Ventromedial prefrontal cortex

Last updated
Ventromedial prefrontal cortex
Ventromedial prefrontal cortex.png
Ventromedial prefrontal cortex shown on medial and ventral views of the brain, reflecting approximate location of damage in patients with decision making deficits. [1]
Gray727-Brodman.png
Medial surface of the brain with Brodmann's areas numbered.
Details
Identifiers
Latin cortex praefrontalis ventromedialis
Anatomical terms of neuroanatomy

The ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) is a part of the prefrontal cortex in the mammalian brain. The ventral medial prefrontal is located in the frontal lobe at the bottom of the cerebral hemispheres and is implicated in the processing of risk and fear, as it is critical in the regulation of amygdala activity in humans. [2] It also plays a role in the inhibition of emotional responses, and in the process of decision-making and self-control. It is also involved in the cognitive evaluation of morality.

Contents

Anatomy

While the ventromedial prefrontal cortex does not have a universally agreed on demarcation, in most sources, it is equivalent to the ventromedial reward network of Öngür and Price. [3] This network includes Brodmann area 10, Brodmann area 14, Brodmann area 25, and Brodmann area 32, as well as portions of Brodmann area 11, Brodmann area 12, and Brodmann area 13. [4] However, not all sources agree on the boundaries of the area. Different researchers use the term ventromedial prefrontal cortex differently. Sometimes, the term is saved for the area above the medial orbitofrontal cortex, while at other times, 'ventromedial prefrontal cortex' is used to describe a broad area in the lower (ventral) central (medial) region of the prefrontal cortex, of which the medial orbitofrontal cortex constitutes the lowermost part. This latter, broader area, corresponds to the area damaged in patients with decision-making impairments investigated by António Damásio and colleagues (see diagram, and below).

The ventromedial prefrontal cortex is connected to and receives input from the ventral tegmental area, amygdala, the temporal lobe, the olfactory system, and the dorsomedial thalamus. It, in turn, sends signals to many different brain regions including; The temporal lobe, amygdala, the lateral hypothalamus, the hippocampal formation, the cingulate cortex, and certain other regions of the prefrontal cortex. [5] This huge network of connections affords the vmPFC the ability to receive and monitor large amounts of sensory data and to affect and influence a plethora of other brain regions, particularly the amygdala.

Function

Functional differences between the orbitofrontal and ventromedial areas of the pre-frontal cortex have not yet been clearly established, although the areas of the ventromedial cortex superior to the orbitofrontal cortex are much less associated with social functions and more with pure emotion regulation. Research in developmental neuroscience also suggested that neural networks in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex are rapidly developing during adolescence and young adulthood supporting emotion regulation through the amygdala, [6] being associated with a decrease in cortisol levels.
There are only a few reports of people with early-onset vmPFC damage during childhood, but these individuals tend to have severe antisocial behavior and impaired moral judgment. Compared to individuals with damage later in life, their behavior pattern is similar but more severe. [7] It is also considered central to the physiology of anxiety and mood disorders. However, the precise mechanisms by which vmPFC contributes to affective processing are not fully understood. [2]

Decision making

Patients with bilateral lesions of the vmPFC develop severe impairments in personal and social decision-making [5] [8] even though most of their intellectual ability is preserved. [8] [9] For instance, they have difficulties in choosing between options with uncertain outcomes, whether the uncertainty is in the form of a risk or of an ambiguity. [10] After their lesion, these patients have an impaired capacity to learn from their mistakes, making the same decisions again and again even though they lead to negative consequences. These patients choose alternatives that give immediate rewards, but seem to be blind to the future consequences of their actions. [8] However, the underlying mechanisms of this behavior are not yet fully understood. [8]

Damage to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (especially in the right hemisphere) has been connected with deficits in detecting irony, sarcasm, and deception. [11] Subjects with damage in this area have been found to be more easily influenced by misleading advertising. [12] This has been attributed to a disruption of a "false tagging mechanism" which provides doubt and skepticism of new beliefs.

People with damage to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex still retain the ability to consciously make moral judgments without error, but only in hypothetical situations presented to them. They are severely impaired in making personal and social decisions. [13] There is a gap in reasoning when applying the same moral principles to similar situations in their own lives. The result is that people make decisions that are inconsistent with their self professed moral values. [5] People with early damage to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex are more likely to endorse self-serving actions that break moral rules or cause harm to others. This is especially true for patients whose damage occurred the earliest in life. [14]

Emotions and an understanding of social norms are used to provide reasoning of the moral nature on our behaviors, beliefs, and the people around us. The vmPFC works as the neural basis in allowing emotion to influence moral judgement. In functional imaging studies, increased activity in the vmPFC is associated with thinking of these personal moral situations, while making harmless decisions does not. [15] Patients with vmPFC lesions made the same decision in impersonal and personal dilemmas. Dysfunction of the vmPFC causes failure in using correct moral emotion, which explains why these patients showed less emotional responses when facing these dilemmas. [16]

Regulation of emotion

The vmPFC plays an important role in regulating and inhibiting our response to emotions. VmPFC seems to use our emotional reactions to model our behavior and control emotional reactions in certain social situations. The inputs of the vmPFC provide it with information from the environment and the plans of the frontal lobe, and its outputs allow the vmPFC to control different physiological responses and behaviors. The role of the vmPFC is especially highlighted in people with damage to this region. A damaged vmPFC causes impairments of behavioral control and decision making, consequences which are rooted in emotional dysregulation.

The first and most famous case of someone with defects to this region was Phineas Gage, a railroad construction foreman who had his vmPFC bilaterally destroyed in an accident in 1848. Before his accident, Gage was described as “serious, industrious and energetic. Afterward he became childish, irresponsible, and thoughtless of others.” [17] Another patient with vmPFC damage wasted away his life savings on foolish investments and failed to make appropriate decisions in his personal life. In patients with vmPFC damage, evidence shows that there is a correlation between emotional disregulation and dysfunction in real world competencies. [17]

The amygdala plays a significant role in instigating the emotional reactions associated with anger and violence. With the vmPFC’s outputs to the amygdala, the vmPFC plays a part in preventing such behavior. Evidence has shown that impulsive murderers have decreased activity in the prefrontal cortex and increased activity in subcortical areas such as the amygdala. This imbalance can enhance actions that are created by negative emotions and limit the ability of the prefrontal cortex to control these emotions. Lower activation in the prefrontal cortex is also correlated with antisocial behavior. The dysfunction of the ventromedial cortex seems to, in part, be caused by lower levels of serotonin release. [17]

The vmPFC also is involved in courage. In experiments with participants allowing snakes to come near or away from them, acts of courage correlated with activation in the vmPFC, specifically the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex. [17] [18]

Activation of the vmPFC is associated with successful suppression of emotional responses to a negative emotional signal. [19] Patients with vmPFC lesions show defects both in emotional response and emotion regulation. [9] Their emotional responsiveness is generally diminished and they show markedly reduced social emotions such as compassion, shame and guilt. These are emotions that are closely associated with moral values. [9] Patients also exhibit poorly regulated anger and frustration tolerance in certain circumstances. [9]

Patients with focal lesions in the vmPFC show personality changes such as lack of empathy, irresponsibility, and poor decision making. These traits are similar to psychopathic personality traits. [20] In addition, a correlation between individuals with a history of physical violence and decreased grey matter density in the vmPFC has been evidenced. [21] Lesions to the vmPFC are associated with resistance to depression, whereas lesions to the dorsolateral PFC is associated with vulnerability to depression. [22]

The right half of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex was associated with regulating the interaction of cognition and affect in the production of empathic responses. Hedonic (pleasure) responses were also associations to orbitofrontal cortex activity level by Morten Kringelbach. This finding contributes findings suggesting ventromedial prefrontal cortex being associated with preference judgement, possibly assigning the ventromedial prefrontal cortex a key role in constructing one's self. fMRI scans have found that the vmPFC is active when people think about themselves. There are cultural differences in the use of this region based on cultural differences in self-perception. Chinese subjects who think of the self in relation to the community have been found to utilize the vmPFC when thinking about their mothers, whereas American subjects do not. [23]

Studies with post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) also supported the idea that the ventromedial prefrontal cortex is an important component for reactivating past emotional associations and events, therefore essentially mediating pathogenesis of PTSD. [24] [25] Dysfunction of the vmPFC has also been identified as playing a role in PTSD-affected parents' response to their own children's mental states. [26] Treatments geared to the activation of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex were therefore suggested for individuals and parent-child relationships affected by PTSD. The right half of the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, being active during emotion regulation, was activated when participants were offered an unfair offer in a scenario. Specific deficits in reversal learning and decision-making have led to the hypothesis that the ventromedial prefrontal cortex is a major locus of dysfunction in the mild stages of the behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia. [27] A study of patients with lesions in the right vmPFC showed a loss of empathy and theory of mind, showing that the brain regions is directly involved in empathy and mentalizing. [28]

The capacity for mature defense mechanisms such as intellectualization, compensation, reaction formation, and isolation has been tied to proper functioning of the right ventromedial prefrontal cortex, while more primitive defense mechanisms such as projection, splitting, verbal denial, and fantasy have been found to rely on other regions, primarily in the left hemisphere. [29]

Somatic marker hypothesis

One particularly notable theory of vmPFC function is the somatic marker hypothesis, accredited to António Damásio. By this hypothesis, the vmPFC has a central role in adapting somatic markers—emotional associations, or associations between mental objects and visceral (bodily) feedback—for use in natural decision making. This account also gives the vmPFC a role in moderating emotions and emotional reactions because whether the vmPFC decides the markers are positive or negative affects the appropriate response in a particular situation.[ citation needed ] However, a critical review of this hypothesis concluded that there is a need for additional empirical data to support the somatic marker theory. [19]

Extinction

Another role that the vmPFC plays is in the process of extinction, the gradual weakening and eventual cessation of a conditioned response, as studies have shown increased activation of the vmPFC after extinction training. [30] The specific role played by the vmPFC concerning extinction is not well understood, but it is believed that it plays a necessary role in the recall of extinction learning after a long period of time. Studies show that it aids in the consolidation of extinction learning. [31] A separate study has implicated the correlation between the cortical thickness of the vmPFC and the degree of extinction memory. Patients with larger vmPFCs tended to have lower responses to the extinct conditioned stimulus, therefore suggesting a superior extinction memory. [32] In general, the ventromedial prefrontal cortex plays a major role in the later stages of memory consolidation. [33]

Gender specific social cues

Ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions were also associated with a deficit in processing gender specific social cues. One experiment tested the ability of patients with vmPFC lesions to categorize gender-specific names, attributes, and attitudes compared to patients with dorsolateral prefrontal cortex lesions and control subjects. Whereas the patients with dorsolateral prefrontal cortex lesions performed similarly to the control subjects on tests indicating gender stereotypes, patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions demonstrated impaired stereotypic social knowledge. [34]

Cocaine abuse

Frequent cocaine users have been shown to have lower than normal activity in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. When asked to perform certain tasks that rely heavily on activation of this area of the brain, the cocaine users perform worse and have less prefrontal cortex activation than the control subjects. [35] The quantity of cocaine used was found to be inversely proportional to the level of activation. [36]

The prefrontal cortex is also physically affected by cocaine use. Chronic use has been shown to lead to a decrease in the amount of gray matter in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. The decrease in gray matter and effect on behavior is analogous to a person having lesions throughout their medial prefrontal cortex. [35] Specifically, the pyramidal cells of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex are known to be linked with drug seeking behaviors. [37] Both an increased and decreased level of activity in these pyramidal cells has shown to lead to extinction of cocaine-seeking behaviors depending on when the activation takes place. Inactivation of these cells was needed to inhibit cocaine-seeking behavior after a longer duration of time, whereas activation was required to reduce the behavior soon after using cocaine. [38]

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Amygdala</span> Each of two small structures deep within the temporal lobe of complex vertebrates

The amygdala is one of two almond-shaped clusters of nuclei located deep and medially within the temporal lobes of the brain's cerebrum in complex vertebrates, including humans. Shown to perform a primary role in the processing of memory, decision making, and emotional responses, the amygdalae are considered part of the limbic system. The term "amygdala" was first introduced by Karl Friedrich Burdach in 1822.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Anterior cingulate cortex</span> Brain region

In the human brain, the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) is the frontal part of the cingulate cortex that resembles a "collar" surrounding the frontal part of the corpus callosum. It consists of Brodmann areas 24, 32, and 33.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Antonio Damasio</span> Portuguese neuroscientist (born 1944)

Antonio Damasio is a Portuguese neuroscientist. He is currently the David Dornsife Chair in Neuroscience, as well as Professor of Psychology, Philosophy, and Neurology, at the University of Southern California, and, additionally, an adjunct professor at the Salk Institute. He was previously the chair of neurology at the University of Iowa for 20 years. Damasio heads the Brain and Creativity Institute, and has authored several books: his work, Self Comes to Mind: Constructing the Conscious Brain (2010), explores the relationship between the brain and consciousness. Damasio's research in neuroscience has shown that emotions play a central role in social cognition and decision-making.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Prefrontal cortex</span> Part of the brain responsible for personality, decision-making, and social behavior

In mammalian brain anatomy, the prefrontal cortex (PFC) covers the front part of the frontal lobe of the cerebral cortex. It is the association cortex in the frontal lobe. The PFC contains the Brodmann areas BA8, BA9, BA10, BA11, BA12, BA13, BA14, BA24, BA25, BA32, BA44, BA45, BA46, and BA47.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Somatic marker hypothesis</span> Hypothesis that emotional processes guide or bias decision-making

The somatic marker hypothesis, formulated by Antonio Damasio and associated researchers, proposes that emotional processes guide behavior, particularly decision-making.

Affective neuroscience is the study of how the brain processes emotions. This field combines neuroscience with the psychological study of personality, emotion, and mood. The basis of emotions and what emotions are remains an issue of debate within the field of affective neuroscience.

Reduced affect display, sometimes referred to as emotional blunting or emotional numbing, is a condition of reduced emotional reactivity in an individual. It manifests as a failure to express feelings either verbally or nonverbally, especially when talking about issues that would normally be expected to engage emotions. In this condition, expressive gestures are rare and there is little animation in facial expression or vocal inflection. Additionally, reduced affect can be symptomatic of autism, schizophrenia, depression, post-traumatic stress disorder, depersonalization disorder, schizoid personality disorder or brain damage. It may also be a side effect of certain medications.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Orbitofrontal cortex</span> Region of the prefrontal cortex of the brain

The orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) is a prefrontal cortex region in the frontal lobes of the brain which is involved in the cognitive process of decision-making. In non-human primates it consists of the association cortex areas Brodmann area 11, 12 and 13; in humans it consists of Brodmann area 10, 11 and 47.

The Iowa gambling task (IGT) is a psychological task thought to simulate real-life decision making. It was introduced by Antoine Bechara, Antonio Damasio, Hanna Damasio and Steven Anderson, then researchers at the University of Iowa. It has been brought to popular attention by Antonio Damasio in his best-selling book Descartes' Error.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Synaptic gating</span>

Synaptic gating is the ability of neural circuits to gate inputs by either suppressing or facilitating specific synaptic activity. Selective inhibition of certain synapses has been studied thoroughly, and recent studies have supported the existence of permissively gated synaptic transmission. In general, synaptic gating involves a mechanism of central control over neuronal output. It includes a sort of gatekeeper neuron, which has the ability to influence transmission of information to selected targets independently of the parts of the synapse upon which it exerts its action.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex</span> Area of the prefrontal cortex of primates

The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex is an area in the prefrontal cortex of the primate brain. It is one of the most recently derived parts of the human brain. It undergoes a prolonged period of maturation which lasts into adulthood. The DLPFC is not an anatomical structure, but rather a functional one. It lies in the middle frontal gyrus of humans. In macaque monkeys, it is around the principal sulcus. Other sources consider that DLPFC is attributed anatomically to BA 9 and 46 and BA 8, 9 and 10.

The Intercalatedcells of the amygdala are GABAergic neurons situated between the basolateral and central nuclei of the amygdala that play a significant role in inhibitory control over the amygdala. They regulate amygdala-dependent emotional processing like fear memory and social behavior. Their function has been best studied with selective ITC ablation which impairs fear extinction, fear generalization, and social behavior. Studies have begun to recognize that ITC clusters may be implicated in reward, addiction, and withdrawal circuits given their heavy expression of dopamine and opioid receptors.

Consumer neuroscience is the combination of consumer research with modern neuroscience. The goal of the field is to find neural explanations for consumer behaviors in individuals both with or without disease.

Emotional lateralization is the asymmetrical representation of emotional control and processing in the brain. There is evidence for the lateralization of other brain functions as well.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Mechanisms of mindfulness meditation</span>

Mindfulness has been defined in modern psychological terms as "paying attention to relevant aspects of experience in a nonjudgmental manner", and maintaining attention on present moment experience with an attitude of openness and acceptance. Meditation is a platform used to achieve mindfulness. Both practices, mindfulness and meditation, have been "directly inspired from the Buddhist tradition" and have been widely promoted by Jon Kabat-Zinn. Mindfulness meditation has been shown to have a positive impact on several psychiatric problems such as depression and therefore has formed the basis of mindfulness programs such as mindfulness-based cognitive therapy, mindfulness-based stress reduction and mindfulness-based pain management. The applications of mindfulness meditation are well established, however the mechanisms that underlie this practice are yet to be fully understood. Many tests and studies on soldiers with PTSD have shown tremendous positive results in decreasing stress levels and being able to cope with problems of the past, paving the way for more tests and studies to normalize and accept mindful based meditation and research, not only for soldiers with PTSD, but numerous mental inabilities or disabilities.

The neurocircuitry that underlies executive function processes and emotional and motivational processes are known to be distinct in the brain. However, there are brain regions that show overlap in function between the two cognitive systems. Brain regions that exist in both systems are interesting mainly for studies on how one system affects the other. Examples of such cross-modal functions are emotional regulation strategies such as emotional suppression and emotional reappraisal, the effect of mood on cognitive tasks, and the effect of emotional stimulation of cognitive tasks.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Dorsomedial prefrontal cortex</span> Area of some species brains

The dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC or DMPFC is a section of the prefrontal cortex in some species' brain anatomy. It includes portions of Brodmann areas BA8, BA9, BA10, BA24 and BA32, although some authors identify it specifically with BA8 and BA9. Some notable sub-components include the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, the prelimbic cortex, and the infralimbic cortex.

Neuromorality is an emerging field of neuroscience that studies the connection between morality and neuronal function. Scientists use fMRI and psychological assessment together to investigate the neural basis of moral cognition and behavior. Evidence shows that the central hub of morality is the prefrontal cortex guiding activity to other nodes of the neuromoral network. A spectrum of functional characteristics within this network to give rise to both altruistic and psychopathological behavior. Evidence from the investigation of neuromorality has applications in both clinical neuropsychiatry and forensic neuropsychiatry.

Social cognitive neuroscience is the scientific study of the biological processes underpinning social cognition. Specifically, it uses the tools of neuroscience to study "the mental mechanisms that create, frame, regulate, and respond to our experience of the social world". Social cognitive neuroscience uses the epistemological foundations of cognitive neuroscience, and is closely related to social neuroscience. Social cognitive neuroscience employs human neuroimaging, typically using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Human brain stimulation techniques such as transcranial magnetic stimulation and transcranial direct-current stimulation are also used. In nonhuman animals, direct electrophysiological recordings and electrical stimulation of single cells and neuronal populations are utilized for investigating lower-level social cognitive processes.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Antoine Bechara</span> American neuroscientist

Antoine Bechara is an American neuroscientist, academic and researcher. He is currently a professor of Psychology and Neuroscience at the University of Southern California.

References

  1. Bechara A, Damasio H, Tranel D, Anderson SW (January 1998). "Dissociation Of working memory from decision making within the human prefrontal cortex". The Journal of Neuroscience. 18 (1): 428–37. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-01-00428.1998. PMC   6793407 . PMID   9412519.
  2. 1 2 Motzkin JC, Philippi CL, Wolf RC, Baskaya MK, Koenigs M (February 2015). "Ventromedial prefrontal cortex is critical for the regulation of amygdala activity in humans". Biological Psychiatry. 77 (3): 276–284. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.02.014. PMC   4145052 . PMID   24673881.
  3. Ongür D, Price JL (March 2000). "The organization of networks within the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of rats, monkeys and humans". Cerebral Cortex. 10 (3): 206–19. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.3.206 . PMID   10731217.
  4. Finger EC, Marsh AA, Mitchell DG, Reid ME, Sims C, Budhani S, Kosson DS, Chen G, Towbin KE, Leibenluft E, Pine DS, Blair JR (May 2008). "Abnormal ventromedial prefrontal cortex function in children with psychopathic traits during reversal learning". Archives of General Psychiatry. 65 (5): 586–94. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.65.5.586. PMC   3104600 . PMID   18458210.
  5. 1 2 3 Carlson NR (2013). Physiology of Behavior (11th ed.). Boston: Pearson.
  6. Decety J, Michalska KJ (November 2010). "Neurodevelopmental changes in the circuits underlying empathy and sympathy from childhood to adulthood". Developmental Science. 13 (6): 886–99. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00940.x. PMID   20977559. S2CID   10647101.
  7. Boes AD, Grafft AH, Joshi C, Chuang NA, Nopoulos P, Anderson SW (December 2011). "Behavioral effects of congenital ventromedial prefrontal cortex malformation". BMC Neurology. 11 (151): 151. doi: 10.1186/1471-2377-11-151 . PMC   3265436 . PMID   22136635.
  8. 1 2 3 4 Bechara A, Tranel D, Damasio H (November 2000). "Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions". Brain. 123 ( Pt 11) (11): 2189–202. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.11.2189 . PMID   11050020.
  9. 1 2 3 4 Koenigs M, Young L, Adolphs R, Tranel D, Cushman F, Hauser M, Damasio A (April 2007). "Damage to the prefrontal cortex increases utilitarian moral judgements". Nature. 446 (7138): 908–11. Bibcode:2007Natur.446..908K. doi:10.1038/nature05631. PMC   2244801 . PMID   17377536.
  10. Fellows LK, Farah MJ (November 2007). "The role of ventromedial prefrontal cortex in decision making: judgment under uncertainty or judgment per se?". Cerebral Cortex. 17 (11): 2669–74. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl176 . PMID   17259643.
  11. Zald DH, Andreotti C (October 2010). "Neuropsychological assessment of the orbital and ventromedial prefrontal cortex". Neuropsychologia. 48 (12): 3377–91. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.08.012. PMID   20728457. S2CID   18918430.
  12. Asp E, Manzel K, Koestner B, Cole CA, Denburg NL, Tranel D (2012). "A neuropsychological test of belief and doubt: damage to ventromedial prefrontal cortex increases credulity for misleading advertising". Frontiers in Neuroscience. 6: 100. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2012.00100 . PMC   3391647 . PMID   22787439.
  13. Bechara A, Tranel D, Damasio H (November 2000). "Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions". Brain. 123 (11): 2189–202. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.11.2189 . PMID   11050020.
  14. Taber-Thomas BC, Asp EW, Koenigs M, Sutterer M, Anderson SW, Tranel D (April 2014). "Arrested development: early prefrontal lesions impair the maturation of moral judgement". Brain. 137 (Pt 4): 1254–61. doi:10.1093/brain/awt377. PMC   3959552 . PMID   24519974.
  15. Nicolle A, Goel V (2013). "What is the role of ventromedial prefrontal cortex in emotional influences on reason?". In Blanchette I (ed.). Emotion and Reasoning. Psychology Press.
  16. Hu C, Jiang X (2014). "An emotion regulation role of ventromedial prefrontal cortex in moral judgment". Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 8: 873. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00873 . PMC   4211379 . PMID   25389402.
  17. 1 2 3 4 Carlson, N. (2012). Physiology of Behavior (11th ed.). Harlow: Prentice Hall.
  18. Nili U, Goldberg H, Weizman A, Dudai Y (June 2010). "Fear thou not: activity of frontal and temporal circuits in moments of real-life courage". Neuron. 66 (6): 949–62. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.06.009 . PMID   20620879. S2CID   13941370.
  19. 1 2 Hänsel A, von Känel R (November 2008). "The ventro-medial prefrontal cortex: a major link between the autonomic nervous system, regulation of emotion, and stress reactivity?". BioPsychoSocial Medicine. 2 (21): 21. doi: 10.1186/1751-0759-2-21 . PMC   2590602 . PMID   18986513.
  20. Motzkin JC, Newman JP, Kiehl KA, Koenigs M (November 2011). "Reduced prefrontal connectivity in psychopathy". The Journal of Neuroscience. 31 (48): 17348–57. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.4215-11.2011. PMC   3311922 . PMID   22131397.
  21. Chester DS, Lynam DR, Milich R, DeWall CN (December 2017). "Physical aggressiveness and gray matter deficits in ventromedial prefrontal cortex". Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior. 97 (Supplement C): 17–22. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2017.09.024. PMC   5716918 . PMID   29073459.
  22. Koenigs, Michael; Huey, Huey D; Calamia, Matthew; Raymont, Vanessa; Tranel, Daniel; Grafman, Jordan (2008). "Distinct regions of prefrontal cortex mediate resistance and vulnerability to depression". J Neurosci. 28 (47): 12341–12348. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2324-08.2008. PMC   2644261 . PMID   19020027.
  23. Zhu, Ying; Zhang, Li; Fan, Jin; Han, Shihui (2007). "Neural basis of cultural influence on self-representation". NeuroImage. Elsevier. 34 (3): 1310–1316. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.08.047. ISSN   1053-8119. PMID   17134915. S2CID   11613104.
  24. Insel TR (April 2009). "Disruptive insights in psychiatry: transforming a clinical discipline". The Journal of Clinical Investigation. 119 (4): 700–5. doi:10.1172/jci38832. PMC   2662575 . PMID   19339761.
  25. Koenigs M, Grafman J (October 2009). "Posttraumatic stress disorder: the role of medial prefrontal cortex and amygdala". The Neuroscientist. 15 (5): 540–8. doi:10.1177/1073858409333072. PMC   2771687 . PMID   19359671.
  26. Schechter DS, Moser DA, Giacobino A, Stenz L, Gex-Fabry M, Adouan W, Cordero MI, Suardi F, Manini A, Sancho-Rossignol A, Merminod G, Aue T, Ansermet F, Dayer AG, Rusconi-Serpa S. (epub May 29, 2015) Methylation of NR3C1 is related to maternal PTSD, parenting stress and maternal medial prefrontal cortical activity in response to child separation among mothers with histories of violence exposure. Frontiers in Psychology. http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00690/abstract
  27. Grossman M, Eslinger PJ, Troiani V, Anderson C, Avants B, Gee JC, McMillan C, Massimo L, Khan A, Antani S (October 2010). "The role of ventral medial prefrontal cortex in social decisions: converging evidence from fMRI and frontotemporal lobar degeneration". Neuropsychologia. 48 (12): 3505–12. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.07.036. PMC   2949451 . PMID   20691197.
  28. Shamay-Tsoory SG, Tomer R, Berger BD, Aharon-Peretz J (April 2003). "Characterization of empathy deficits following prefrontal brain damage: the role of the right ventromedial prefrontal cortex". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 15 (3): 324–37. doi:10.1162/089892903321593063. PMID   12729486. S2CID   8416412.
  29. Northoff G (2010). "Region-based approach versus mechanism-based approach to the brain". Neuropsychoanalysis. 12 (2): 167–170. doi:10.1080/15294145.2010.10773640. S2CID   741542.
  30. Madsen HB, Guerin AA, Kim JH (November 2017). "Investigating the role of dopamine receptor- and parvalbumin-expressing cells in extinction of conditioned fear". Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 145: 7–17. doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2017.08.009. PMID   28842281. S2CID   26875742.
  31. Quirk GJ, Russo GK, Barron JL, Lebron K (August 2000). "The role of ventromedial prefrontal cortex in the recovery of extinguished fear". The Journal of Neuroscience. 20 (16): 6225–31. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-16-06225.2000. PMC   6772571 . PMID   10934272.
  32. Milad MR, Quinn BT, Pitman RK, Orr SP, Fischl B, Rauch SL (July 2005). "Thickness of ventromedial prefrontal cortex in humans is correlated with extinction memory". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 102 (30): 10706–11. Bibcode:2005PNAS..10210706M. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0502441102 . PMC   1180773 . PMID   16024728. These results are a possible factor for explaining why different people show different degrees of controlling their fear.
  33. Nieuwenhuis IL, Takashima A (April 2011). "The role of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in memory consolidation". Behavioural Brain Research. 218 (2): 325–34. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2010.12.009. hdl: 2066/99828 . PMID   21147169. S2CID   15659237.
  34. Milne E, Grafman J (June 2001). "Ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions in humans eliminate implicit gender stereotyping". The Journal of Neuroscience. 21 (12): RC150. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-12-j0001.2001. PMC   6762729 . PMID   11404442.
  35. 1 2 Carlson N (1977). Physiology of Behavior (11th ed.). Boston: Allyn and Bacon. pp. 621–622. ISBN   978-0-205-05706-1.
  36. Bolla K, Ernst M, Kiehl K, Mouratidis M, Eldreth D, Contoreggi C, Matochik J, Kurian V, Cadet J, Kimes A, Funderburk F, London E (2004). "Prefrontal cortical dysfunction in abstinent cocaine abusers". The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 16 (4): 456–64. doi:10.1176/appi.neuropsych.16.4.456. PMC   2771441 . PMID   15616172.
  37. Kalivas PW, Volkow N, Seamans J (March 2005). "Unmanageable motivation in addiction: a pathology in prefrontal-accumbens glutamate transmission". Neuron. 45 (5): 647–50. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.02.005 . PMID   15748840. S2CID   2803383.
  38. Van den Oever MC, Rotaru DC, Heinsbroek JA, Gouwenberg Y, Deisseroth K, Stuber GD, Mansvelder HD, Smit AB (November 2013). "Ventromedial prefrontal cortex pyramidal cells have a temporal dynamic role in recall and extinction of cocaine-associated memory". The Journal of Neuroscience. 33 (46): 18225–33. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2412-13.2013. PMC   3828471 . PMID   24227731.

Further reading