the Sak, Kadu, and Ganan languages. In recent years, Luish languages have been influenced by Burmese and Chakma.

Scott DeLancey is an American linguist from the University of Oregon. His work focuses on typology and historical linguistics of Tibeto-Burman languages as well as North American indigenous languages such as the Penutian family, particularly the Klamath. His research is known for its diversity of its thematic and theoretical reach.

The Burmo-Qiangic or Eastern Tibeto-Burman languages are a proposed family of Sino-Tibetan languages spoken in Southwest China and Myanmar. It consists of the Lolo-Burmese and Qiangic branches, including the extinct Tangut language.

Mruic or Mru–Hkongso is a small group of Sino-Tibetan languages consisting of two languages, Mru and Anu-Hkongso. Their relationship within Sino-Tibetan is unclear.

Proto-Sino-Tibetan (PST) is the hypothetical linguistic reconstruction of the Sino-Tibetan proto-language and the common ancestor of all languages in it, including the Sinitic languages, the Tibetic languages, Yi, Bai, Burmese, Karen, Tangut, and Naga. Paul K. Benedict (1972) placed a particular emphasis on Old Chinese, Classical Tibetan, Jingpho, Written Burmese, Garo, and Mizo in his discussion of Proto-Sino-Tibetan.

The East Asian languages are a language family proposed by Stanley Starosta in 2001. The proposal has since been adopted by George van Driem and others.

The Sino-Tibetan Etymological Dictionary and Thesaurus was a linguistics research project hosted at the University of California at Berkeley. The project, which focused on Sino-Tibetan historical linguistics, started in 1987 and lasted until 2015.

References

Citations

  1. van Driem (2014), p. 16.
  2. List, Lai & Starostin (2019), p. 1.
  3. Handel (2008), p. 422.
  4. "Sino Tibetan Languages" . Retrieved December 30, 2023.
  5. Handel (2008), pp. 422, 434–436.
  6. Sagart et al. (2019), p. 10317.
  7. Logan (1856), p. 31.
  8. Logan (1858).
  9. 1 2 Hale (1982), p. 4.
  10. van Driem (2001), p. 334.
  11. Klaproth (1823), pp. 346, 363–365.
  12. van Driem (2001), p. 344.
  13. Finck (1909), p. 57.
  14. 1 2 Przyluski (1924), p. 361.
  15. Sapir (1925), p. 373.
  16. Przyluski (1924), p. 380.
  17. Przyluski & Luce (1931).
  18. van Driem (2014), p. 15.
  19. Miller (1974), p. 195.
  20. Miller (1974), pp. 195–196.
  21. Benedict (1972), p. v.
  22. Matisoff (1991), p. 473.
  23. 1 2 Handel (2008), p. 434.
  24. Benedict (1972), pp. 20–21.
  25. Benedict (1972), pp. 17–18, 133–139, 164–171.
  26. 1 2 Handel (2008), pp. 425–426.
  27. Miller (1974), p. 197.
  28. Matisoff (2003), p. 16.
  29. Beckwith (1996).
  30. Beckwith (2002b).
  31. Benedict (1972), pp. 98–123.
  32. Matisoff (1991), pp. 471–472.
  33. Norman (1988), p. 45.
  34. Baxter (1992), pp. 25–26.
  35. Bodman (1980), p. 47.
  36. Baxter (1992), pp. 197, 199–202.
  37. Baxter (1992), pp. 315–317.
  38. Beckwith (2002a), pp. xiii–xiv.
  39. Thurgood (2003), p. 17.
  40. Hill (2015).
  41. Gong (1980).
  42. Handel (2008), p. 431.
  43. Hill (2014), pp. 97–104.
  44. Matisoff (1991), pp. 472–473.
  45. Hale (1982), pp. 4–5.
  46. Matisoff (1991), pp. 470, 476–478.
  47. Handel (2008), p. 435.
  48. Matisoff (1991), p. 482.
  49. 1 2 Eberhard, Simons & Fennig (2019).
  50. Eberhard, Simons & Fennig (2019), "Chinese".
  51. Norman (1988), p. 4.
  52. Norman (1988), pp. 187–188.
  53. Eberhard, Simons & Fennig (2019), "Burmese".
  54. 1 2 Taylor (1992), p. 165.
  55. 1 2 Wheatley (2003), p. 195.
  56. Thurgood (2003), pp. 8–9.
  57. Tournadre (2014), p. 117.
  58. Tournadre (2014), p. 107.
  59. Tournadre (2014), p. 120.
  60. Thurgood (2003), p. 18.
  61. Handel (2008), pp. 424–425.
  62. Handel (2008), p. 423.
  63. 1 2 3 Zhang et al. (2019), p. 112.
  64. Archaeological evidence for initial migration of Neolithic Proto Sino-Tibetan speakers from Yellow River valley to Tibetan Plateau. Li Liua,, Jian Chen, Jiajing Wang, Yanan Zhao, and Xingcan Chen. Edited by Melinda Zeder, Smithsonian Institution, Frederick, MD; received July 12, 2022; accepted October 22, 2022. PNAS. https://www.pnas.org/doi/epdf/10.1073/pnas.2212006119
  65. Matisoff (1991), pp. 470–471.
  66. van Driem (2005), pp. 91–95.
  67. Blench (2009).
  68. Blench & Post (2014), p. 89.
  69. Blench & Post (2014), pp. 90, 92.
  70. 1 2 3 Sagart et al. (2019), pp. 10319–10320.
  71. van Driem (2005), pp. 94–97.
  72. Blench (2009), p. 14.
  73. Blench, Roger; Post, Mark (2010). "NE Indian languages and NE Indian languages and the origin of Sino the origin of Sino-Tibetan" (PDF). rogerblench.info. p. 20. Retrieved 2021-10-28.
  74. Handel (2008), p. 426.
  75. DeLancey (2009), p. 695.
  76. Li (1937), pp. 60–63.
  77. 1 2 3 Handel (2008), p. 424.
  78. Matisoff (1991), p. 487.
  79. Benedict (1942), p. 600.
  80. Benedict (1972), pp. 2–4.
  81. Shafer (1955), pp. 94–96.
  82. Shafer (1955), pp. 99–108.
  83. Shafer (1966), p. 1.
  84. Shafer (1955), pp. 97–99.
  85. van Driem (2001), pp. 343–344.
  86. Matisoff (2015), pp. xxxii, 1123–1127.
  87. Matisoff (2015), p. xxxi.
  88. van Driem (2001), p. 383.
  89. van Driem (1997).
  90. Matisoff (2000).
  91. van Driem (2001), p. 403.
  92. van Driem (2014), p. 19.
  93. van Driem (2007), p. 226.
  94. Orlandi, Georg (2021). "Once again on the history and validity of the Sino-Tibetan bifurcate model". Journal of Language Relationship . 19 (3–4): 263–292.
  95. Zhang et al. (2019), p. 113.
  96. Dryer (2003), p. 43.
  97. Dryer (2003), pp. 50.
  98. Dryer (2003), pp. 43–45.
  99. Charles N. Li & Sandra A. Thompson (1974). "An explanation of word order change SVO > SOV". Foundations of Language. 12: 201–214.
  100. Djamouri, Redouane; Paul, Wautraud; Whitman, John (2007). "Reconstructing VO constituent order for proto-Sino-Tibetan". 8th International Conference on Historical Linguistics.
  101. Benedict 1972, p. 85.
  102. Matisoff 2003, p. 241.
  103. Matisoff 2003, p. 12.
  104. Arcodia, Giorgio Francesco; Basciano, Bianca (2020-01-30). "Morphology in Sino-Tibetan Languages". Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Linguistics. doi:10.1093/acrefore/9780199384655.013.530. ISBN   978-0-19-938465-5 . Retrieved 2021-11-26.
  105. LaPolla (2003), pp. 23.
  106. 1 2 Shuya, Jacques & Yunfan 2019, p. 74.
  107. Baxter & Sagart 2014, p. 54.
  108. LaPolla (2003), pp. 34–35.
  109. Jacques & Pellard 2022, p. 2.
  110. Jacques & Pellard 2022, pp. 3–4.
  111. Handel (2008), p. 430.
  112. LaPolla (2003), pp. 29–32.
  113. Aikhenvald & LaPolla 2007, p. 3.
  114. DeLancey 1997.
  115. Aikhenvald & LaPolla 2007, p. 11.
  116. San Roque, Floyd & Norcliffe 2018, pp. 5–6.
  117. Baxter (1992).
  118. 1 2 Hill (2012).
  119. 1 2 Burling (1983), p. 28.
  120. van Driem (1987), pp. 32–33.
  121. Sharma (1988), p. 116.
  122. Tian Desheng and He Tianzhen, et al., eds. 1986. . Beijing: Nationalities Press. Accessed via STEDT database https://stedt.berkeley.edu/search/ on 2021-04-05.
  123. Gutman, Alejandro. "Garo". Languages Gulper. Retrieved 2020-12-18.
  124. Yanson (2006), p. 106.
  125. Li, Paul Jenkuei (1995). "Is Chinese genetically related to Austronesian?". In Wang, William S-Y. (ed.). The Ancestry of the Chinese Language. Journal of Chinese Linguistics Monograph Series. Vol. 8. Chinese University Press. pp. 92–112. JSTOR   23826144.
  126. Blust, Robert (1995). "An Austronesianist looks at Sino-Austronesian". In Wang, William S-Y. (ed.). The Ancestry of the Chinese Language. Journal of Chinese Linguistics Monograph Series. Vol. 8. Chinese University Press. pp. 283–298. JSTOR   23826144.
  127. Vovin, Alexander (1997). "The comparative method and ventures beyond Sino-Tibetan". Journal of Chinese Linguistics. 25 (2): 308–336. JSTOR   23756693.
  128. See Tailleur 1994
  129. See Werner 1994
  130. See Donner 1930
  131. See Bouda 1963 and Bouda 1957
  132. Sedláček, Kamil (2008). "The Yeniseian Languages of the 18th Century and Ket and Sino-Tibetan Word Comparisons". Central Asiatic Journal. 52 (2): 219–305. doi:10.13173/CAJ/2008/2/6. ISSN   0008-9192. JSTOR   41928491. S2CID   163603829.
  133. 高晶一, Jingyi Gao (2017). "Xia and Ket Identified by Sinitic and Yeniseian Shared Etymologies // 確定夏國及凱特人的語言為屬於漢語族和葉尼塞語系共同詞源". Central Asiatic Journal. 60 (1–2): 51–58. doi:10.13173/centasiaj.60.1-2.0051. ISSN   0008-9192. JSTOR   10.13173/centasiaj.60.1-2.0051. S2CID   165893686.
  134. Goddard, Ives (1996). "The Classification of the Native Languages of North America". In Ives Goddard, ed., "Languages". Vol. 17 of William Sturtevant, ed., Handbook of North American Indians. Washington, D.C.: Smithsonian Institution. pg. 318
  135. Trask, R. L. (2000). The Dictionary of Historical and Comparative Linguistics. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press. pg. 85
  136. Sanchez-Mazas, Alicia; Blench, Roger; Ross, Malcolm D.; Peiros, Ilia; Lin, Marie (2008). Past Human Migrations in East Asia: Matching Archaeology, Linguistics and Genetics. Routledge. ISBN   9781134149629.
  137. Ruhlen, Merritt (1998-11-10). "The origin of the Na-Dene". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 95 (23): 13994–13996. Bibcode:1998PNAS...9513994R. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.23.13994 . ISSN   0027-8424. PMC   25007 . PMID   9811914.
  138. Caveney, Geoffrey (2014). "Sino-Tibetan ŋ- and Na-Dene *kw- / *gw- / *xw-: 1st Person Pronouns and Lexical Cognate Sets". Journal of Chinese Linguistics. 42 (2): 461–487. JSTOR   24774894.
  139. Bradley, David (2023-07-24). "Ancient Connections of Sinitic". Languages. 8 (3): 176. doi: 10.3390/languages8030176 . ISSN   2226-471X.
  140. R. Shafer. Eurasial. Orbis. 1963, 12: 19–14.
  141. R. Shafer. The Eurasial Linguistic Superfamily. Anthropos. 1965, 60: 1965.

Works cited

General