Dyslexia

Last updated

Dyslexia
Other namesReading disorder, alexia
Dyslexia handwriting Greek.jpg
Dyslexic handwriting in Greek
Specialty Neurology, pediatrics
Symptoms Trouble reading [1]
Usual onsetSchool age [2]
Types Surface dyslexia
Causes Genetic and environmental factors [2]
Risk factors Family history, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder [3]
Diagnostic method Series memory, spelling, vision, and reading test [4]
Differential diagnosis Hearing or vision problems, insufficient teaching [2]
TreatmentAdjusting teaching methods [1]
Frequency3–7% [2] [5]

Dyslexia, also known as reading disorder, is characterized by trouble with reading despite normal intelligence. [1] [6] Different people are affected to different degrees. [3] Problems may include difficulties in spelling words, reading quickly, writing words, "sounding out" words in the head, pronouncing words when reading aloud and understanding what one reads. [3] [7] Often these difficulties are first noticed at school. [2] When someone who previously could read loses their ability, it is known as "alexia". [3] The difficulties are involuntary and people with this disorder have a normal desire to learn. [3] People with dyslexia have higher rates of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), developmental language disorders, and difficulties with numbers. [2] [8] [9]

Contents

Dyslexia is believed to be caused by the interaction of genetic and environmental factors. [2] Some cases run in families. [3] Dyslexia that develops due to a traumatic brain injury, stroke, or dementia is called "acquired dyslexia". [1] The underlying mechanisms of dyslexia are problems within the brain's language processing. [3] Dyslexia is diagnosed through a series of tests of memory, vision, spelling, and reading skills. [4] Dyslexia is separate from reading difficulties caused by hearing or vision problems or by insufficient teaching or opportunity to learn. [2]

Treatment involves adjusting teaching methods to meet the person's needs. [1] While not curing the underlying problem, it may decrease the degree or impact of symptoms. [10] Treatments targeting vision are not effective. [11] Dyslexia is the most common learning disability and occurs in all areas of the world. [12] It affects 3–7% of the population, [2] [5] however, up to 20% of the general population may have some degree of symptoms. [13] While dyslexia is more often diagnosed in men, [2] it has been suggested that it affects men and women equally. [12] Some believe that dyslexia should be best considered as a different way of learning, with both benefits and downsides. [14] [15]

Classification

Dyslexia is divided into developmental and acquired forms. This article is primarily about developmental dyslexia, i.e., dyslexia that begins in early childhood. [16] Acquired dyslexia occurs subsequent to neurological insult, such as traumatic brain injury or stroke. People with acquired dyslexia exhibit some of the signs or symptoms of the developmental disorder, but requiring different assessment strategies and treatment approaches. [17]

Signs and symptoms

In early childhood, symptoms that correlate with a later diagnosis of dyslexia include delayed onset of speech and a lack of phonological awareness. [11] A common myth closely associates dyslexia with mirror writing and reading letters or words backwards. [18] These behaviors are seen in many children as they learn to read and write, and are not considered to be defining characteristics of dyslexia. [11]

School-age children with dyslexia may exhibit signs of difficulty in identifying or generating rhyming words, or counting the number of syllables in words–both of which depend on phonological awareness. [19] They may also show difficulty in segmenting words into individual sounds or may blend sounds when producing words, indicating reduced phonemic awareness. [20] Difficulties with word retrieval or naming things is also associated with dyslexia. [21] :647 People with dyslexia are commonly poor spellers, a feature sometimes called dysorthographia or dysgraphia, which depends on orthographic coding. [11]

Problems persist into adolescence and adulthood and may include difficulties with summarizing stories, memorization, reading aloud, or learning foreign languages. Adults with dyslexia can often read with good comprehension, though they tend to read more slowly than others without a learning difficulty and perform worse in spelling tests or when reading nonsense words–a measure of phonological awareness. [22]

Associated conditions

Dyslexia often co-occurs with other learning disorders, but the reasons for this comorbidity have not been clearly identified. [23] These associated disabilities include:

Causes

Inferior parietal lobule - superior view animation Inferior parietal lobule - superior view animation.gif
Inferior parietal lobule – superior view animation

Researchers have been trying to find the neurobiological basis of dyslexia since the condition was first identified in 1881. [34] [35] For example, some have tried to associate the common problem among people with dyslexia of not being able to see letters clearly to abnormal development of their visual nerve cells. [36]

Neuroanatomy

Neuroimaging techniques, such as functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and positron emission tomography (PET), have shown a correlation between both functional and structural differences in the brains of children with reading difficulties. [37] Some people with dyslexia show less electrical activation in parts of the left hemisphere of the brain involved with reading, such as the inferior frontal gyrus, inferior parietal lobule, and the middle and ventral temporal cortex. [31] Over the past decade, brain activation studies using PET to study language have produced a breakthrough in the understanding of the neural basis of language. Neural bases for the visual lexicon and for auditory verbal short-term memory components have been proposed, [38] with some implication that the observed neural manifestation of developmental dyslexia is task-specific (i.e., functional rather than structural). fMRIs of people with dyslexia indicate an interactive role of the cerebellum and cerebral cortex as well as other brain structures in reading. [39] [40]

The cerebellar theory of dyslexia proposes that impairment of cerebellum-controlled muscle movement affects the formation of words by the tongue and facial muscles, resulting in the fluency problems that some people with dyslexia experience. The cerebellum is also involved in the automatization of some tasks, such as reading. [41] The fact that some children with dyslexia have motor task and balance impairments could be consistent with a cerebellar role in their reading difficulties. However, the cerebellar theory has not been supported by controlled research studies. [42]

Genetics

Research into potential genetic causes of dyslexia has its roots in post-autopsy examination of the brains of people with dyslexia. [36] Observed anatomical differences in the language centers of such brains include microscopic cortical malformations known as ectopias, and more rarely, vascular micro-malformations, and microgyrus—a smaller than usual size for the gyrus. [43] The previously cited studies and others [44] suggest that abnormal cortical development, presumed to occur before or during the sixth month of fetal brain development, may have caused the abnormalities. Abnormal cell formations in people with dyslexia have also been reported in non-language cerebral and subcortical brain structures. [45] Several genes have been associated with dyslexia, including DCDC2 [46] and KIAA0319 [47] on chromosome 6, and DYX1C1 on chromosome 15. [48]

Gene–environment interaction

The contribution of gene–environment interaction to reading disability has been intensely studied using twin studies, which estimate the proportion of variance associated with a person's environment and the proportion associated with their genes. Both environmental and genetic factors appear to contribute to reading development. Studies examining the influence of environmental factors such as parental education [49] and teaching quality [50] have determined that genetics have greater influence in supportive, rather than less optimal, environments. [51] However, more optimal conditions may just allow those genetic risk factors to account for more of the variance in outcome because the environmental risk factors have been minimized. [51]

As environment plays a large role in learning and memory, it is likely that epigenetic modifications play an important role in reading ability. Measures of gene expression, histone modifications, and methylation in the human periphery are used to study epigenetic processes; however, all of these have limitations in the extrapolation of results for application to the human brain. [52] [53]

Language

The orthographic complexity of a language directly affects how difficult it is to learn to read it. [54] :266 English and French have comparatively "deep" phonemic orthographies within the Latin alphabet writing system, with complex structures employing spelling patterns on several levels: letter-sound correspondence, syllables, and morphemes. [55] :421 Languages such as Spanish, Italian and Finnish have mostly alphabetic orthographies, which primarily employ letter-sound correspondence—so-called "shallow" orthographies—which makes them easier to learn for people with dyslexia. [54] :266 Logographic writing systems, such as Chinese characters, have extensive symbol use; and these also pose problems for dyslexic learners. [56]

Pathophysiology

Corpus callosum view, front part at top of image Gray733.png
Corpus callosum view, front part at top of image

Most people who are right-hand dominant have the left hemisphere of their brain specialize more in language processing. In terms of the mechanism of dyslexia, fMRI studies suggest that this specialization may be less pronounced or even absent in cases with dyslexia. Additionally, anatomical differences in the corpus callosum, the bundle of nerve fibers that connects the left and right hemispheres, have been linked to dyslexia via different studies. [57]

Data via diffusion tensor MRI indicate changes in connectivity or in gray matter density in areas related to reading/language. Finally, the left inferior frontal gyrus has shown differences in phonological processing in people with dyslexia. [57] Neurophysiological and imaging procedures are being used to ascertain phenotypic characteristics in people with dyslexia thus identifying the effects of certain genes. [58]

Dual route theory

The dual-route theory of reading aloud was first described in the early 1970s. [59] This theory suggests that two separate mental mechanisms, or cognitive routes, are involved in reading aloud. [60] One mechanism is the lexical route, which is the process whereby skilled readers can recognize known words by sight alone, through a "dictionary" lookup procedure. [61] The other mechanism is the nonlexical or sublexical route, which is the process whereby the reader can "sound out" a written word. [61] [62] This is done by identifying the word's constituent parts (letters, phonemes, graphemes) and applying knowledge of how these parts are associated with each other, for example, how a string of neighboring letters sound together. [59] The dual-route system could explain the different rates of dyslexia occurrence between different languages (e.g., the consistency of phonological rules in the Spanish language could account for the fact that Spanish-speaking children show a higher level of performance in non-word reading, when compared to English-speakers). [54] [63]

Diagnosis

Dyslexia is a heterogeneous, dimensional learning disorder that impairs accurate and fluent word reading and spelling. [64] [65] Typical—but not universal—features include difficulties with phonological awareness; inefficient and often inaccurate processing of sounds in oral language (phonological processing); and verbal working memory deficits. [66] [67]

Dyslexia is a neurodevelopmental disorder, subcategorized in diagnostic guides as a learning disorder with impairment in reading (ICD-11 prefixes "developmental" to "learning disorder"; DSM-5 uses "specific"). [68] [69] [70] Dyslexia is not a problem with intelligence. Emotional problems often arise secondary to learning difficulties. [71] The National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke describes dyslexia as "difficulty with phonological processing (the manipulation of sounds), spelling, and/or rapid visual-verbal responding". [1]

The British Dyslexia Association defines dyslexia as "a learning difficulty that primarily affects the skills involved in accurate and fluent word reading and spelling" and is characterized by "difficulties in phonological awareness, verbal memory and verbal processing speed". [72] Phonological awareness enables one to identify, discriminate, remember (working memory), and mentally manipulate the sound structures of language—phonemes, onsite-rime segments, syllables, and words. [73] [74]

Assessment

Assessment tests

There is a wide range of tests that are used in clinical and educational settings to evaluate the possibility that a person might have dyslexia. [81] If initial testing suggests that a person might have dyslexia, such tests are often followed up with a full diagnostic assessment to determine the extent and nature of the disorder. [82] Some tests can be administered by a teacher or computer; others require specialized training and are given by psychologists. [83] Some test results indicate how to carry out teaching strategies. [83] [84] Because a variety of different cognitive, behavioral, emotional, and environmental factors all could contribute to difficultly learning to read, a comprehensive evaluation should consider these different possibilities. These tests and observations can include: [85]

  • General measures of cognitive ability, such as the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Cognitive Abilities, or Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales. Low general cognitive ability would make reading more difficult. Cognitive ability measures also often try to measure different cognitive processes, such as verbal ability, nonverbal and spatial reasoning, working memory, and processing speed. There are different versions of these tests for different age groups. Almost all of these require additional training to give and score correctly, and are done by psychologists. According to Mather and Schneider (2015), a confirmatory profile and/or pattern of scores on cognitive tests confirming or ruling-out reading disorder has not yet been identified. [86]
  • Screening or evaluation for mental health conditions: Parents and teachers can complete rating scales or behavior checklists to gather information about emotional and behavioral functioning for younger people. Many checklists have similar versions for parents, teachers, and younger people old enough to read reasonably well (often 11 years and older) to complete. Examples include the Behavioral Assessment System for Children, and the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire. All of these have nationally representative norms, making it possible to compare the level of symptoms to what would be typical for the younger person's age and biological sex. Other checklists link more specifically to psychiatric diagnoses, such as the Vanderbilt ADHD Rating Scales or the Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders (SCARED). Screening uses brief tools that are designed to catch cases with a disorder, but they often get false positive scores for people who do not have the disorder. Screeners should be followed up by a more accurate test or diagnostic interview as a result. Depressive disorders and anxiety disorders are two-three times higher in people with dyslexia, and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder is more common, as well. [87] [88] [89] [90]
  • Review of academic achievement and skills: Average spelling/reading ability for a dyslexic is a percentage ranking <16, well below normal. In addition to reviewing grades and teacher notes, standardized test results are helpful in evaluating progress. These include group administered tests, such as the Iowa Tests of Educational Development, that a teacher may give to a group or whole classroom of younger people at the same time. They also could include individually administered tests of achievement, such as the Wide Range Achievement Test, or the Woodcock-Johnson (which also includes a set of achievement tests). The individually administered tests again require more specialized training. [91] [92] [93]

Screening

Screening procedures seek to identify children who show signs of possible dyslexia. In the preschool years, a family history of dyslexia, particularly in biological parents and siblings, predicts an eventual dyslexia diagnosis better than any test. [94] In primary school (ages 5–7), the ideal screening procedure consist of training primary school teachers to carefully observe and record their pupils' progress through the phonics curriculum, and thereby identify children progressing slowly. [95] [96] When teachers identify such students they can supplement their observations with screening tests such as the Phonics screening check [97] used by United Kingdom schools during Year one.

In the medical setting, child and adolescent psychiatrist M. S. Thambirajah emphasizes that "[g]iven the high prevalence of developmental disorders in school-aged children, all children seen in clinics should be systematically screened for developmental disorders irrespective of the presenting problem/s." Thambirajah recommends screening for developmental disorders, including dyslexia, by conducting a brief developmental history, a preliminary psychosocial developmental examination, and obtaining a school report regarding academic and social functioning. [98]

Management

Through the use of compensation strategies, therapy and educational support, individuals with dyslexia can learn to read and write. [99] There are techniques and technical aids that help to manage or conceal symptoms of the disorder. [100] Reducing stress and anxiety can sometimes improve written comprehension. [101] For dyslexia intervention with alphabet-writing systems, the fundamental aim is to increase a child's awareness of correspondences between graphemes (letters) and phonemes (sounds), and to relate these to reading and spelling by teaching how sounds blend into words. Reinforced collateral training focused on reading and spelling may yield longer-lasting gains than oral phonological training alone. [102] Early intervention can be successful in reducing reading failure. [103]

Research does not suggest that specially-tailored fonts (such as Dyslexie and OpenDyslexic) help with reading. [104] Children with dyslexia read text set in a regular font such as Times New Roman and Arial just as quickly, and they show a preference for regular fonts over specially-tailored fonts. [104] Some research has pointed to increased letter-spacing being beneficial. [104]

There is currently no evidence showing that music education significantly improves the reading skills of adolescents with dyslexia. [105]

Prognosis

Dyslexic children require special instruction for word analysis and spelling from an early age. [106] The prognosis, generally speaking, is positive for individuals who are identified in childhood and receive support from friends and family. [1] The New York educational system (NYED) indicates "a daily uninterrupted 90-minute block of instruction in reading", furthermore "instruction in phonemic awareness, phonics, vocabulary development, reading fluency" so as to improve the individual's reading ability. [107]

Epidemiology

The percentage of people with dyslexia is unknown, but it has been estimated to be as low as 5% and as high as 17% of the population. [108] While it is diagnosed more often in males, [2] some believe that it affects males and females equally.

There are different definitions of dyslexia used throughout the world, but despite significant differences in writing systems, dyslexia occurs in different populations. [109] Dyslexia is not limited to difficulty in converting letters to sounds, and Chinese people with dyslexia may have difficulty converting Chinese characters into their meanings. [110] [111] The Chinese vocabulary uses logographic, monographic, non-alphabet writing where one character can represent an individual phoneme. [112]

The phonological-processing hypothesis attempts to explain why dyslexia occurs in a wide variety of languages. Furthermore, the relationship between phonological capacity and reading appears to be influenced by orthography. [113]

History

Dyslexia was clinically described by Oswald Berkhan in 1881, [34] but the term dyslexia was coined in 1883 by Rudolf Berlin, an ophthalmologist in Stuttgart. [114] [115] [116] He used the term to refer to the case of a young boy who had severe difficulty learning to read and write, despite showing typical intelligence and physical abilities in all other respects. [117] In 1896, W. Pringle Morgan, a British physician from Seaford, East Sussex, published a description of a reading-specific learning disorder in a report to the British Medical Journal titled "Congenital Word Blindness". [118] The distinction between phonological versus surface types of dyslexia is only descriptive, and without any etiological assumption as to the underlying brain mechanisms. However, studies have alluded to potential differences due to variation in performance. [119]

Society and culture

As is the case with any disorder, society often makes an assessment based on incomplete information. Before the 1980s, dyslexia was thought to be a consequence of education, rather than a neurological disability. As a result, society often misjudges those with the disorder. [101] There is also sometimes a workplace stigma and negative attitude towards those with dyslexia. [120] If the instructors of a person with dyslexia lack the necessary training to support a child with the condition, there is often a negative effect on the student's learning participation. [121]

Since at least the 1960s in the UK, the children diagnosed with developmental dyslexia have consistently been from privileged families. [122] Although half of prisoners in the UK have significant reading difficulties, very few have ever been evaluated for dyslexia. [122] Access to some special educational resources and funding is contingent upon having a diagnosis of dyslexia. [122] As a result, when Staffordshire and Warwickshire proposed in 2018 to teach reading to all children with reading difficulties, using techniques proven to be successful for most children with a diagnosis of dyslexia, without first requiring the families to obtain an official diagnosis, dyslexia advocates and parents of children with dyslexia were fearful that they were losing a privileged status. [122]

Research

Writing Systems Writing Systems Template Image.svg
Writing Systems

Most dyslexia research relates to alphabetic writing systems, and especially to European languages. [123] However, substantial research is also available regarding people with dyslexia who speak Arabic, Chinese, Hebrew, or other languages. [124] The outward expression of individuals with reading disability and regular poor readers is the same in some respects. [125]

See also

Related Research Articles

Dyscalculia is a disability resulting in difficulty learning or comprehending arithmetic, such as difficulty in understanding numbers, learning how to manipulate numbers, performing mathematical calculations and learning facts in mathematics. It is sometimes informally known as "math dyslexia", though this can be misleading as dyslexia is a different condition from dyscalculia.

Dysgraphia is a deficiency in the ability to write, primarily handwriting, but also coherence. Dysgraphia is a specific learning disability as well as a transcription disability, meaning that it is a writing disorder associated with impaired handwriting, orthographic coding, and finger sequencing. It often overlaps with other learning disabilities such as speech impairment, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, or developmental coordination disorder. In the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV), dysgraphia is characterized as a learning disability in the category of written expression when one's writing skills are below those expected given a person's age measured through intelligence and age-appropriate education. The DSM is not clear in whether or not writing refers only to the motor skills involved in writing, or if it also includes orthographic skills and spelling.

Reading for special needs has become an area of interest as the understanding of reading has improved. Teaching children with special needs how to read was not historically pursued due to perspectives of a Reading Readiness model. This model assumes that a reader must learn to read in a hierarchical manner such that one skill must be mastered before learning the next skill. This approach often led to teaching sub-skills of reading in a decontextualized manner. This style of teaching made it difficult for children to master these early skills, and as a result, did not advance to more advanced literacy instruction and often continued to receive age-inappropriate instruction.

Specific language impairment (SLI) is diagnosed when a child's language does not develop normally and the difficulties cannot be accounted for by generally slow development, physical abnormality of the speech apparatus, autism spectrum disorder, apraxia, acquired brain damage or hearing loss. Twin studies have shown that it is under genetic influence. Although language impairment can result from a single-gene mutation, this is unusual. More commonly SLI results from the combined influence of multiple genetic variants, each of which is found in the general population, as well as environmental influences.

A reading disability is a condition in which a sufferer displays difficulty reading. Examples of reading disabilities include: developmental dyslexia, alexia, and hyperlexia.

Developmental coordination disorder (DCD), also known as developmental motor coordination disorder, developmental dyspraxia or simply dyspraxia, is a chronic neurological disorder beginning in childhood. It is also known to affect planning of movements and co-ordination as a result of brain messages not being accurately transmitted to the body. Impairments in skilled motor movements per a child's chronological age interfere with activities of daily living. A diagnosis of DCD is then reached only in the absence of other neurological impairments such as cerebral palsy, multiple sclerosis, or Parkinson's disease.

Learning disability Range of neurodevelopmental conditions

Learning disability, learning disorder, or learning difficulty is a condition in the brain that causes difficulties comprehending or processing information and can be caused by several different factors. Given the "difficulty learning in a typical manner", this does not exclude the ability to learn in a different manner. Therefore, some people can be more accurately described as having a "learning difference", thus avoiding any misconception of being disabled with a lack of ability to learn and possible negative stereotyping. In the United Kingdom, the term "learning disability" generally refers to an intellectual disability, while difficulties such as dyslexia and dyspraxia are usually referred to as "learning difficulties".

The phonological deficit hypothesis is a prevalent cognitive-level explanation for the cause of reading difficulties and dyslexia. It stems from evidence that individuals with dyslexia tend to do poorly on tests which measure their ability to decode nonsense words using conventional phonetic rules, and that there is a high correlation between difficulties in connecting the sounds of language to letters and reading delays or failure in children.

Auditory processing disorder (APD), rarely known as King-Kopetzky syndrome or auditory disability with normal hearing (ADN), is an umbrella term for a variety of disorders that affect the way the brain processes auditory information. Individuals with APD usually have normal structure and function of the outer, middle, and inner ear. However, they cannot process the information they hear in the same way as others do, which leads to difficulties in recognizing and interpreting sounds, especially the sounds composing speech. It is thought that these difficulties arise from dysfunction in the central nervous system.

Management of dyslexia

Management of dyslexia depends on a multiple of variables; there is no one specific strategy or set of strategies which will work for all who have dyslexia.

Deep dyslexia is a form of dyslexia that disrupts reading processes. Deep dyslexia may occur as a result of a head injury, stroke, disease, or operation. This injury results in the occurrence of semantic errors during reading and the impairment of nonword reading.

Language-based learning disabilities or LBLD are "heterogeneous" neurological differences that can affect skills such as listening, reasoning, speaking, reading, writing, and math calculations. It is also associated with movement, coordination, and direct attention. LBLD is not usually identified until the child reaches school age. Most people with this disability find it hard to communicate, to express ideas efficiently and what they say may be ambiguous and hard to understand It is a neurological difference. It is often hereditary, and is frequently associated to specific language problems.

The history of dyslexia research spans from the late 1800s to the present.

Dyslexia is a neurological symptom wherein an individual experiences difficulty reading. The neurological nature and underlying causes of dyslexia are an active area of research, and the distinction of dyslexia as a condition is a topic of some controversy.

Dyslexia is a complex, lifelong disorder involving difficulty in learning to read or interpret words, letters and other symbols. Dyslexia does not affect general intelligence, but is often co-diagnosed with ADHD. There are at least three sub-types of dyslexia that have been recognized by researchers: orthographic, or surface dyslexia, phonological dyslexia and mixed dyslexia where individuals exhibit symptoms of both orthographic and phonological dyslexia. Studies have shown that dyslexia is genetic and can be passed down through families, but it is important to note that, although a genetic disorder, there is no specific locus in the brain for reading and writing. The human brain does have language centers, but written language is a cultural artifact, and a very complex one requiring brain regions designed to recognize and interpret written symbols as representations of language in rapid synchronization. The complexity of the system and the lack of genetic predisposition for it is one possible explanation for the difficulty in acquiring and understanding written language.

Dyslexia is a disorder characterized by problems with the visual notation of speech, which in most languages of European origin are problems with alphabet writing systems which have a phonetic construction.

Cognitive musicology is a branch of cognitive science concerned with computationally modeling musical knowledge with the goal of understanding both music and cognition.

Educational neuroscience is an emerging scientific field that brings together researchers in cognitive neuroscience, developmental cognitive neuroscience, educational psychology, educational technology, education theory and other related disciplines to explore the interactions between biological processes and education. Researchers in educational neuroscience investigate the neural mechanisms of reading, numerical cognition, attention and their attendant difficulties including dyslexia, dyscalculia and ADHD as they relate to education. Researchers in this area may link basic findings in cognitive neuroscience with educational technology to help in curriculum implementation for mathematics education and reading education. The aim of educational neuroscience is to generate basic and applied research that will provide a new transdisciplinary account of learning and teaching, which is capable of informing education. A major goal of educational neuroscience is to bridge the gap between the two fields through a direct dialogue between researchers and educators, avoiding the "middlemen of the brain-based learning industry". These middlemen have a vested commercial interest in the selling of "neuromyths" and their supposed remedies.

The Planning, Attention-Arousal, Simultaneous and Successive (PASS) theory of intelligence, first proposed in 1975, and later elaborated by Das, Naglieri & Kirby (1994) and Das, Kar & Parrila, (1996) challenges g-theory on the grounds that the brain is made up of interdependent, but separate, functional systems. Neuroimaging studies and clinical studies of individuals with brain lesions make it clear that the brain is modularized; for example, damage to a very specific area of the left temporal lobe will impair the production of spoken and written language. Damage to an adjacent area will have the opposite impact, preserving the individual's ability to produce, but not understand speech and text.

Usha Claire Goswami is a researcher and professor of Cognitive Developmental Neuroscience at the University of Cambridge, a Fellow of St. John's College, Cambridge, and the director of the Centre for Neuroscience in Education, Downing Site. She obtained her Ph.D. in developmental psychology from the University of Oxford before becoming a professor of cognitive developmental psychology at the University College London. Goswami's work is primarily in educational neuroscience with major focuses on reading development and developmental dyslexia.

References

  1. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 "Dyslexia Information Page". National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke. 2 November 2018.
  2. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 Peterson, Robin L.; Pennington, Bruce F. (May 2012). "Developmental dyslexia". Lancet. 379 (9830): 1997–2007. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(12)60198-6. PMC   3465717 . PMID   22513218.
  3. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 "What are reading disorders?". National Institutes of Health. 1 December 2016.
  4. 1 2 "How are reading disorders diagnosed?". National Institutes of Health. Archived from the original on 2 April 2015. Retrieved 15 March 2015.
  5. 1 2 3 Kooij, J. J. Sandra (2013). Adult ADHD diagnostic assessment and treatment (3rd ed.). London: Springer. p. 83. ISBN   9781447141389. Archived from the original on 30 April 2016.
  6. Siegel LS (November 2006). "Perspectives on dyslexia". Paediatrics & Child Health. 11 (9): 581–7. doi:10.1093/pch/11.9.581. PMC   2528651 . PMID   19030329.
  7. "What are the symptoms of reading disorders?". National Institutes of Health. 1 December 2016.
  8. Sexton, Chris C.; Gelhorn, Heather L.; Bell, Jill A.; Classi, Peter M. (November 2012). "The Co-occurrence of Reading Disorder and ADHD: Epidemiology, Treatment, Psychosocial Impact, and Economic Burden". Journal of Learning Disabilities. 45 (6): 538–564. doi:10.1177/0022219411407772. PMID   21757683. S2CID   385238.
  9. Bishop, DV; Snowling, MJ; Thompson, PA; Greenhalgh, T; CATALISE, consortium. (2016). "CATALISE: A Multinational and Multidisciplinary Delphi Consensus Study. Identifying Language Impairments in Children". PLOS ONE. 11 (7): e0158753. Bibcode:2016PLoSO..1158753B. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0158753. PMC   4938414 . PMID   27392128. Language impairment frequently co-occurs with other neurodevelopmental difficulties, including... reading difficulties
  10. "What are common treatments for reading disorders?". National Institutes of Health. Archived from the original on 2 April 2015. Retrieved 15 March 2015.
  11. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Handler, SM; Fierson, WM; Section on, Ophthalmology; Council on Children with, Disabilities; American Academy of, Ophthalmology; American Association for Pediatric Ophthalmology and, Strabismus; American Association of Certified, Orthoptists (March 2011). "Learning disabilities, dyslexia, and vision". Pediatrics. 127 (3): e818–56. doi: 10.1542/peds.2010-3670 . PMID   21357342.
  12. 1 2 Umphred, Darcy Ann; Lazaro, Rolando T.; Roller, Margaret; Burton, Gordon (2013). Neurological Rehabilitation. Elsevier Health Sciences. p. 383. ISBN   978-0-323-26649-9. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017.
  13. "How many people are affected by/at risk for reading disorders?". National Institutes of Health. Archived from the original on 2 April 2015. Retrieved 15 March 2015.
  14. Venton, Danielle (September 2011). "The Unappreciated Benefits of Dyslexia". Wired. Archived from the original on 5 August 2016. Retrieved 10 August 2016.
  15. Mathew, Schneps (August 2014). "The Advantages of Dyslexia". ScientificAmerican.com. Scientific American. Archived from the original on 4 August 2016. Retrieved 10 August 2016.
  16. Oxford English Dictionary. 3rd ed. "dyslexia, n. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 2012 ("a learning disability specifically affecting the attainment of literacy, with difficulty esp. in word recognition, spelling, and the conversion of letters to sounds, occurring in a child with otherwise normal development, and now usually regarded as a neurodevelopmental disorder with a genetic component.")
  17. Woollams, Anna M. (19 January 2014). "Connectionist neuropsychology: uncovering ultimate causes of acquired dyslexia". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 369 (1634): 20120398. doi:10.1098/rstb.2012.0398. ISSN   0962-8436. PMC   3866427 . PMID   24324241.
  18. Lilienfeld, Scott O.; Lynn, Steven Jay; Ruscio, John; Beyerstein, Barry L. (15 September 2011). 50 Great Myths of Popular Psychology: Shattering Widespread Misconceptions about Human Behavior. John Wiley & Sons. pp. 88–89. ISBN   978-1-4443-6074-5. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017. Retrieved 19 May 2016.
  19. "Dyslexia and Related Disorders" (PDF). Alabama Dyslexia Association. International Dyslexia Association. January 2003. Archived (PDF) from the original on 4 March 2016. Retrieved 29 April 2015.
  20. Peer, Lindsay; Reid, Gavin (2014). Multilingualism, Literacy and Dyslexia. Routledge. p. 219. ISBN   978-1-136-60899-5. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017.
  21. Shaywitz, Sally E.; Shaywitz, Bennett A. (2013). "Chapter 34 Making a Hidden Disability Visible: What Has Been Learned from Neurobiological Studies of Dyslexia". In Swanson, H. Lee; Harris, Karen R.; Graham, Steve (eds.). Handbook of Learning Disabilities (2 ed.). Guilford Press. ISBN   978-1-4625-0856-3. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017.
  22. Jarrad, Lum (October 2013). "Procedural learning is impaired in dyslexia: evidence from a meta-analysis of serial reaction time studies". Research of Developmental Disabilities. 34 (10): 3460–76. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2013.07.017. PMC   3784964 . PMID   23920029.
  23. Nicolson, R. I.; Fawcett, A. J. (September 2009). "Dyslexia, dysgraphia, procedural learning and the cerebellum". Cortex. 47 (1): 117–27. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2009.08.016. PMID   19818437. S2CID   32228208.
  24. 1 2 Reynolds, Cecil R.; Fletcher-Janzen, Elaine (2 January 2007). Encyclopedia of Special Education. John Wiley & Sons. p.  771. ISBN   978-0-471-67798-7.
  25. "Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder". NIH: National Institute of Mental Health. March 2016. Archived from the original on 23 July 2016. Retrieved 26 July 2016.
  26. Comer, Ronald (2011). Psychology Around Us. RR Donnelley. p. 1. ISBN   978-0-471-38519-6. Archived from the original on 4 June 2016.
  27. Germanò, E; Gagliano, A; Curatolo, P (2010). "Comorbidity of ADHD and Dyslexia" (PDF). Developmental Neuropsychology. 35 (5): 475–493. doi:10.1080/87565641.2010.494748. PMID   20721770. S2CID   42046958.
  28. Fatemi, S. Hossein; Sartorius, Norman; Clayton, Paula J. (2008). The Medical Basis of Psychiatry (3 ed.). Springer Science & Business Media. p. 308. ISBN   978-1-59745-252-6. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017.
  29. Capellini, Simone Aparecida (2007). Neuropsycholinguistic Perspectives on Dyslexia and Other Learning Disabilities. Nova Publishers. p. 94. ISBN   978-1-60021-537-7. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017.
  30. Moore, D. R. (July 2011). "The diagnosis and management of auditory processing disorder". Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools. 42 (3): 303–8. doi:10.1044/0161-1461(2011/10-0032). PMID   21757566.
  31. 1 2 Pammer, Kristen (January 2014). "Brain mechanisms and reading remediation: more questions than answers". Scientifica. 2014: 802741. doi:10.1155/2014/802741. PMC   3913493 . PMID   24527259.
  32. Law, J (2014). "relationship of phonological ability, speech perception, and auditory perception in adults with dyslexia". Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 8: 482. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00482. PMC   4078926 . PMID   25071512.
  33. Susan J. Pickering (2012). "Chapter 2. Working Memory in Dyslexia". In Alloway, Tracy Packiam; Gathercole, Susan E. (eds.). Working Memory and Neurodevelopmental Disorders. Psychology Press. ISBN   978-1-135-42134-2. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017.
  34. 1 2 Berkhan O (1917). "Über die Wortblindheit, ein Stammeln im Sprechen und Schreiben, ein Fehl im Lesen" [About word blindness, adyslalia of speech and writing, a weakness in reading]. Neurologisches Centralblatt (in German). 36: 914–27.
  35. Reid, Gavin; Fawcett, Angela; Manis, Frank; Siegel, Linda (2008). The SAGE Handbook of Dyslexia. SAGE Publications. p. 127. ISBN   978-1-84860-037-9. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017.
  36. 1 2 Stein, John (2014). "Dyslexia: the Role of Vision and Visual Attention". Current Developmental Disorders Reports. 1 (4): 267–80. doi:10.1007/s40474-014-0030-6. PMC   4203994 . PMID   25346883.
  37. Whitaker, Harry A. (2010). Concise Encyclopedia of Brain and Language. Elsevier. p. 180. ISBN   978-0-08-096499-7. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017.
  38. Price, cathy (16 August 2012). "A Review and Synthesis of the first 20 years of Pet and fMRI studies of heard Speech, Spoken Language and Reading". NeuroImage. 62 (2): 816–847. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.04.062. PMC   3398395 . PMID   22584224.
  39. Sharifi, S (May 2014). "Neuroimaging essentials in essential tremor: a systematic review". NeuroImage: Clinical. 5: 217–231. doi:10.1016/j.nicl.2014.05.003. PMC   4110352 . PMID   25068111.
  40. Brandler, William (February 2014). "The genetic relationship between handedness and neurodevelopmental disorders". Trends in Molecular Medicine. 20 (2): 83–90. doi:10.1016/j.molmed.2013.10.008. PMC   3969300 . PMID   24275328.
  41. Cain, Kate (2010). Reading development and difficulties (1st ed.). TJ International. p. 134. ISBN   9781405151559 . Retrieved 21 March 2015.
  42. Levav, Itzhak (2009). Psychiatric and Behavioral Disorders in Israel: From Epidemiology to Mental health. Green Publishing. p. 52. ISBN   9789652294685 . Retrieved 21 March 2015.
  43. Faust, Miriam (2012). The Handbook of the Neuropsychology of Language. John Wiley & Sons. pp. 941–43. ISBN   978-1-4443-3040-3. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017.
  44. Benitez, A (November 2010). "Neurobiology and neurogenetics of dyslexia". Neurology (In Spanish). 25 (9): 563–81. doi: 10.1016/j.nrl.2009.12.010 . PMID   21093706.
  45. Kere, Julia (September 2014). "The molecular genetics and neurobiology of developmental dyslexia as model of a complex phenotype". Biochemical and Biophysical Research Communications. 452 (2): 236–43. doi: 10.1016/j.bbrc.2014.07.102 . PMID   25078623.
  46. Marshall, Chloë R. (2012). Current Issues in Developmental Disorders. Psychology Press. pp. 53–56. ISBN   978-1-136-23067-7. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017.
  47. Paracchini S, Thomas A, Castro S, Lai C, Paramasivam M, Wang Y, Keating B J, Taylor J M, Hacking D F, Scerri T, Francks C, Richardson A J, Wade-Martins R, Stein J F, Knight J C, Copp A J, LoTurco J, Monaco A P (15 May 2006). "The chromosome 6p22 haplotype associated with dyslexia reduces the expression of KIAA0319, a novel gene involved in neuronal migration". Human Molecular Genetics. 15 (10): 1659–1666. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddl089 . PMID   16600991.CS1 maint: uses authors parameter (link)
  48. Rosen, Glenn D. (2013). The Dyslexic Brain: New Pathways in Neuroscience Discovery. Psychology Press. p. 342. ISBN   978-1-134-81550-0. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017.
  49. Friend, A; Defries, J. C.; Olson, R. K. (November 2008). "Parental Education Moderates Genetic Influences on Reading Disability". Psychol. Sci. 19 (11): 1124–30. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02213.x. PMC   2605635 . PMID   19076484.
  50. Taylor, J.; Roehrig, A. D.; Hensler, B. Soden; Connor, C. M.; Schatschneider, C. (2010). "Teacher Quality Moderates the Genetic Effects on Early Reading". Science. 328 (5977): 512–4. Bibcode:2010Sci...328..512T. doi:10.1126/science.1186149. PMC   2905841 . PMID   20413504.
  51. 1 2 Pennington, Bruce F.; McGrath, Lauren M.; Rosenberg, Jenni; Barnard, Holly; Smith, Shelley D.; Willcutt, Erik G.; Friend, Angela; Defries, John C.; Olson, Richard K. (January 2009). "Gene × Environment Interactions in Reading Disability and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder". Developmental Psychology. 45 (1): 77–89. doi:10.1037/a0014549. PMC   2743891 . PMID   19209992.
  52. Roth, Tania L.; Roth, Eric D.; Sweatt, J. David (September 2010). "Epigenetic regulation of genes in learning and memory". Essays in Biochemistry. 48 (1): 263–74. doi:10.1042/bse0480263. PMID   20822498. S2CID   23229766.
  53. Smith, Shelley D. (December 2011). "Approach to epigenetic analysis in language disorders". Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders. 3 (4): 356–364. doi:10.1007/s11689-011-9099-y. ISSN   1866-1947. PMC   3261263 . PMID   22113455.
  54. 1 2 3 Paulesu, Eraldo; Brunswick, Nicola and Paganelli, Federica (2010). "Cross-cultural differences in unimpaired and dyslexic reading: Behavioral and functional anatomical observations in readers of regular and irregular orthographies. Chapter 12 in Reading and Dyslexia in Different Orthographies Archived 9 January 2017 at the Wayback Machine . Eds. Nicola Brunswick, Siné McDougall, and Paul de Mornay Davies. Psychology Press. ISBN   9781135167813
  55. Juel, Connie (2013). "The Impact of Early School Experiences on Initial Reading". In David K. Dickinson; Susan B. Neuman (eds.). Handbook of Early Literacy Research. Guilford Publications. ISBN   978-1-4625-1470-0. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017.
  56. Snowling, Margaret J; Hulme, Charles (1 May 2012). "Annual Research Review: The nature and classification of reading disorders – a commentary on proposals for DSM-5". Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 53 (5): 593–607. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02495.x. PMC   3492851 . PMID   22141434.
  57. 1 2 Habib, Michael (2013). "Dyslexia". Pediatric Neurology Part I. Handbook of Clinical Neurology. 111. pp. 229–235. doi:10.1016/B978-0-444-52891-9.00023-3. ISBN   9780444528919. PMID   23622168 . Retrieved 19 December 2018.
  58. Schumacher, Johannes; Hoffmann, Per; Schmäl, Christine; Schulte‐Körne, Gerd; Nöthen, Markus M (2007). "Genetics of dyslexia: the evolving landscape". Journal of Medical Genetics. 44 (5): 289–297. doi:10.1136/jmg.2006.046516. PMC   2597981 . PMID   17307837.
  59. 1 2 Pritchard SC, Coltheart M, Palethorpe S, Castles A; Coltheart; Palethorpe; Castles (October 2012). "Nonword reading: comparing dual-route cascaded and connectionist dual-process models with human data". J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 38 (5): 1268–88. doi:10.1037/a0026703. PMID   22309087.CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  60. Eysenck, Michael; Keane, Mark T. (2013). Cognitive Psychology 6e. Psychology Press. p. 373. ISBN   978-1-134-44046-7. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017.
  61. 1 2 Eysenck, Michael; Keane, Mark T. (2013). Cognitive Psychology 6e. Psychology Press. p. 450. ISBN   978-1-134-44046-7. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017.
  62. Hulme, Charles; Joshi, R. Malatesha; Snowling, Margaret J. (2012). Reading and Spelling: Development and Disorders. Routledge. p. 151. ISBN   978-1-136-49807-7. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017.
  63. Sprenger-Charolles, Liliane (2011). "Prevalence and Reliability of Phonological, Surface, and Mixed Profiles in Dyslexia: A Review of Studies Conducted in Languages Varying in Orthographic Depth". Scientific Studies of Reading. 15 (6): 498–521. doi:10.1080/10888438.2010.524463. S2CID   15227374. Archived from the original on 30 August 2017.
  64. Boada, Richard; Willcutt, Erik G.; Pennington, Bruce F. (2012). "Understanding the Comorbidity Between Dyslexia and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder". Topics in Language Disorders. 32 (3): 270. doi:10.1097/tld.0b013e31826203ac. S2CID   43200465. ... Pennington proposed a multiple deficit model for complex disorders like dyslexia, hypothesizing that such complex disorders are heterogeneous conditions that arise from the additive and interactive effects of multiple genetic and environmental risk factors, which then lead to weaknesses in multiple cognitive domains.
  65. Pennington, B (September 2006). "From single to multiple deficit models of developmental disorders". Cognition. 101 (2): 385–413. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2006.04.008. PMID   16844106. S2CID   7433822.
  66. Peterson, Robin L.; Pennington, Bruce F. (28 March 2015). "Developmental Dyslexia". Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 11 (1): 283–307. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032814-112842. PMID   25594880. SSRN   2588407 .
  67. Snowling, Margaret J. Dyslexia: A Very Short Introduction. Oxford University Press, 2019. ISBN   9780192550422
  68. "6A03.0 Developmental learning disorder with impairment in reading". International Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems, 11th rev. (ICD-11) (Mortality and Morbidity Statistics). World Health Organization. Retrieved 7 October 2019.
  69. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders : DSM-5 . DSM-5 Task Force. (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association. 2013. ISBN   9780890425541. OCLC   830807378. Specific Learning Disorder with impairment in reading ... Dyslexia is an alternative term used to refer to a pattern of learning difficulties characterized by problems with accurate or fluent word recognition, poor decoding, and poor spelling abilities.CS1 maint: others (link)
  70. FragaGonzález, Gorka; Karipidis, Iliana; Tijms, Jurgen (19 October 2018). "Dyslexia as a Neurodevelopmental Disorder and What Makes It Different from a Chess Disorder". Brain Sciences. 8 (10): 189. doi:10.3390/brainsci8100189. ISSN   2076-3425. PMC   6209961 . PMID   30347764.
  71. Campbell, Robert Jean (2009). Campbell's Psychiatric Dictionary. Oxford University Press. pp. 310–312. ISBN   978-0-19-534159-1. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017.
  72. Phillips, Sylvia; Kelly, Kathleen; Symes, Liz (2013). Assessment of Learners with Dyslexic-Type Difficulties. SAGE. p. 7. ISBN   978-1-4462-8704-0. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017.
  73. Stahl, Steven A.; Murray, Bruce A. (1994). "Defining phonological awareness and its relationship to early reading". Journal of Educational Psychology. 86 (2): 221–234. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.86.2.221.
  74. Rvachew, Susan; Ohberg, Alyssa; Grawburg, Meghann; Heyding, Joan (1 November 2003). "Phonological Awareness and Phonemic Perception in 4-Year-Old Children With Delayed Expressive Phonology Skills". American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology. 12 (4): 463–471. doi:10.1044/1058-0360(2003/092). PMID   14658998. S2CID   16983189.
  75. Catherine Christo, John M. Davis, and Stephen E. Brock, Identifying, Assessing, and Treating Dyslexia at School (New York: Springer Science+Business Media, 2009), 59.
  76. Mather, Nancy and Barbara J. Wendling. Essentials of Dyslexia Assessment and Intervention. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, 2012.
  77. Reid, Gavin and Jennie Guise. The Dyslexia Assessment. London: Bloomsbury, 2017 ("... assessment for dyslexia includes more than tests; it involves comprehensive insights into the student's learning. This requires a full and comprehensive individual assessment as well as consideration of the environment and contextual factors.").
  78. M. S. Thambirajah, Developmental Assessment of the School-Aged Child with Developmental Disabilities: A Clinician's Guide (London: Jessica Kingsley, 2011), 74.
  79. Jimerson, Shane R., Matthew K. Burns, and Amanda M. VanDerHeyden. Handbook of Response to Intervention: The Science and Practice of Multi-Tiered Systems of Support. 2nd ed. New York: Springer Science+Business Media, 2016.
  80. Snowling Margaret J (2013). "Early Identification and Interventions for Dyslexia: A Contemporary View". Journal of Research in Special Education Needs. 13 (1): 7–14. doi:10.1111/j.1471-3802.2012.01262.x. PMC   4538781 . PMID   26290655.
  81. "Tests for Dyslexia and Learning Disabilities". University of Michigan. Archived from the original on 13 March 2015. Retrieved 15 March 2015.
  82. Peer, Lindsay; Reid, Gavin (2013). Introduction to Dyslexia. Taylor & Francis. pp. 35–40. ISBN   978-1-135-37290-3. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017.
  83. 1 2 "Screening and assessment". British Dyslexia Association. Archived from the original on 30 March 2015. Retrieved 11 March 2015.
  84. Fletcher, Jack (2009). "Dyslexia: the evolution of a scientific concept". Journal of International Neuropsychology Society. 15 (4): 501–508. doi:10.1017/S1355617709090900. PMC   3079378 . PMID   19573267.
  85. Schulte-Körne, Gerd (October 2010). "The Prevention, Diagnosis, and Treatment of Dyslexia". Deutsches Ärzteblatt International. 107 (41): 718–727. doi:10.3238/arztebl.2010.0718. ISSN   1866-0452. PMC   2967798 . PMID   21046003.
  86. Mather, N., & Schneider, D. The use of intelligence tests in the diagnosis of specific reading disability.Goldstein, Sam; Princiotta, Dana; Naglieri, Jack A. (2014). Handbook of Intelligence: Evolutionary Theory, Historical Perspective, and Current Concepts. Springer. pp. 415–434. ISBN   9781493915620 . Retrieved 10 January 2019.
  87. Collett, Brent R.; Ohan, Jeneva L.; Myers, Kathleen M. (1 September 2003). "Ten-Year Review of Rating Scales. V: Scales Assessing Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder". Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 42 (9): 1015–1037. doi:10.1097/01.CHI.0000070245.24125.B6. ISSN   0890-8567. PMID   12960702 . Retrieved 3 October 2019.
  88. Stone, Lisanne L; Janssens, Jan M A M; Vermulst, Ad A; Van Der Maten, Marloes; Engels, Rutger C M E; Otten, Roy (20 February 2015). "The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: psychometric properties of the parent and teacher version in children aged 4–7". BMC Psychology. 3 (1): 4. doi:10.1186/s40359-015-0061-8. ISSN   2050-7283. PMC   4364334 . PMID   25815194.
  89. Swart, G. T. (2005). "The Clinician's Guide to the Behavior Assessment System For Children". The Canadian Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Review. 14 (3): 90. ISSN   1716-9119. PMC   2542918 .
  90. Birmaher, B.; Khetarpal, S.; Brent, D.; Cully, M.; Balach, L.; Kaufman, J.; Neer, S. M. (1997). "The Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders (SCARED): scale construction and psychometric characteristics". Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 36 (4): 545–553. doi:10.1097/00004583-199704000-00018. ISSN   0890-8567. PMID   9100430.
  91. Lindquist, E. F. (1953). The Iowa tests of educational development: how to use the test results; a manual for teachers and counselors. Science Research Associates. Retrieved 3 October 2019.
  92. Dell, Cindy Ann; Harrold, Barbara; Dell, Thomas (1 October 2008). "Test Review: Wilkinson, G. S., & Robertson, G. J. (2006). Wide Range Achievement Test—Fourth Edition. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. WRAT4 Introductory Kit (includes manual, 25 test/response forms [blue and green], and accompanying test materials): $243.00". Rehabilitation Counseling Bulletin. 52 (1): 57–60. doi:10.1177/0034355208320076. ISSN   0034-3552. S2CID   145644409.
  93. Semrud-Clikeman, Margaret; Ellison, Phyllis Anne Teeter (2009). Child Neuropsychology: Assessment and Interventions for Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 2nd Edition. Springer Science & Business Media. p. 119. ISBN   9780387889634 . Retrieved 3 October 2019.
  94. Catherine Christo, John M. Davis, and Stephen E. Brock, Identifying, Assessing, and Treating Dyslexia at School (New York: Springer Science+Business Media, 2009), 56. ISBN   9780387885995
  95. Margaret J. Snowling, Dyslexia: A Very Short Introduction (Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 2019), 93–94.
  96. Letters and Sounds: Principles and Practice of High Quality Phonics, Ref: DFES-00281-2007 (00281-2007BKT-EN), Primary National Strategy, Department for Education and Skills (United Kingdom), 2007.
  97. "Phonics screening check: 2019 materials". United Kingdom Department for Education, Standards and Testing Agency. Retrieved 14 October 2019.
  98. Thambirajah, M. S. (2011). Developmental assessment of the school-aged child with developmental disabilities : a clinician's guide. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers. ISBN   9780857003256. OCLC   747410566.
  99. Bogon, Johana (October 2014). "TVA based assessment of visual attention functions in developmental dyslexia". Frontiers in Psychology. 5: 1172. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01172. PMC   4199262 . PMID   25360129.
  100. Brunswick, Nicola (10 April 2012). Supporting Dyslexic Adults in Higher Education and the Workplace. John Wiley & Sons. pp. 115–. ISBN   978-0-470-97479-7. Archived from the original on 31 December 2013. Retrieved 10 April 2012.
  101. 1 2 Schulte-Körne, G (October 2010). "The prevention, diagnosis, and treatment of dyslexia". Deutsches Ärzteblatt International. 107 (41): 718–26. doi:10.3238/arztebl.2010.0718. PMC   2967798 . PMID   21046003.
  102. Lyytinen, Heikki; Erskine, Jane; Aro, Mikko and Richardson, Ulla (2009). "Reading and reading disorders". In Hoff, Erika (ed.). Blackwell Handbook of Language Development. Blackwell. pp. 454–474. ISBN   978-1-4051-9459-4. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017.CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  103. van der Leij, Aryan (1 November 2013). "Dyslexia and early intervention: what did we learn from the Dutch Dyslexia Programme?". Dyslexia. 19 (4): 241–255. doi:10.1002/dys.1466. ISSN   1099-0909. PMID   24133037.
  104. 1 2 3 Kuster, Sanne M.; van Weerdenburg, Marjolijn; Gompel, Marjolein; Bosman, Anna M. T. (April 2018). "Dyslexie font does not benefit reading in children with or without dyslexia". Annals of Dyslexia. 68 (1): 25–42. doi:10.1007/s11881-017-0154-6. ISSN   0736-9387. PMC   5934461 . PMID   29204931.
  105. Cogo-Moreira, Hugo; Andriolo, Régis B; Yazigi, Latife; Ploubidis, George B; Brandão de Ávila, Clara Regina; Mari, Jair J (15 August 2012). "Music education for improving reading skills in children and adolescents with dyslexia" (PDF). Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews (8): CD009133. doi:10.1002/14651858.cd009133.pub2. PMID   22895983.
  106. O’Hare, Anne (2010). "Dyslexia: what do paediatricians need to know?". Paediatrics and Child Health. 20 (7): 338–343. doi:10.1016/j.paed.2010.04.004.
  107. "Response to Intervention Guidance – Minimum Requirements of a Response to Intervention Program (RtI) – Instruction Matched to Student Need: Special Education : P12 : NYSED". p12.nysed.gov. Retrieved 10 January 2019.
  108. Tasman, Allan; Kay, Jerald; Lieberman, Jeffrey A.; First, Michael B.; Riba, Michelle (29 January 2015). Psychiatry, 2 Volume Set. John Wiley & Sons. ISBN   9781118845493. Archived from the original on 6 September 2015.
  109. Protopapas, Athanassios (2013). "From temporal processing to developmental language disorders: mind the gap". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 369 (1634): 20130090. doi:10.1098/rstb.2013.0090. PMC   3866431 . PMID   24324245.
  110. Zhao, Jing (November 2014). "The visual magnocellular-dorsal dysfunction in Chinese children with developmental dyslexia impedes Chinese character recognition". Scientific Reports. 4: 7068. Bibcode:2014NatSR...4E7068Z. doi:10.1038/srep07068. PMC   4238300 . PMID   25412386.
  111. Marshall, Chloe (2012). Current Issues in Developmental Disorders. Psychology Press. p. 152. ISBN   978-1-84872-084-8. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017.
  112. Garralda, Elena; Raynaud, Jean-Philippe (16 January 2012). Brain, Mind, and Developmental Psychopathology in Childhood. Jason Aronson. ISBN   9780765708663.
  113. Navas, Ana Luiza Gomes Pinto; Ferraz, Érica de Cássia; Borges, Juliana Postigo Amorina; Navas, Ana Luiza Gomes Pinto; Ferraz, Érica de Cássia; Borges, Juliana Postigo Amorina (2014). "Phonological processing deficits as a universal model for dyslexia: evidence from different orthographies". CoDAS. 26 (6): 509–519. doi: 10.1590/2317-1782/20142014135 . PMID   25590915.
  114. Berlin, Rudolf. [No title.] Medicinisches Correspondenzblatt des Württembergischen Ärztlichen Landesvereins [Correspondence Sheet of the Württemberg Medical Association] 53 (1883): 209.
  115. Webster's Third New International Dictionary. "History and Etymology for dyslexia", s.v. "dyslexia, noun". Springfield, MA: Merriam-Webster, 1961, rev. 2016.
  116. "Über Dyslexie" [About dyslexia]. Archiv für Psychiatrie. 15: 276–278. 1884.
  117. Annual of the Universal Medical Sciences and Analytical Index: A Yearly Report of the Progress of the General Sanitary Sciences Throughout the World. F. A. Davis Company. 1888. p. 39. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017.
  118. Brooks, Patricia (2014). Encyclopedia of language development. SAGE. p. 30. ISBN   9781483346434.
  119. Mishra, Srikanta K. (October 2014). "Medial efferent mechanisms in children with auditory processing disorders". Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 8: 860. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00860. PMC   4209830 . PMID   25386132.
  120. de Berr, J (2014). "Factors influencing work participation of adults with developmental dyslexia". BMC Public Health. 14: 77. doi:10.1186/1471-2458-14-77. PMC   3913008 . PMID   24460949.
  121. Pino, Marco; Mortari, Luigina (1 November 2014). "The Inclusion of Students with Dyslexia in Higher Education: A Systematic Review Using Narrative Synthesis". Dyslexia (Chichester, England). 20 (4): 346–369. doi:10.1002/dys.1484. PMC   4253321 . PMID   25293652.
  122. 1 2 3 4 Kale, Sirin (17 September 2020). "The Battle over Dyslexia". The Guardian. Retrieved 28 September 2020.
  123. Reid, Gavin (2012). The Routledge Companion to Dyslexia. Routledge. p. 16. ISBN   978-1-136-61710-2. Archived from the original on 9 January 2017.
  124. Richlan, Fabio (May 2014). "Functional neuroanatomy of developmental dyslexia; the role of orthographic depth". Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 8: 347. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00347. PMC   4033006 . PMID   24904383.
  125. "Reading Difficulty and Disability" (PDF). report.nih.gov. NIH. Archived from the original (PDF) on 11 February 2019. Retrieved 10 January 2019.

Open Access logo PLoS transparent.svg This article was submitted to WikiJournal of Medicine for external academic peer review in 2018 (reviewer reports). The updated content was reintegrated into the Wikipedia page under a CC-BY-SA-3.0 license (2019). The version of record as reviewed is: Osmin Anis; et al. (2019). "Dyslexia". WikiJournal of Medicine. 6 (1): 5. doi:10.15347/WJM/2019.005. ISSN   2002-4436. Wikidata   Q73053061.

Further reading

Classification
D
External resources