Plant intelligence

Last updated
J. C. Bose has been described as the "father of plant neurobiology" J.C.Bose.JPG
J. C. Bose has been described as the "father of plant neurobiology"

Plant intelligence (also known as plant cognition or plant neurobiology) is a field of plant biology which aims to understand how plants process the information they obtain from their environment. [2] [3] [4] Plant neurobiological researchers claim that plants possess abilities associated with cognition including anticipation, decision making, learning and memory. [3] [5] [6]

Contents

Terminology used in plant neurobiology is rejected by the majority of plant scientists as misleading as plants do not possess consciousness or neurons. [7] [8] [9] [10]

History

Early research

In 1811, James Perchard Tupper authored An Essay on the Probability of Sensation in Vegetables which argued that plants possess a low form of sensation. [11] [12] He has been cited as an early botanist "attracted to the notion that the ability of plants to feel pain or pleasure demonstrated the universal beneficence of a Creator". [13]

The notion that plants are capable of feeling emotions was first recorded in 1848, when Gustav Fechner, an experimental psychologist, suggested that plants are capable of emotions and that one could promote healthy growth with talk, attention, attitude, and affection. [14] Federico Delpino wrote about plant intelligence in 1867. [15]

The idea of cognition in plants was explored by Charles Darwin in 1880 in the book The Power of Movement in Plants , co-authored with his son Francis. Using a neurological metaphor, he described the sensitivity of plant roots in proposing that the tip of roots acts like the brain of some lower animals. This involves reacting to sensation in order to determine their next movement [16] even though plants possess neither brains nor nerves. John Ellor Taylor in his 1884 book The Sagacity and Morality of Plants argued that plants are conscious agents. [17]

Jagadish Chandra Bose invented various devices and instruments to measure electrical responses in plants. [18] [19] According to biologist Patrick Geddes "In his investigations on response in general Bose had found that even ordinary plants and their different organs were sensitive— exhibiting, under mechanical or other stimuli, an electric response, indicative of excitation." [20] One visitor to his laboratory, the vegetarian playwright George Bernard Shaw, was intensely disturbed upon witnessing a demonstration in which a cabbage had "convulsions" as it boiled to death. [21] Jagadish Chandra Bose is considered an important forerunner of plant neurobiology by proponents of plant cognition. [22] [23] [1] Bose was the author of The Nervous Mechanism of Plants , published in 1926. Karl F. Kellerman, Associate Chief of the Bureau of Plant Industry, United States Department of Agriculture criticized Bose's interpretation of the results from his experiments, stating that he failed to prove the conclusions from his reports that plants feel pain. Kellerman commented that "Sir Jagadar passed an electric current through plants, and his instruments recorded a break in the current. Such variations in resistance to electric current are found even when passing a current through dead matter". [24]

In 1900, ornithologist Thomas G. Gentry authored Intelligence in Plants and Animals which argued that plants have consciousness. Historian Ed Folsom described it as "an exhaustive investigation of how such animals as bees, ants, worms and buzzards, as well as all kinds of plants, display intelligence and thus have souls". [25] Captain Arthur Smith in the early 1900s authored the first article on "plant consciousness". [26] [27] In 1905, Rev. Charles Fletcher Argyll Saxby authored a pamphlet, Do Plants Think? Some speculations concerning a neurology and psychology of plants. [28] Maurice Maeterlinck wrote about the intelligence of flowers in 1907. [29]

Cleve Backster

Cleve Backster in 1969 Cleve Backster 1969.png
Cleve Backster in 1969

In the 1960s Cleve Backster, an interrogation specialist with the CIA, conducted research that led him to believe that plants can feel and respond to emotions and intents from other organisms including humans. Backster's interest in the subject began in February 1966 when he tried to measure the rate at which water rises from a philodendron's root into its leaves. Because a polygraph or "lie detector" can measure electrical resistance, which would alter when the plant was watered, he attached a polygraph to one of the plant's leaves. Backster stated that, to his immense surprise, "the tracing began to show a pattern typical of the response you get when you subject a human to emotional stimulation of short duration". [30] His ideas about primary perception (plants responding to emotions and intents) became known as the "Backster effect". [31] [32]

In 1975, K. A. Horowitz, D. C. Lewis and E. L. Gasteiger published an article in Science giving their results when repeating one of Backster's effects plant response to the killing of brine shrimp in boiling water. [33] The researchers grounded the plants to reduce electrical interference and rinsed them to remove dust particles. As a control, three of five pipettes contained brine shrimp while the remaining two only had water; the pipettes were delivered to the boiling water at random. This investigation used a total of 60 brine shrimp deliveries to boiling water while Backster's had used 13. Positive correlations did not occur at a rate great enough to be considered statistically significant. [33] Other controlled experiments that attempted to replicate Backster's findings also produced negative results. [34] [35] [36] [37]

Botanist Arthur Galston and physiologist Clifford L. Slayman who investigated Backster's claims wrote:

There is no objective scientific evidence for the existence of such complex behaviour in plants. The recent spate of popular literature on "plant consciousness" appears to have been triggered by "experiments" with a lie detector, subsequently reported and embellished in a book called The Secret Life of Plants . Unfortunately, when scientists in the discipline of plant physiology attempted to repeat the experiments, using either identical or improved equipment, the results were uniformly negative. Further investigation has shown that the original observations probably arose from defective measuring procedures. [34]

John M. Kmetz noted that the Backster effect was based on observations of only seven plants which nobody including Backster was able to replicate. [31]

The television show MythBusters also performed experiments (season 4, episode 18, 2006) to test the concept. The tests involved connecting plants to a polygraph galvanometer and employing actual and imagined harm upon the plants or upon others in the plants' vicinity. The galvanometer showed a reaction about one third of the time. The experimenters, who were in the room with the plant, posited that the vibrations of their actions or the room itself could have affected the polygraph. After isolating the plant, the polygraph showed a response slightly less than one third of the time. Later experiments with an EEG failed to detect anything. The show concluded that the results were not repeatable, and that the theory was not true. [38]

Backster's research was cited in the pseudoscientific book The Secret Life of Plants in 1973. [33] [39] Whilst the book captured public attention it severely damaged the credibility of the field of plant intelligence. Philosopher Yogi H. Hendlin noted that the book's "combination of haphazard, panpsychist metaphysical speculations and unmethodical citizen science stigmatised legitimate progressive plant research, alongside the era’s new-age pseudoscience, tarring the discipline’s serious inquiry". [40]

Modern research

Anthony Trewavas is credited with reintroducing the idea of plant intelligence in the early 2000s. [29] [41] [42] Modern research on plant cognition is conducted by researchers associated with the Society for Plant Neurobiology that was established in 2005. [6] Due to criticisms from botanists and complaints from early members that affiliations with the Society were negatively impacting their careers, the Society was renamed the Society of Plant Signaling and Behavior (SPSB) in 2009. [6] [43]

In 2003, Anthony Trewavas led a study to see how the roots interact with one another and study their signal transduction methods. He was able to draw similarities between water stress signals in plants affecting developmental changes and signal transductions in neural networks causing responses in muscle. [41] Particularly, when plants are under water stress, there are abscisic acid dependent and independent effects on development. [44] This brings to light further possibilities of plant decision-making based on its environmental stresses. The integration of multiple chemical interactions show evidence of the complexity in these root systems. [45]

In 2012, Paco Calvo Garzón and Fred Keijzer speculated that plants exhibited structures equivalent to (1) action potentials (2) neurotransmitters and (3) synapses. Also, they stated that a large part of plant activity takes place underground, and that the notion of a 'root brain' was first mooted by Charles Darwin in 1880. Free movement was not necessarily a criterion of cognition, they held. The authors gave five conditions of minimal cognition in living beings, and concluded that 'plants are cognitive in a minimal, embodied sense that also applies to many animals and even bacteria.' [46] In 2017 biologists from University of Birmingham announced that they found a "decision-making center" in the root tip of dormant Arabidopsis seeds. [47]

In 2014, Anthony Trewavas released a book called Plant Behavior and Intelligence that highlighted a plant's cognition through its colonial-organization skills reflecting insect swarm behaviors. [48] This organizational skill reflects the plant's ability to interact with its surroundings to improve its survivability, and a plant's ability to identify exterior factors. Evidence of the plant's minimal cognition of spatial awareness can be seen in their root allocation relative to neighboring plants. [46] The organization of these roots have been found to originate from the root tip of plants. [49]

On the other hand, Peter A. Crisp and his colleagues proposed a different view on plant memory in their review: plant memory could be advantageous under recurring and predictable stress; however, resetting or forgetting about the brief period of stress may be more beneficial for plants to grow as soon as the desirable condition returns. [50]

Affifi (2018) proposed an empirical approach to examining the ways plants model coordinate goal-based behaviour to environmental contingency as a way of understanding plant learning. [51] According to this author, associative learning will only demonstrate intelligence if it is seen as part of teleologically integrated activity. Otherwise, it can be reduced to mechanistic explanation.

In 2017 Yokawa, K. et al. found that, when exposed to anesthetics, a number of plants lost both their autonomous and touch-induced movements. Venus flytraps no longer generate electrical signals and their traps remain open when trigger hairs were touched, and growing pea tendrils stopped their autonomous movements and were immobilized in a curled shape. [52]

Raja et al (2020) found that potted French bean plants, when planted 30 centimetres from a garden cane, would adjust their growth patterns to enable themselves to use the cane as a support in the future. Raja later stated that "If the movement of plants is controlled and affected by objects in their vicinity, then we are talking about more complex behaviours (rather than simple) reactions". Raja proposed that researchers should look for corresponding cognitive signatures. [53] [54]

A minority of researchers within the field of plant neurobiology argue that plants are conscious organisms. [55] [56] [57] Peter Wohlleben argued for plant sentience in his 2016 book The Hidden Life of Trees . [58] The book was widely criticized by biologists and forest scientists for using strong anthropomorphic and teleological language such as describing trees as having friendships and registering fear, love and pain. [58] It has been described as containing a "conglomeration of half-truths, biased judgements, and wishful thinking". [58] František Baluška argues for a model called the Cellular Basis of Consciousness (CBC) which proposes that all cells are conscious. [55] The model has been criticized for being based on only speculation and lacking empirical evidence for its claim that cells have consciousness. [59] [60]

Criticism

The idea of plant cognition is a source of controversy and is rejected by the majority of plant scientists. [7] [8] [9] [61] Plant neurobiology has been criticized for misleading the public with false terminology. [8] [62] There is no scientific evidence that plants possess consciousness or are sentient. [7] [8] [9] [63]

Amadeo Alpi and 35 other scientists published an article in 2007 titled "Plant Neurobiology: No Brain, No Gain?" in Trends in Plant Science . [7] In this article, they argue that since there is no evidence for the presence of neurons in plants, the idea of plant neurobiology and cognition is unfounded and needs to be redefined. [7] They commented that "plant neurobiology does not add to our understanding of plant physiology, plant cell biology or signaling". [7] In response to this article, Francisco Calvo Garzón published an article in Plant Signaling and Behavior. [5] He states that, while plants do not have neurons as animals do, they do possess an information-processing system composed of cells. He argues that this system can be used as a basis for discussing the cognitive abilities of plants.

See also

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Pollen tube</span> Tubular structure to conduct male gametes of plants to the female gametes

A pollen tube is a tubular structure produced by the male gametophyte of seed plants when it germinates. Pollen tube elongation is an integral stage in the plant life cycle. The pollen tube acts as a conduit to transport the male gamete cells from the pollen grain—either from the stigma to the ovules at the base of the pistil or directly through ovule tissue in some gymnosperms. In maize, this single cell can grow longer than 12 inches (30 cm) to traverse the length of the pistil.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Casparian strip</span> Thickening in the root endodermis of vascular plants

The Casparian strip is a band-like thickening in the center of the root endodermis of vascular plants. The composition of the region is mainly suberin, lignin and some structural proteins, which are capable of reducing the diffusive apoplastic flow of water and solutes into the stele and its width varies between species. The Casparian strip is impervious to water so can control the transportation of water and inorganic salts between the cortex and the vascular bundle, preventing water and inorganic salts from being transported to the stele through the apoplast, so that it must enter the cell membrane and move to the stele through the symplastic pathway, blocking the internal and external objects of the cell. The function of mass transportation are similar to that of animal tissues.. The development of the Casparian strip is regulated by transcription factors such as SHORT-ROOT (SHR), SCARECROW (SCR) and MYB36, as well as polypeptide hormone synthesised by midcolumn cells.

A gamma wave or gamma rhythm is a pattern of neural oscillation in humans with a frequency between 30 and 100 Hz, the 40 Hz point being of particular interest. Gamma rhythms are correlated with large-scale brain network activity and cognitive phenomena such as working memory, attention, and perceptual grouping, and can be increased in amplitude via meditation or neurostimulation. Altered gamma activity has been observed in many mood and cognitive disorders such as Alzheimer's disease, epilepsy, and schizophrenia.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">The Crow and the Pitcher</span> Aesops fable

The Crow and the Pitcher is one of Aesop's Fables, numbered 390 in the Perry Index. It relates ancient observation of corvid behaviour that recent scientific studies have confirmed is goal-directed and indicative of causal knowledge rather than simply being due to instrumental conditioning.

Anthony James Trewavas is Emeritus Professor in the School of Biological Sciences of the University of Edinburgh best known for his research in the fields of plant physiology and molecular biology. His research investigates plant behaviour.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Plant perception (physiology)</span> Plants interaction to environment

Plant perception is the ability of plants to sense and respond to the environment by adjusting their morphology and physiology. Botanical research has revealed that plants are capable of reacting to a broad range of stimuli, including chemicals, gravity, light, moisture, infections, temperature, oxygen and carbon dioxide concentrations, parasite infestation, disease, physical disruption, sound, and touch. The scientific study of plant perception is informed by numerous disciplines, such as plant physiology, ecology, and molecular biology.

Comparative cognition is the comparative study of the mechanisms and origins of cognition in various species, and is sometimes seen as more general than, or similar to, comparative psychology. From a biological point of view, work is being done on the brains of fruit flies that should yield techniques precise enough to allow an understanding of the workings of the human brain on a scale appreciative of individual groups of neurons rather than the more regional scale previously used. Similarly, gene activity in the human brain is better understood through examination of the brains of mice by the Seattle-based Allen Institute for Brain Science, yielding the freely available Allen Brain Atlas. This type of study is related to comparative cognition, but better classified as one of comparative genomics. Increasing emphasis in psychology and ethology on the biological aspects of perception and behavior is bridging the gap between genomics and behavioral analysis.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Animal consciousness</span> Quality or state of self-awareness within an animal

Animal consciousness, or animal awareness, is the quality or state of self-awareness within an animal, or of being aware of an external object or something within itself. In humans, consciousness has been defined as: sentience, awareness, subjectivity, qualia, the ability to experience or to feel, wakefulness, having a sense of selfhood, and the executive control system of the mind. Despite the difficulty in definition, many philosophers believe there is a broadly shared underlying intuition about what consciousness is.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Pain in crustaceans</span> Ethical debate

There is a scientific debate which questions whether crustaceans experience pain. It is a complex mental state, with a distinct perceptual quality but also associated with suffering, which is an emotional state. Because of this complexity, the presence of pain in an animal, or another human for that matter, cannot be determined unambiguously using observational methods, but the conclusion that animals experience pain is often inferred on the basis of likely presence of phenomenal consciousness which is deduced from comparative brain physiology as well as physical and behavioural reactions.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Serotonin pathway</span> Neurons in the brain that communicate using serotonin

A serotonin pathway identifies aggregate projections from neurons which synthesize and communicate the monoamine neurotransmitter serotonin. These pathways are relevant to different psychiatric and neurological disorders.

Stuart C. Sealfon is an American neurologist who studies the mechanisms of both the therapeutic and adverse effects of drugs. He was an early adopter of the use of massively parallel qPCR and fluorescent in situ hybridization to characterize cell response state and his research accomplishments have included the identification of the primary structure of the gonadotropin-releasing hormone receptor, finding new signaling pathways activated by drugs for Parkinson's disease, elucidating the mechanism of action of hallucinogens and finding a new brain receptor complex implicated in schizophrenia as a novel target for antipsychotics.

Plant rights are rights to which certain plants may be entitled. Such issues are often raised in connection with discussions about human rights, animal rights, biocentrism, or sentientism.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Neurocriminology</span> Usage of neuroscience in criminology

Neurocriminology is an emerging sub-discipline of biocriminology and criminology that applies brain imaging techniques and principles from neuroscience to understand, predict, and prevent crime.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">LY-2183240</span> Chemical compound

LY-2183240 is a drug which acts both as a potent inhibitor of the reuptake of the endocannabinoid anandamide and as an inhibitor of fatty acid amide hydrolase (FAAH), the primary enzyme responsible for degrading anandamide. This leads to markedly elevated anandamide levels in the brain, and LY-2183240 has been shown to produce both analgesic and anxiolytic effects in animal models. While LY-2183240 is a potent inhibitor of FAAH, it has relatively poor selectivity and also inhibits several other enzyme side targets. Consequently, it was never developed for clinical use, though it remains widely used in research, and has also been sold as a designer drug.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Pain in amphibians</span> Ethical issue

Pain is an aversive sensation and feeling associated with actual, or potential, tissue damage. It is widely accepted by a broad spectrum of scientists and philosophers that non-human animals can perceive pain, including pain in amphibians.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Pain in cephalopods</span> Contentious issue

Pain in cephalopods is a contentious issue. Pain is a complex mental state, with a distinct perceptual quality but also associated with suffering, which is an emotional state. Because of this complexity, the presence of pain in non-human animals, or another human for that matter, cannot be determined unambiguously using observational methods, but the conclusion that animals experience pain is often inferred on the basis of likely presence of phenomenal consciousness which is deduced from comparative brain physiology as well as physical and behavioural reactions.

Joseph A. Bulbulia is a Professor of Psychology in the Faculty of Science at Victoria University of Wellington (2020-present). He was the Maclaurin Goodfellow Chair in the School of Humanities, Faculty of Arts at University of Auckland (2018-2020). He previously served as a Professor in the School of Art History, Classics and Religious Studies at Victoria University of Wellington. Bulbulia is regarded as one of the founders of the contemporary evolutionary religious studies. He is a past president of the International Association for the Cognitive Science of Religion and is currently co-editor of Religion, Brain & Behavior. Bulbulia is one of four on the Senior Management Team of the New Zealand Attitudes and Values Study, a national longitudinal study started in 2009 that has repeatedly sampled over 45,000 New Zealanders. He is an associate investigator for Pulotu, a database of 116 Pacific cultures purpose-built to investigate the evolutionary dynamics of religion. In 2016 Bulbulia won a Research Excellence Award at Victoria University.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Stefano Mancuso</span> Italian botanist

Stefano Mancuso is an Italian botanist, professor of the Agriculture, Food, Environment and Forestry department at his alma mater, the University of Florence. He is the director of the International Laboratory of Plant Neurobiology, steering committee member of the Society of Plant Signaling and Behavior, editor-in-chief of the Plant Signaling & Behavior journal and a member of the Accademia dei Georgofili.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">The Nervous Mechanism of Plants</span> Botany book by Jagadish Chandra Bose

The Nervous Mechanism of Plants, published in 1926, is a botany book by Sir Jagadish Chandra Bose which summarises his most recent findings in the area of plant physiology. Bose had previously investigated this topic in books such as Plant response as a means of physiological investigation from 1906, or The physiology of photosynthesis, published in 1924. In this book, he proposes that the response mechanisms of plants to stimuli are physiologically similar to those in animals.

Fan Wang is a neuroscientist and professor in the MIT Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences. She is an investigator at the McGovern Institute for Brain Research. Wang is known for her work identifying neural circuits underlying touch, pain, and anesthesia; and the development of a technique for capturing activated neuronal ensembles (CANE) to label and manipulate neurons activated by stimuli or behavioral paradigms.

References

  1. 1 2 Minorsky, Peter V. (2021). "American racism and the lost legacy of Sir Jagadis Chandra Bose, the father of plant neurobiology". Plant Signal Behav. 16 (1): 1818030. doi:10.1080/15592324.2020.1818030. PMC   7781790 . PMID   33275072.
  2. Brenner ED, Stahlberg R, Mancuso S, Vivanco J, Baluska F, Van Volkenburgh E. (2006). "Plant neurobiology: an integrated view of plant signaling". Trends Plant Sci. 11 (8): 413–419. doi:10.1016/j.tplants.2006.06.009. PMID   16843034.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  3. 1 2 Lee, Jonny (2023). "What is cognitive about 'plant cognition'?". Biology & Philosophy. 38 (18). doi:10.1007/s10539-023-09907-z.
  4. Trewavas, Anthony (2017). "The foundations of plant intelligence". Interface Focus. 7 (3): 20160098. doi:10.1098/rsfs.2016.0098. PMC   5413888 . PMID   28479977.
  5. 1 2 Garzón FC (July 2007). "The quest for cognition in plant neurobiology". Plant Signaling & Behavior. 2 (4): 208–11. Bibcode:2007PlSiB...2..208C. doi:10.4161/psb.2.4.4470. PMC   2634130 . PMID   19516990.
  6. 1 2 3 Minorsky, Peter V. (2024). "The "plant neurobiology" revolution". Plant Signaling & Behavior. 19 (1). doi:10.1080/15592324.2024.2345413. PMC   11085955 . PMID   38709727.
  7. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Alpi A, Amrhein N, Bertl A, Blatt MR, Blumwald E, Cervone F, et al. (April 2007). "Plant Neurobiology: No Brain, No Gain?". Trends in Plant Science. 12 (4): 135–6. Bibcode:2007TPS....12..135A. doi:10.1016/j.tplants.2007.03.002. PMID   17368081.
  8. 1 2 3 4 Taiz, Lincoln; Alkon, Daniel; Draguhn, Andreas; Murphy, Angus; Blatt, Michael; Hawes, Chris; Thiel, Gerhard; Robinson, David G. (2019). "Plants Neither Possess nor Require Consciousness". Trends in Plant Science. 24 (8): 677–687. doi:10.1016/j.tplants.2019.05.008. PMID   31279732.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  9. 1 2 3 Mallatt J, Blatt MR, Draguhn A, Robinson DG, Taiz L. (2020). "Debunking a myth: plant consciousness". Protoplasma. 258 (3): 459–476. doi:10.1007/s00709-020-01579-w. PMC   8052213 . PMID   33196907.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  10. Pigliucci, Massimo (2024). "Are Plants Conscious?". Skeptical Inquirer. 48 (5).
  11. Macdougal, D. T. (1895). "Irritability and Movement in Plants". Popular Science Monthly . 47: 225–234.
  12. Sha, Richard C. (2009). Perverse Romanticism: Aesthetics and Sexuality in Britain, 1750–1832. Johns Hopkins University. pp. 60-61. ISBN   978-0-8018-9041-3
  13. Whippo, Craig W; Hangarter, Roger P. (2009). "The "Sensational" Power of Movement in Plants: A Darwinian System for Studying the Evolution of Behavior". American Journal of Botany . 96 (12): 2115–2127. doi:10.3732/ajb.0900220. PMID   21622330.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  14. Heidelberger, Michael. (2004). Nature From Within: Gustav Theodor Fechner and his Psychophysical Worldview. University of Pittsburgh Press. p. 54. ISBN   0-8229-4210-0
  15. Mancuso S (September 2010). "Federico Delpino and the foundation of plant biology". Plant Signaling & Behavior. 5 (9): 1067–71. Bibcode:2010PlSiB...5.1067M. doi:10.4161/psb.5.9.12102. PMC   3115070 . PMID   21490417.
  16. Darwin, C. (1880). The Power of Movement in Plants. London: John Murray. Darwin Online  : "The course pursued by the radicle in penetrating the ground must be determined by the tip; hence it has acquired such diverse kinds of sensitiveness. It is hardly an exaggeration to say that the tip of the radicle thus endowed, and having the power of directing the movements of the adjoining parts, acts like the brain of one of the lower animals; the brain being seated within the anterior end of the body, receiving impressions from the sense-organs, and directing the several movements."
  17. "The Sagacity of Plants". The Month. 57 (264): 217–225. 1886.
  18. Galston, Arthur W; Slayman, Clifford L. (1979). "The Not-So-Secret Life of Plants" (PDF). American Scientist. 67 (3): 337–344. JSTOR   27849226.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  19. V. A Shepard cited in Alexander Volkov. (2012). Plant Electrophysiology: Methods and Cell Electrophysiology. Springer. p. 12. ISBN   978-3-642-29119-7 "Bose began by applying delicate instrumentation he had invented in his semiconductor research to deliver electrical stimuli and record electrical responses from various plant parts... He discovered that both living animal and plant tissues exhibited a diminution of sensitivity after continuous stimulation, recovery after rest, a 'staircase' or summation of electrical effects following mechanical stimulation, abolition of current flow after applying poisons and reduced sensitivity at low temperature."
  20. Geddes, Patrick. (1920). The Life and Work of Sir Jagadis C. Bose. Longmans, Green & Company. p. 120
  21. Geddes, Patrick. (1920). The Life and Work of Sir Jagadis C. Bose. Longmans, Green & Company. p. 146
  22. Kingsland, Sharon E; Taiz, Lincoln (2024). "Plant "intelligence" and the misuse of historical sources as evidence". Protoplasma. doi:10.1007/s00709-024-01988-1. PMID   39276228.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  23. Tandon PN (2019). "Jagdish Chandra Bose & plant neurobiology". Indian J Med Res. 149 (5): 593–599. doi: 10.4103/ijmr.IJMR_392_19 . PMC   6702694 . PMID   31417026.
  24. "Pain in Plants". The Florists' Review. 58 (4): 36. 1926.
  25. Folsom, Ed (1983). "The Mystical Ornithologist and the Iowa Tufthunter: Two Unpublished Whitman Letters and Some Identifications" (PDF). Walt Whitman Quarterly Review. 1: 18–29. doi: 10.13008/2153-3695.1003 .
  26. Smith, Arthur (1907). "Plant Consciousness". The Arena. 37 (211): 570–576.
  27. Smith, Arthur (1913). "The Brain Power of Plants". Gardener's Chronicle of America. 16 (5): 427–429.
  28. "Do Plants Think?". The Gardeners' Chronicle. 3 (39): 57. 1906.
  29. 1 2 Cvrcková F, Lipavská H, Zárský V. (2009). "Plant intelligence: Why, why not or where?". Plant Signal Behav. 4 (5). doi:10.4161/psb.4.5.8276. PMC   2676749 . PMID   19816094.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  30. Backster, Cleve. (2003). Primary Perception: Biocommunication with Plants, Living Foods, and Human Cells. White Rose Millennium Press. ISBN   978-0966435436
  31. 1 2 Kmetz, John M. (1978). "Plant Primary Perception: The Other Side of the Leaf" (PDF). Skeptical Inquirer. 2 (2): 57–61.
  32. Jensen, Derrick (1997). "The Plants Respond". The Sun. Archived from the original on July 23, 2024.
  33. 1 2 3 Horowitz KA, Lewis DC, Gasteiger EL (1975). "Plant "primary perception": electrophysiological unresponsiveness to brine shrimp killing". Science. 189 (4201): 478–480. doi:10.1126/science.189.4201.478.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  34. 1 2 Galston, Arthur W; Slayman, Clifford L. Plant Sensitivity and Sensation. In George Ogden Abell, Barry Singer. (1981). Science and the Paranormal: Probing the Existence of the Supernatural. Junction Books. pp. 40-55. ISBN   0-86245-037-3
  35. Schwebs, Ursula. (1973). Do Plants Have Feelings? Harpers . pp. 75-76
  36. Chedd, Graham. (1975). AAAS takes on Emotional Plants. New Scientist . 13 February. pp. 400-401
  37. Neher, Andrew. (2011). Paranormal and Transcendental Experience: A Psychological Examination. Dover Publications. pp. 155-156. ISBN   978-0486261676
  38. "Episode 61: Deadly Straw, Primary Perception". Annotated Mythbusters. September 6, 2006. Archived from the original on May 15, 2021. Retrieved February 28, 2010.
  39. Mescher, Mark C; Moraes, Consuelo M. De (2015). "Role of plant sensory perception in plant–animal interactions". Journal of Experimental Biology. 66 (2): 425–433. doi:10.1093/jxb/eru414.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  40. Hendlin, Yogi H. (2022). "Plant Philosophy and Interpretation" (PDF). Environmental Values. 31 (3): 253–276. doi:10.3197/096327121X16141642287755.
  41. 1 2 Trewavas A (July 2003). "Aspects of plant intelligence". Annals of Botany. 92 (1): 1–20. doi: 10.1093/aob/mcg101 . PMC   4243628 . PMID   12740212.
  42. Trewavas, Anthony (2002). "Plant intelligence: Mindless mastery". Nature. 415 (841). doi:10.1038/415841a.
  43. Nasser, Latif (2012). "The long, strange quest to detect plant consciousness". The Boston Globe. Archived from the original on November 30, 2024.
  44. Shinozaki K (2000). "Molecular responses to dehydration and low temperature: differences and cross-talk between two stress signaling pathways". Current Opinion in Plant Biology. 3 (3): 217–223. doi:10.1016/s1369-5266(00)00067-4. PMID   10837265.
  45. McCully ME (June 1999). "ROOTS IN SOIL: Unearthing the Complexities of Roots and Their Rhizospheres". Annual Review of Plant Physiology and Plant Molecular Biology. 50: 695–718. doi:10.1146/annurev.arplant.50.1.695. PMID   15012224.
  46. 1 2 Garzon P, Keijzer F (2011). "Plants: Adaptive behavior, root-brains, and minimal cognition" (PDF). Adaptive Behavior. 19 (3): 155–171. doi:10.1177/1059712311409446. S2CID   5060470.
  47. Topham AT, Taylor RE, Yan D, Nambara E, Johnston IG, Bassel GW (June 2017). "Temperature variability is integrated by a spatially embedded decision-making center to break dormancy in Arabidopsis seeds". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 114 (25): 6629–6634. Bibcode:2017PNAS..114.6629T. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1704745114 . PMC   5488954 . PMID   28584126.
  48. Trewavas 2014, p. 95-96.
  49. Trewavas 2014, p. 140.
  50. Crisp PA, Ganguly D, Eichten SR, Borevitz JO, Pogson BJ (February 2016). "Reconsidering plant memory: Intersections between stress recovery, RNA turnover, and epigenetics". Science Advances. 2 (2): e1501340. Bibcode:2016SciA....2E1340C. doi:10.1126/sciadv.1501340. PMC   4788475 . PMID   26989783.
  51. Affifi R (2018). "Deweyan Psychology in Plant Intelligence Research: Transforming Stimulus and Response". In Baluska F, Gagliano M, Witzany G (eds.). Memory and Learning in Plants. Signaling and Communication in Plants. Cham.: Springer. pp. 17–33. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-75596-0_2. ISBN   978-3-319-75595-3.
  52. Yokawa, K; Kagenishi, T; Pavlovič, A; Gall, S; Weiland, M; Mancuso, S; Baluška, F (11 December 2017). "Anaesthetics stop diverse plant organ movements, affect endocytic vesicle recycling and ROS homeostasis, and block action potentials in Venus flytraps". Annals of Botany. 122 (5): 747–756. doi:10.1093/aob/mcx155. PMC   6215046 . PMID   29236942.
  53. "Plants: Are they conscious?". BBC Science Focus Magazine. 5 February 2021. Retrieved 6 February 2021.
  54. Raja, Vicente; Silva, Paula L.; Holghoomi, Roghaieh; Calvo, Paco (December 2020). "The dynamics of plant nutation". Scientific Reports. 10 (1): 19465. Bibcode:2020NatSR..1019465R. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-76588-z . PMC   7655864 . PMID   33173160.
  55. 1 2 Reber, Arthur S; Baluška, František (2021). "Cognition in some surprising places". Biochemical and Biophysical Research Communications. 564: 150–157. doi:10.1016/j.bbrc.2020.08.115.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  56. Mallatt J, Taiz L, Draguhn A, Blatt MR, Robinson DG. (2021). "Integrated information theory does not make plant consciousness more convincing". Biochem Biophys Res Commun. 564: 166–169. doi:10.1016/j.bbrc.2021.01.022. PMID   33485631.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  57. Hansen, Mads Jørgensen (2024). "A critical review of plant sentience: moving beyond traditional approaches". Biology & Philosophy. 39 (13). doi: 10.1007/s10539-024-09953-1 .
  58. 1 2 3 Kingsland, Sharon Elizabeth (2018). "Facts or Fairy Tales? Peter Wohlleben and the Hidden Life of Trees". Bulletin of the Ecological Society of America. 99 (4): e01443. doi: 10.1002/bes2.1443 .
  59. Key, Brian (2016). ""Cellular basis of consciousness": Not just radical but wrong". Animal Sentience. 11 (5): 1–2. doi: 10.51291/2377-7478.1163 .
  60. Robinson DG, Mallatt J, Peer WA, Sourjik V, Taiz L. (2024). "Cell consciousness: a dissenting opinion: The cellular basis of consciousness theory lacks empirical evidence for its claims that all cells have consciousness". EMBO Reports. 25 (5): 2162–2167. doi:10.1038/s44319-024-00127-4. PMC   11094104 . PMID   38548972.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  61. Draguhn A, Mallatt JM, Robinson DG. (2021). "Anesthetics and plants: no pain, no brain, and therefore no consciousness". Protoplasma. 258 (2): 239–248. doi:10.1007/s00709-020-01550-9. PMC   7907021 . PMID   32880005.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  62. Robinson, David G; Draguhn, Andreas; Taiz, Lincoln (2020). "Plant "intelligence" changes nothing". EMBO Reports. 21 (5): e50395. doi:10.15252/embr.202050395. PMC   7202214 . PMID   32301219.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  63. Hamilton, Adam; McBrayer, Justin (2020). "Do Plants Feel Pain?". Disputatio. 12 (56): 71–98. doi: 10.2478/disp-2020-0003 .{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)

Further reading

Plant intelligence and neurobiology

Criticism