Out-of-body experience

Last updated

Artist's depiction of the separation stage of an out-of-body experience, which often precedes free movement Astral 640.jpg
Artist's depiction of the separation stage of an out-of-body experience, which often precedes free movement

An out-of-body experience (OBE or sometimes OOBE) is a phenomenon in which a person perceives the world from a location outside their physical body. An OBE is a form of autoscopy (literally "seeing self"), although this term is more commonly used to refer to the pathological condition of seeing a second self, or doppelgänger.

Contents

The term out-of-body experience was introduced in 1943 by G. N. M. Tyrrell in his book Apparitions, [1] and was adopted by researchers such as Celia Green, [2] and Robert Monroe, [3] as an alternative to belief-centric labels such as "astral projection" or "spirit walking". OBEs can be induced by traumatic brain injuries, sensory deprivation, near-death experiences, dissociative and psychedelic drugs, dehydration, sleep disorders, dreaming, and electrical stimulation of the brain, [4] among other causes. It can also be deliberately induced by some. [5] One in ten people has an OBE once, or more commonly, several times in their life. [6] [7]

Psychologists and neuroscientists regard OBEs as dissociative experiences occurring along different psychological and neurological factors. [5] [8] [9] [10] [11] [12] [13] [14]

Spontaneous OBEs

During/near sleep

Those experiencing OBEs sometimes report (among other types of immediate and spontaneous experience) a preceding and initiating lucid-dream state. In many cases, people who claim to have had an OBE report being on the verge of sleep, or being already asleep shortly before the experience. A large percentage of these cases refer to situations where the sleep was not particularly deep (due to illness, noises in other rooms, emotional stress, exhaustion from overworking, frequent re-awakening, etc.). In most of these cases subjects perceive themselves as being awake; about half of them note a feeling of sleep paralysis. [15]

Near-death experiences

Another form of spontaneous OBE is the near-death experience (NDE). Some subjects report having had an OBE at times of severe physical trauma such as near-drownings or major surgery. Near-death experiences may include subjective impressions of being outside the physical body, sometimes visions of deceased relatives and religious figures, and transcendence of ego and spatiotemporal boundaries. [16] The experience typically includes such factors as: a sense of being dead; a feeling of peace and painlessness; hearing of various non-physical sounds, an out-of-body experience; a tunnel experience (the sense of moving up or through a narrow passageway); encountering "beings of light" and a God-like figure or similar entities; being given a "life review", and a reluctance to return to life. [17]

Resulting from extreme physical effort

Along the same lines as an NDE, extreme physical effort during activities such as high-altitude climbing and marathon running can induce OBEs. A sense of bilocation may be experienced, with both ground and air-based perspectives being experienced simultaneously. [18]

Induced OBEs

Chemical

Mental induction

Mechanical induction

OBE theories

Psychological

In the fields of cognitive science and psychology OBEs are considered dissociative experiences arising from different psychological and neurological factors. [5] [8] [9] [10] [12] [13] [14] Scientists consider the OBE to be an experience from a mental state, like a dream or an altered state of consciousness without recourse to the paranormal. [41]

Charles Richet (1887) held that OBEs are created by the subject's memory and imagination processes and are no different from dreams. [42] [43] James H. Hyslop (1912) wrote that OBEs occur when the activity of the subconscious mind dramatizes certain images to give the impression the subject is in a different physical location. [44] Eugéne Osty (1930) considered OBEs to be nothing more than the product of imagination. [45] Other early researchers (such as Schmeing, 1938) supported psychophysiological theories. [46] G. N. M. Tyrrell interpreted OBEs as hallucinatory constructs relating to subconscious levels of personality. [47]

Donovan Rawcliffe (1959) connected the OBE experience with psychosis and hysteria. [48] Other researchers have discussed the phenomena of the OBE in terms of a distortion of the body image (Horowitz, 1970) and depersonalization (Whitlock, 1978). [49] [50] The psychologists Nandor Fodor (1959) and Jan Ehrenwald (1974) proposed that an OBE is a defense mechanism designed to deal with the threat of death. [51] [52] According to (Irin and Watt, 2007) Jan Ehrenwald had described the out-of-body experience (OBE) "as an imaginal confirmation of the quest for immortality, a delusory attempt to assure ourselves that we possess a soul that exists independently of the physical body". [53] The psychologists Donald Hebb (1960) and Cyril Burt (1968) wrote on the psychological interpretation of the OBE involving body image and visual imagery. [54] [55] Graham Reed (1974) suggested that the OBE is a stress reaction to a painful situation, such as the loss of love. [56] John Palmer (1978) wrote that the OBE is a response to a body image change causing a threat to personal identity. [57]

Carl Sagan (1977) and Barbara Honegger (1983) wrote that the OBE experience may be based on a rebirth fantasy or reliving of the birth process based on reports of tunnel-like passageways and a cord-like connection by some OBErs which they compared to an umbilical cord. [58] [59] Susan Blackmore (1978) came to the conclusion that the OBE is a hallucinatory fantasy as it has the characteristics of imaginary perceptions, perceptual distortions and fantasy-like perceptions of the self (such as having no body). [60] [61] Ronald Siegel (1980) also wrote that OBEs are hallucinatory fantasies. [62]

Harvey Irwin (1985) presented a theory of the OBE involving attentional cognitive processes and somatic sensory activity. His theory involved a cognitive personality construct known as psychological absorption and gave instances of the classification of an OBE as examples of autoscopy, depersonalization and mental dissociation. [38] The psychophysiologist Stephen Laberge (1985) has written that the explanation for OBEs can be found in lucid dreaming. [63] David Hufford (1989) linked the OBE experience with a phenomenon he described as a nightmare waking experience, a type of sleep paralysis. [64] Other scientists have also linked OBEs to cases of hypnagogia and sleep paralysis (cataplexy). [65] [66]

In case studies fantasy proneness has been shown to be higher among OBErs than those who have not had an OBE. [67] The data has shown a link between the OBE experience in some cases to fantasy prone personality (FPP). [68] In a case study involving 167 participants the findings revealed that those who claimed to have experienced the OBE were "more fantasy prone, higher in their belief in the paranormal and displayed greater somatoform dissociation." [69] Research from studies has also suggested that OBEs are related to cognitive-perceptual schizotypy. [70]

Neurological

Terence Hines (2003) has written that spontaneous out-of-body experiences can be generated by artificial stimulation of the brain and this strongly suggests that the OBE experience is caused from "temporary, minor brain malfunctions, not by the person's spirit (or whatever) actually leaving the body." [71] In a study review of neurological and neurocognitive data (Bünning and Blanke, 2005) wrote that OBEs are due to "functional disintegration of lower-level multisensory processing and abnormal higher-level self-processing at the temporoparietal junction." [72] Some scientists suspect that OBEs are the result of a mismatch between visual and tactile signals. [73] [74]

Richard Wiseman (2011) has noted that OBE research has focused on finding a psychological explanation and "out-of-body experiences are not paranormal and do not provide evidence for the soul. Instead, they reveal something far more remarkable about the everyday workings of your brain and body." [75] A study conducted by Jason Braithwaite and colleagues (2011) linked the OBE to "neural instabilities in the brain's temporal lobes and to errors in the body's sense of itself". [76] [77] Braithwaite et al. (2013) reported that the "current and dominant view is that the OBE occurs due to a temporary disruption in multi-sensory integration processes." [78] A study led by Josef Parvizi found that direct electrical stimulation of the anterior portion of the precuneus can induce an out-of-body experience. [23]

Paranormal

Writers in the fields of parapsychology and occultism have written that OBEs are not psychological and that a soul, spirit or subtle body can detach itself out of the body and visit distant locations. Out-of-the-body experiences were known during the Victorian period in spiritualist literature as "travelling clairvoyance". In old Indian scriptures, such a state of consciousness is also referred to as Turiya, which can be achieved by deep yogic and meditative activities, during which yogis may be liberated from the duality of mind and body, allowing them to intentionally leave the body and then return to it. The body carrying out this journey is called "Vigyan dehi" ("Scientific body"). The psychical researcher Frederic Myers referred to the OBE as a "psychical excursion". [79] An early study that described alleged cases of OBE was the two-volume Phantasms of the Living , published in 1886 by the psychical researchers Edmund Gurney, Myers, and Frank Podmore. The book was largely criticized by the scientific community because the anecdotal reports in almost every case lacked evidential substantiation. [80] [81]

A 19th-century illustration of Robert Blair's poem The Grave, depicting the soul leaving the body Schiavonetti Soul leaving body 1808.jpg
A 19th-century illustration of Robert Blair's poem The Grave , depicting the soul leaving the body

The theosophist Arthur Powell (1927) was an early author to advocate the subtle body theory of OBEs. [82] Sylvan Muldoon (1936) embraced the concept of an etheric body to explain the OBE experience. [83] The psychical researcher Ernesto Bozzano (1938) had also supported a similar view describing the phenomena of the OBE experience in terms of bilocation in which an "etheric body" can release itself from the physical body in rare circumstances. [84] The subtle body theory was also supported by occult writers such as Ralph Shirley (1938), Benjamin Walker (1977), and Douglas Baker (1979). [85] [86] [87] James Baker (1954) wrote that a mental body enters an "intercosmic region" during the OBE. [88] Robert Crookall supported the subtle body theory of OBEs in several publications. [89] [90]

The paranormal interpretation of OBEs has not been supported by all researchers within the study of parapsychology. Gardner Murphy (1961) wrote that OBEs are "not very far from the known terrain of general psychology, which we are beginning to understand more and more without recourse to the paranormal". [91]

In the 1970s, Karlis Osis conducted many OBE experiments with the psychic Alex Tanous. In one series of these experiments, he was asked whilst in an OBE state whether he could identify coloured targets that were placed in remote locations. Osis reported that there were 114 hits in 197 trials. However, the controls for the experiments have been criticized and, according to Susan Blackmore, the final result was not particularly significant since 108 hits would have been expected by chance alone. Blackmore noted that the results provide "no evidence for accurate perception in the OBE". [92]

In April 1977, a patient from Harborview Medical Center known as Maria claimed to have experienced an out-of-body experience. During her OBE she claimed to have floated outside her body and outside the hospital. Maria later told her social worker Kimberly Clark that during the OBE she had observed a tennis shoe on the third floor window ledge to the north side of the building. Clark then went to the north wing of the building and by looking out of the window could see a tennis shoe on one of the ledges. Clark published the account in 1984. The story has since been used in many paranormal books as evidence that a spirit can leave the body. [93] [94]

In 1996, Hayden Ebbern, Sean Mulligan and Barry Beyerstein visited the Medical Center to investigate Clark's story. They placed a tennis shoe on the same ledge and found that it was visible from within the building and could easily have been observed by a patient lying in bed. They also discovered that the tennis shoe was easy to observe from outside the building and suggested that Maria may have overheard a comment about it during her three days in the hospital and then incorporated it into her OBE. They concluded "Maria's story merely reveals the naiveté and the power of wishful thinking" from OBE researchers seeking a paranormal explanation. [95] Clark did not publish the description of the case until seven years after it happened, casting doubt on the story. Richard Wiseman has said that although the story is not evidence for anything paranormal it has been "endlessly repeated by writers who either couldn't be bothered to check the facts, or were unwilling to present their readers with the more skeptical side of the story." [94] Clark responded to the accusations made in a separate paper. [96]

Astral projection

Astral projection is a paranormal interpretation of out-of-body experiences that assumes the existence of one or more non-physical planes of existence and an associated body beyond the physical. Commonly such planes are called astral, etheric , or spiritual . Astral projection is often experienced as the spirit or astral body leaving the physical body to travel in the spirit world or astral plane. [97]

OBE studies

Early collections of OBE cases had been made by Ernesto Bozzano (Italy) and Robert Crookall (UK). Crookall approached the subject from a spiritualistic position, and collected his cases predominantly from spiritualist newspapers such as the Psychic News , which appears to have biased his results in various ways. For example, the majority of his subjects reported seeing a cord connecting the physical body and its observing counterpart; whereas Green (see below) found that less than 4% of her subjects noticed anything of this sort, and some 80% reported feeling they were a "disembodied consciousness", with no external body at all.

The first extensive scientific study of OBEs was made by Celia Green (1968). [2] She collected written, first-hand accounts from a total of 400 subjects, recruited by means of appeals in the mainstream media, and followed up by questionnaires. Her purpose was to provide a taxonomy of the different types of OBE, viewed simply as an anomalous perceptual experience or hallucination, while leaving open the question of whether some of the cases might incorporate information derived by extrasensory perception.

International Academy of Consciousness - Global Survey

In 1999, at the 1st International Forum of Consciousness Research in Barcelona, research-practitioners Wagner Alegretti and Nanci Trivellato presented preliminary findings of an online survey on the out-of-body experience answered by internet users interested in the subject; therefore, not a sample representative of the general population. [98]

1,007 (85%) of the first 1,185 respondents reported having had an OBE. 37% claimed to have had between two and ten OBEs. 5.5% claimed more than 100 such experiences. 45% of those who reported an OBE said they successfully induced at least one OBE by using a specific technique. 62% of participants claiming to have had an OBE also reported having enjoyed nonphysical flight; 40% reported experiencing the phenomenon of self-bilocation (i.e. seeing one's own physical body whilst outside the body); and 38% claimed having experienced self-permeability (passing through physical objects such as walls). The most commonly reported sensations experienced in connection with the OBE were falling, floating, repercussions e.g. myoclonia (the jerking of limbs, jerking awake), sinking, torpidity (numbness), intracranial sounds, tingling, clairvoyance, oscillation and serenity.

Another reported common sensation related to OBE was temporary or projective catalepsy, a more common feature of sleep paralysis. The sleep paralysis and OBE correlation was later corroborated by the Out-of-Body Experience and Arousal study published in Neurology by Kevin Nelson and his colleagues from the University of Kentucky in 2007. [99] The study discovered that people who have out-of-body experiences are more likely to experience sleep paralysis. [100]

Also noteworthy, is the Waterloo Unusual Sleep Experiences Questionnaire [101] that further illustrates the correlation.

Miss Z study

In 1968, Charles Tart conducted an OBE experiment with a subject known as Miss Z for four nights in his sleep laboratory. The subject was attached to an EEG machine and a five-digit code was placed on a shelf above her bed. She did not claim to see the number on the first three nights but on the fourth gave the number correctly. [102] [103] The psychologist James Alcock criticized the experiment for inadequate controls and questioned why the subject was not visually monitored by a video camera. [104] Martin Gardner has written the experiment was not evidence for an OBE and suggested that whilst Tart was "snoring behind the window, Miss Z simply stood up in bed, without detaching the electrodes, and peeked." [105] Susan Blackmore wrote "If Miss Z had tried to climb up, the brain-wave record would have showed a pattern of interference. And that was exactly what it did show." [106]

Neurology and OBE-like experiences

There are several possible physiological explanations for parts of the OBE. OBE-like experiences have been induced by stimulation of the brain. OBE-like experience has also been induced through stimulation of the posterior part of the right superior temporal gyrus in a patient. [107] Positron-emission tomography was also used in this study to identify brain regions affected by this stimulation. The term OBE-like is used above because the experiences described in these experiments either lacked some of the clarity or details of normal OBEs, or were described by subjects who had never experienced an OBE before. Such subjects were therefore not qualified to make claims about the authenticity of the experimentally-induced OBE.

British psychologist Susan Blackmore and others suggest that an OBE begins when a person loses contact with sensory input from the body while remaining conscious. [108] The person retains the illusion of having a body, but that perception is no longer derived from the senses. The perceived world may resemble the world he or she generally inhabits while awake, but this perception does not come from the senses either. The vivid body and world is made by our brain's ability to create fully convincing realms, even in the absence of sensory information. This process is witnessed by each of us every night in our dreams, though OBEs are claimed to be far more vivid than even a lucid dream.

Irwin [38] pointed out that OBEs appear to occur under conditions of either very high or very low arousal. For example, Green [2] found that three quarters of a group of 176 subjects reporting a single OBE were lying down at the time of the experience, and of these 12% considered they had been asleep when it started. By contrast, a substantial minority of her cases occurred under conditions of maximum arousal, such as a rock-climbing fall, a traffic accident, or childbirth. McCreery [109] [110] has suggested that this paradox may be explained by reference to the fact that sleep can supervene as a reaction to extreme stress or hyper-arousal. [111] He proposes that OBEs under both conditions, relaxation and hyper-arousal, represent a form of "waking dream", or the intrusion of Stage 1 sleep processes into waking consciousness.

Olaf Blanke studies

Research by Olaf Blanke in Switzerland found that it is possible to reliably elicit experiences somewhat similar to the OBE by stimulating regions of the brain called the right temporoparietal junction (TPJ; a region where the temporal lobe and the parietal lobe of the brain come together). Blanke and his collaborators in Switzerland have explored the neural basis of OBEs by showing that they are reliably associated with lesions in the right TPJ region [112] and that they can be reliably elicited with electrical stimulation of this region in a patient with epilepsy. [113] These elicited experiences may include perceptions of transformations of the patient's arms and legs (complex somatosensory responses) and whole-body displacements (vestibular responses). [114] [115]

In neurologically normal subjects, Blanke and colleagues then showed that the conscious experience of the self and body being in the same location depends on multisensory integration in the TPJ. Using event-related potentials, Blanke and colleagues showed the selective activation of the TPJ 330–400 ms after stimulus onset when healthy volunteers imagined themselves in the position and visual perspective that generally are reported by people experiencing spontaneous OBEs. Transcranial magnetic stimulation in the same subjects impaired mental transformation of the participant's own body. No such effects were found with stimulation of another site or for imagined spatial transformations of external objects, suggesting the selective implication of the TPJ in mental imagery of one's own body. [116]

In a follow-up study, Arzy et al. showed that the location and timing of brain activation depended on whether mental imagery is performed with mentally embodied or disembodied self location. When subjects performed mental imagery with an embodied location, there was increased activation of a region called the "extrastriate body area" (EBA), but when subjects performed mental imagery with a disembodied location, as reported in OBEs, there was increased activation in the region of the TPJ. This leads Arzy et al. to argue that "these data show that distributed brain activity at the EBA and TPJ as well as their timing are crucial for the coding of the self as embodied and as spatially situated within the human body." [117]

Blanke and colleagues thus propose that the right temporal-parietal junction is important for the sense of spatial location of the self, and that when these normal processes go awry, an OBE arises. [118]

In August 2007 Blanke's lab published research in Science demonstrating that conflicting visual-somatosensory input in virtual reality could disrupt the spatial unity between the self and the body. During multisensory conflict, participants felt as if a virtual body seen in front of them was their own body and mislocalized themselves toward the virtual body, to a position outside their bodily borders. This indicates that spatial unity and bodily self-consciousness can be studied experimentally and is based on multisensory and cognitive processing of bodily information. [119]

Ehrsson study

In August 2007, Henrik Ehrsson, then at the Institute of Neurology at University College of London (now at the Karolinska Institute in Sweden), published research in Science demonstrating the first experimental method that, according to the scientist's claims in the publication, induced an out-of-body experience in healthy participants. [120] The experiment was conducted in the following way:

The study participant sits in a chair wearing a pair of head-mounted video displays. These have two small screens over each eye, which show a live film recorded by two video cameras placed beside each other two metres behind the participant's head. The image from the left video camera is presented on the left-eye display and the image from the right camera on the right-eye display. The participant sees these as one "stereoscopic" (3D) image, so they see their own back displayed from the perspective of someone sitting behind them.

The researcher then stands just beside the participant (in their view) and uses two plastic rods to simultaneously touch the participant's actual chest out-of-view and the chest of the illusory body, moving this second rod towards where the illusory chest would be located, just below the camera's view.

The participants confirmed that they had experienced sitting behind their physical body and looking at it from that location. [73] [121]

Both critics and the experimenter himself note that the study fell short of replicating "full-blown" OBEs. As with previous experiments which induced sensations of floating outside of the body, Ehrsson's work does not explain how a brain malfunction might cause an OBE. Essentially, Ehrsson created an illusion that fits a definition of an OBE in which "a person who is awake sees his or her body from a location outside the physical body." [122]

Awareness during Resuscitation Study

In 2001, Sam Parnia and colleagues investigated out of body claims by placing figures on suspended boards facing the ceiling, not visible from the floor. Parnia wrote "anybody who claimed to have left their body and be near the ceiling during resuscitation attempts would be expected to identify those targets. If, however, such perceptions are psychological, then one would obviously not expect the targets to be identified." [123] The philosopher Keith Augustine, who examined Parnia's study, has written that all target identification experiments have produced negative results. [124] [125] Psychologist Chris French wrote regarding the study "unfortunately, and somewhat atypically, none of the survivors in this sample experienced an OBE." [126]

In the autumn of 2008, 25 UK and US hospitals began participation in a study, coordinated by Sam Parnia and Southampton University known as the AWARE study (AWAreness during REsuscitation). Following on from the work of Pim van Lommel in the Netherlands, the study aims to examine near-death experiences in 1,500 cardiac arrest survivors and so determine whether people without a heartbeat or brain activity can have documentable out-of-body experiences. [127] As part of the study Parnia and colleagues have investigated out of body claims by using hidden targets placed on shelves that could only be seen from above. [127] Parnia has written "if no one sees the pictures, it shows these experiences are illusions or false memories". [127]

In 2014 Parnia issued a statement indicating that the first phase of the project has been completed and the results are undergoing peer review for publication in a medical journal. [128] No subjects saw the images mounted out of sight according to Parnia's early report of the results of the study at an American Heart Association meeting in November 2013. Only two out of the 152 patients reported any visual experiences, and one of them described events that could be verified (as the other one's condition worsened before the detailed interview). [129] The two NDEs occurred in an area where "no visual targets had been placed". [130]

On October 6, 2014, the results of the study were published in the journal Resuscitation. Less than 20% of cardiac arrest patients were able to be interviewed, as most of them died or were too sick even after successful resuscitation. Among those who reported a perception of awareness and completed further interviews, 46% experienced a broad range of mental recollections in relation to death that were not compatible with the commonly used term of NDEs. These included fearful and persecutory experiences. Only 9% had experiences compatible with NDEs and 2% exhibited full awareness compatible with OBEs with explicit recall of 'seeing' and 'hearing' events. One case was validated and timed using auditory stimuli during cardiac arrest. [131] According to Caroline Watt "The one 'verifiable period of conscious awareness' that Parnia was able to report did not relate to this objective test. Rather, it was a patient giving a supposedly accurate report of events during his resuscitation. He didn't identify the pictures, he described the defibrillator machine noise. But that's not very impressive since many people know what goes on in an emergency room setting from seeing recreations on television." [132] [133] However, it was impossible for him to describe any hidden targets, as there were none in the room where his OBE occurred, and the rest of his description was also very precise, including the description and later correct identification of a doctor who took part in his resuscitation.

AWARE Study II

As of May 2016, a posting at the UK Clinical Trials Gateway website describes plans for AWARE II, a two-year multicenter observational study of 900-1500 patients experiencing cardiac arrest, with subjects being recruited starting on 1 August 2014 and that the scheduled end date was 31 May 2017. [134] The study was extended, continuing until 2020. [135] Results have been published in 2023. [136]

Smith & Messier

In 2014, a functional imaging study reported the case of a woman who could experience out of body experience at will. She reported developing the ability as a child and associated it with difficulties in falling sleep. Her OBEs continued into adulthood but became less frequent. She was able to see herself rotating in the air above her body, lying flat, and rolling in the horizontal plane. She reported sometimes watching herself move from above but remained aware of her unmoving "real" body. The participant reported no particular emotions linked to the experience. "[T]he brain functional changes associated with the reported extra-corporeal experience (ECE) were different than those observed in motor imagery. Activations were mainly left-sided and involved the left supplementary motor area and supramarginal and posterior superior temporal gyri, the last two overlapping with the temporal parietal junction that has been associated with out-of-body experiences. The cerebellum also showed activation that is consistent with the participant's report of the impression of movement during the ECE. There was also left middle and superior orbital frontal gyri activity, regions often associated with action monitoring." [137]

OBE training and research facilities

The Monroe Institute's Nancy Penn Center is a facility specializing in out-of-body experience induction. The Center for Higher Studies of the Consciousness in Brazil is another large OBE training facility. Olaf Blanke's Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience has become a well-known laboratory for OBE research. [138]

See also

Notes

  1. G. N. M. Tyrrell, Apparitions, Gerald Duckworth and Co. Ltd, London, 1943, pp. 149. ISBN   978-1169831537
  2. 1 2 3 Green, C.E. (1968). Out-of-the-body Experiences. London: Hamish Hamilton. ISBN   978-0345248435.
  3. Monroe, Robert (1971). Journeys Out of the Body . Harmony/Rodale. ISBN   0-385-00861-9.
  4. Aspell, Jane; Blanke, Olaf (2009). "Understanding the out-of-body experience from a neuroscientific perspective" (PDF). In Murray, Craig D. (ed.). Psychological Scientific Perspectives on Out of Body and Near Death Experiences. Psychology Research Progress. New York: Nova Science Publishers. ISBN   978-1-60741-705-7.
  5. 1 2 3 Brent, S. B. (1979). "Deliberately induced, premortem, out-of-body experiences: An experimental and theoretical approach". In Kastenbaum, B. (ed.). Between life and death. New York: Springer. pp. 89–123. ISBN   978-0826125408.
  6. Blackmore, Susan (1984). "A Postal Survey of OBEs and Other Experiences".
  7. "(Aug. 24, 2007) First Out-of-body Experience Induced In Laboratory Setting". ScienceDaily. August 24, 2007. Retrieved October 6, 2011.
  8. 1 2 Gabbard, G. O., & Twemlow, A. W. (1984). With the eyes of the mind: An empirical analysis of out-of-body states. New York: Praeger Scientific. ISBN   978-0030689260
  9. 1 2 Leonard Zusne, Warren H. Jones (1989). Anomalistic Psychology: A Study of Magical Thinking. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. ISBN   0-8058-0508-7
  10. 1 2 Blanke O, Landis T, Seeck M (2004). "Out-of-body experience and autoscopy of neurological origin". Brain. 127 (2): 243–258. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh040 . PMID   14662516.
  11. Blanke O, Mohr C (2005). "Out-of-body experience, heautoscopy, and autoscopic hallucination of neurological origin. Implications for mechanisms of corporeal awareness and self consciousness". Brain Research Reviews. 50 (1): 184–199. doi:10.1016/j.brainresrev.2005.05.008. PMID   16019077. S2CID   10376314.
  12. 1 2 Meyerson, Joseph; Gelkopf, Marc (2004). "Therapeutic Utilization of Spontaneous Out-of-Body Experiences in Hypnotherapy". American Journal of Psychotherapy. 58 (1): 90–102. doi: 10.1176/appi.psychotherapy.2004.58.1.90 . PMID   15106402.[ permanent dead link ]
  13. 1 2 Cheyne, James Allan (Fall 2008). "When Is an OBE Not an OBE? A New Look at Out-of-Body Experiences". Skeptic.[ permanent dead link ]
  14. 1 2 Blanke, Olaf (December 2004). "Out Of Body Experiences And Their Neural Basis: They Are Linked To Multisensory And Cognitive Processing In The Brain". British Medical Journal. 329 (7480): 1414–1415. doi:10.1136/bmj.329.7480.1414. JSTOR   25469629. PMC   535951 . PMID   15604159. The reviewed evidence from neurological patients experiencing this striking dissociation between self and body shows that out of body experiences are culturally invariant phenomena that can be investigated scientifically.
  15. "SOBEs". Oberf.org. Retrieved October 6, 2011.
  16. Greyson Bruce (2003). "Near-Death Experiences in a Psychiatric Outpatient Clinic Population". Psychiatr Serv. 54 (12): 1649–1651. doi:10.1176/appi.ps.54.12.1649. PMID   14645808.
  17. Mauro, James. (1992). Bright lights, big Mystery. Psychology Today.
  18. Metzinger (citing Alvarado), 2005 "Out-of-Body Experiences as the Origin of the Concept of a 'Soul'", Mind & Matter 3(1), p. 65.
  19. Siegel Ronald (1980). "The Psychology of Life after Death". American Psychologist. 35 (10): 911–931. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.35.10.911. PMID   7436117.
  20. Bressloff PC, Cowan JD, Golubitsky M, Thomas PJ, Wiener M (2002). "What geometric visual hallucinations tell us about the visual cortex". Neural Computation. 14 (3): 473–491. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.146.572 . doi:10.1162/089976602317250861. PMID   11860679. S2CID   207683037.
  21. Shermer, Michael B. (2002) [1997]. Why People Believe Weird Things: Pseudoscience, Superstition, and Other Confusions of Our Time (Revised & enlarged ed.). New York: Henry Holt and Company. p. 80. ISBN   978-0-8050-7089-7.
  22. Hines, Terence (2003). Pseudoscience and the Paranormal (2nd ed.). Amherst, NY: Prometheus Books. pp. 102–103. ISBN   978-1-57392-979-0.
  23. 1 2 Jon Hamilton (August 4, 2023). "This sausage-shaped part of your brain causes out-of-body experiences". Short Wave. NPR.
  24. Frederick Aardema (2012). "Focus 10: Mind Awake/Body Asleep". Archived from the original on August 12, 2007. Retrieved June 18, 2012.
  25. Pre-Grams of Tomorrow dreams as pathway to a New World Perspective: Forrer, Kurt
  26. Hereward Carrington, Sylvan Muldoon. (1981). The Projection Of The Astral Body. Weiser Books. ISBN   978-0-87728-069-9
  27. Monroe, Robert (1972). Journeys out of the Body. Souvenir. pp. 207–210. ISBN   0-285-62753-8.
  28. Bruce, Robert (2009). Astral Dynamics: The Complete Book of Out-of-Body Experiences. Hampton Roads Publishing Company. pp. 208–9. ISBN   978-1-57174-616-0.
  29. Frederick Aardema (2012). "The Vigil Method". Explorations In Consciousness: A New Approach To Out-of-Body Experiences. Archived from the original on March 21, 2018. Retrieved June 27, 2012.
  30. Ophiel (1974). The Art and Practice of Astral Projection . Weiser Books. ISBN   978-0-87728-246-4.
  31. Sadigh, M. R.; Kozicky, P. W. "The Effects of Hemi-Sync® on Electrocortical Activity: A Review of Three Empirical Studies". Archived from the original on March 29, 2017. Retrieved March 29, 2017.
  32. Campbell, Thomas (2007). My Big TOE . Lightning Strike Books. pp.  75, 79. ISBN   978-0-9725094-6-6.
  33. Buhlman, William (2001). The Secret of the Soul. Harper Collins. p. 198. ISBN   978-0-06-251671-8.
  34. Bruce, Robert (2009). Astral Dynamics: The Complete Book of Out-of-Body Experiences. Hampton Roads Publishing Company. p. 164. ISBN   978-1-57174-616-0.
  35. Haven, Janine. (2006). At a Glance' Religious and Spiritual Competency for Psychotherapists. AuthorHouse. ISBN   978-1425906849
  36. Cheyne J. A.; Girard T. A. (2009). "The body unbound: vestibular-motor hallucinations and out-of-body experiences". Cortex. 45 (2): 201–215. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2007.05.002. PMID   18621363. S2CID   7501305.
  37. Nicholls, Graham. (2012). Navigating the Out-of-Body Experience: Radical New Techniques. Llewellyn Publications. ISBN   978-0738727615
  38. 1 2 3 Irwin, Harvey. (1985). Flight of Mind: A Psychological Study of the Out-Of-Body Experience. Metuchen, NJ: Scarecrow Press. ISBN   978-0810817371
  39. "Out of Body, Roger" . Retrieved December 12, 2014.
  40. "Creating The Illusion Of A Different Body". February 25, 2011. Retrieved December 12, 2014.
  41. Blackmore, Susan J. (2002). "Out-of-Body Experiences". In Shermer, Michael (ed.). The Skeptic Encyclopedia of Pseudoscience . ABC-CLIO. pp. 164–169. ISBN   978-1-57607-653-8.
  42. Richet, C. (1887). L 'homme et l'intelligence: Fragments de physiologie et de psychologie (2nd ed.). Paris: Felix Alcan.
  43. Richet, C. (1922). Traith de metapsychique. Paris: Felix Alcan.
  44. Hyslop JH (1912). "A review, a record and a discussion". Journal of the American Society for Psychical Research. 6: 490–516.
  45. Osty, E. (1930). La vision de soi. Revue metapsychique. No. 3, 185-197.
  46. Schmeing, K. (1938). Flugtraiume und "Exkursion des Ich. Archiv für die Gesamte Psychologie.
  47. Tyrrell, G. N. M. (1953). Apparitions. London: Gerald Duckworth. (Originally published, 1942.)
  48. Rawcliffe, D.H. (1959). Illusions and Delusions of the Supernatural and the Occult. New York, NY: Dover. (Original work published 1952).
  49. Horowitz, M. J. (1970). Image formation and cognition. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
  50. Whitlock FA (1978). "The psychiatry and psychopathology of paranormal phenomena". Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry. 12 (1): 11–19. doi:10.3109/00048677809159583. PMID   277171. S2CID   34344069.
  51. Fodor, N. (1959). The Haunted Mind. New York: Helix Press.
  52. Ehrenwald J (1974). "Out-of-the-body experiences and the denial of death". Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 159 (4): 227–233. doi:10.1097/00005053-197410000-00001. PMID   4422769. S2CID   26493164.
  53. Harvey J. Irwin, Caroline Watt An Introduction to Parapsychology 2007, p. 188
  54. Hebb DO (1960). "The American Revolution". American Psychologist. 15 (12): 735–745. doi:10.1037/h0043506.
  55. Burt, C. (1968). Psychology and Psychical Research. London: Society for Psychical Research.
  56. Reed, Graham. (1974). The Psychology of Anomalous Experience. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
  57. Palmer J (1978). "The out-of-body experience: A psychological theory". Parapsychology Review. 9 (5): 19–22.
  58. Sagan, C. (1977). Broca's Brain. Random House.
  59. Honegger, B. (1983). The OBE as a near-birth experience. In Roll, W. G., Beloff, J., and White, R. A. (Eds.), Research in Parapsychology. Scarecrow Press. pp. 230-231.
  60. Blackmore, Susan (1978). Parapsychology and out-of-the-body experiences. London: Transpersonal Books/Society for Psychical Research.
  61. Sheikh, Anees. (1983). Imagery: Current Theory, Research, and Application. John Wiley & Sons. p. 372. "Blackmore (1978) reviewed the evidence that indicates that out-of-the- body experiences have the following characteristics that can be expected of hallucinatory fantasies: (1) imaginary perceptions; (2) errors in perception; (3) perceptual distortions (such as seeing through things); (4) instantaneous traveling to distant locations; and (5) fantasy like perceptions of self such as not having a body, having a replica of one's body, and perceiving oneself as a point or a ball of light. She concluded from the data that out-of-the-body experiences should be viewed as hallucinatory fantasies."
  62. Siegel RK (1980). "The Psychology of Life After Death". American Psychologist. 35 (10): 911–931. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.35.10.911. PMID   7436117.
  63. LaBerge, S. (1985). Lucid Dreaming. Los Angeles: Jeremy P. Tarcher.
  64. Hufford, David. (1989) The Terror That Comes in the Night: An Experience-Centered Study of Supernatural Assault Traditions (Publications of the American Folklore Society). University of Pennsylvania Press. ISBN   978-0812213058
  65. Adler, Shelley. (2010) Sleep Paralysis: Night-mares, Nocebos, and the Mind-Body Connection (Studies in Medical Anthropology). Rutgers University Press. ISBN   978-0813548869.
  66. Mavromatis, Andreas. (2010). Hypnagogia: The Unique State of Consciousness Between Wakefulness and Sleep. Thyrsos Press. ISBN   978-0955305214
  67. Myers. S. A., Austrin, H. R., Grisso, J. T., & Nickeson, R. C. (1983). Personality Characteristics as related to the out-of-body experience. Journal of Parapsychology, 47. 131-144.
  68. Wilson, S. C., & Barber T. X. (1982). The fantasy-prone personality: Implications for understanding imagery, hypnosis, and parapsychological phenomena. In A. A. Sheikh (Ed.) Imagery: Current theory, Research and Application. New York: John Wiley.
  69. Gow, K., Lang, T. and Chant, D. (2004). Fantasy proneness, paranormal beliefs and personality features in out-of-body experiences. Contemp. Hypnosis, 21: 107–125.
  70. Parra, Alejandro. (2009). Out-of-Body Experiences and Hallucinatory Experiences: A Psychological Approach. Journal: Imagination, Cognition and Personality, vol. 29, no. 3, pp. 211-223
  71. Hines 2003 , pp. 104–106
  72. Bünning S., Blanke O. (2005). "The out-of body experience: precipitating factors and neural correlates". The Boundaries of Consciousness: Neurobiology and Neuropathology. Progress in Brain Research. Vol. 150. pp. 331–50. doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(05)50024-4. ISBN   9780444518514. PMID   16186034.
  73. 1 2 "Out-of-body experience recreated". BBC News. August 24, 2007. Retrieved May 20, 2010.
  74. Blakeslee, Sandra (October 3, 2006). "Out-of-Body Experience? Your Brain Is to Blame". The New York Times. Retrieved October 6, 2011.
  75. Wiseman, Richard (2011). Paranormality: Why We See What Isn't There. Macmillan. p. 60. ISBN   978-0-230-75298-6.
  76. Braithwaite Jason J.; Samson Dana; Apperly Ian; Broglia Emma; Hulleman Johan (2011). "Cognitive correlates of the spontaneous out-of-body experience (OBE) in the psychologically normal population: Evidence for an increased role of temporal-lobe instability, body-distortion processing, and impairments in own-body transformations". Cortex. 47 (7): 839–853. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2010.05.002. PMID   20598679. S2CID   206983737.
  77. "Out-of-body experiences linked to neural instability and biases in body representation". ScienceDaily . July 11, 2011.
  78. Braithwaite JJ, James K, Dewe H, Medford N, Takahashi C, Kessler K (2013). "Fractionating the unitary notion of dissociation: Disembodied but not embodied dissociative experiences are associated with exocentric perspective-taking". Frontiers in Neuroscience. 7: 719. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00719 . PMC   3812871 . PMID   24198776.
  79. Pearsall, Ronald J. (2004) [1972]. Table-Rappers: The Victorians and the Occult (New ed.). The History Press Ltd. p. 197. ISBN   978-0-75-093684-2.
  80. Innes, Alexander Taylor (1887). "Where Are the Letters? A Cross-Examination of Certain Phantasms". The Nineteenth Century . No. 22. pp. 174–194.
  81. Peirce, Charles Sanders (1958). Collected Papers of Charles Sanders Peirce, Volume 4. Harvard University Press. p. 360.
  82. Powell, Arthur (2010) [1927]. The Astral Body and Other Astral Phenomena. Kessinger Publishing. ISBN   978-1-162-57095-2.
  83. Muldoon, Sylvan (2010) [1936]. The Case for Astral Projection: Hallucination or Reality!. Kessinger Publishing. ISBN   978-1-162-73868-0.
  84. Bozzano, Ernesto (August 27, 1938). "Discarnate Influence in Human Life: A review of the case for spirit intervention". Nature . 142 (3591): 376–ff. doi:10.1038/142376c0. S2CID   4023701.
  85. Shirley, Ralph (2010) [1938]. The Mystery of the Human Double: The Case for Astral Projection. Kessinger Publishing. ISBN   978-1-162-73867-3.
  86. Walker, Benjamin (1977). Beyond the Body: Human Double and the Astral Planes. Routledge. ISBN   978-0-7100-8581-8.
  87. Baker, Douglas (1977). Practical Techniques of Astral Projection. Red Wheel Weiser. ISBN   978-0-87728-321-8.
  88. Baker, James Jr. (1954). The exteriorization of the mental body: A scientific interpretation of the out-of-the-body experience known as pneumakinesis. New York: The William-Frederick Press.
  89. Crookall, Robert (2011) [1961]. The Study and Practice of Astral Projection. Whitefish, MT: Literary Licensing. ISBN   978-1-258-00883-3.
  90. Crookall, Robert (1987) [1965]. Intimations of Immortality: 'Seeing' that leads to 'believing'. Cambridge, UK: James Clarke & Co Ltd. ISBN   978-0-227-67662-2.
  91. Murphy, Gardner (1979) [1961]. Challenge of Psychical Research: Primer of Parapsychology (New ed.). Praeger Publishers. ISBN   978-0-31320944-4.
  92. Blackmore, Susan J. (1992) [1983]. Beyond the Body: An Investigation of Out-of-the-Body Experiences (illustrated, reprint ed.). Academy Chicago Publishers. pp. 193–195. ISBN   978-0-89733-344-3.
  93. Clark, Kimberly (1984). "Clinical Interventions with Near-Death Experiencers". In Greyson, Bruce; Flynn, Charles P. (eds.). The Near-Death Experience: Problems, Prospects, Perspectives. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas Publishers. pp. 242–255. ISBN   978-0-398-05008-5.
  94. 1 2 Wiseman 2011 , pp. 44–45
  95. Ebbern, Hayden; Mulligan, Sean; Beyerstein, Barry (July–August 1996). "Maria's Near-Death Experience: Waiting for the Other Shoe to Drop" (PDF). Skeptical Inquirer . 20 (4): 27–33. Archived (PDF) from the original on September 26, 2020.
  96. Clark, Kimberley. "The Other Shoe Drops: Commentary on "Does Paranormal Perception Occur in Near-Death Experiences?"" (PDF). University of North Texas Libraries. Archived (PDF) from the original on July 25, 2018. Retrieved September 13, 2021.
  97. "Historical Terms Glossary – ASTRAL BODY". parapsych.org. Parapsychological Association. Archived from the original on February 19, 2003. Retrieved October 6, 2011.
  98. "preliminary findings". Out-of-body-experience.org. Archived from the original on September 25, 2008. Retrieved October 6, 2011.
  99. "Out-of-body experience and arousal". Neurology.org. March 6, 2007. Retrieved October 6, 2011.
  100. Highfield, Roger. (2007). What really happens in out of body experiences. The Telegraph.
  101. "Waterloo Unusual Sleep Experiences Questionnaire". Watarts.uwaterloo.ca. June 25, 1940. Archived from the original on October 11, 2011. Retrieved October 6, 2011.
  102. Zusne, Leonard; Jones, Warren. (1989). Anomalistic Psychology: A Study of Magical Thinking. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. p. 126. ISBN   0-8058-0508-7
  103. Carroll, Robert Todd (2003). The Skeptic's Dictionary: A Collection of Strange Beliefs, Amusing Deceptions, and Dangerous Delusions (Illustrated ed.). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons. p. 110. ISBN   978-0-471-27242-7.
  104. Alcock, James E. (1981). Parapsychology, Science or Magic?: A Psychological Perspective. Pergamon Press. pp. 130–131. ISBN   978-0-08-025773-0.
  105. Gardner, Martin (1989). How Not to Test a Psychic: Ten Years of Remarkable Experiments with Renowned Clairvoyant Pavel Stepanek. Prometheus Books. p. 246. ISBN   978-0-87975-512-6.
  106. Blackmore, Susan J. (1986). The Adventures of a Parapsychologist. Buffalo, NY: Prometheus Books. p. 176. ISBN   978-0-87975-360-3.
  107. De Ridder D, Van Laere K, Dupont P, Menovsky T, Van de Heyning P. Visualizing out-of-body experience in the brain. N Engl J Med. 2007 Nov 1;357(18):1829–33.
  108. Blackmore, Susan. (1992). Beyond the Body: An Investigation of Out-of-the-Body Experiences . Chicago: Academy Chicago Publishers.
  109. McCreery, C. (1997). "Hallucinations and arousability: pointers to a theory of psychosis". In Claridge, G. (ed.). Schizotypy, Implications for Illness and Health. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  110. McCreery, C. (2008). Dreams and psychosis: a new look at an old hypothesis (PDF). Psychological Paper No. 2008–1. Oxford: Oxford Forum. ISBN   978-09536772-83. Archived from the original (PDF) on February 4, 2019. Retrieved March 25, 2008.
  111. Oswald, I. (1962). Sleeping and Waking: Physiology and Psychology. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
  112. Blanke O.; Landis T.; Spinelli L.; Seeck M. (2004). "Out-of-body experience and autoscopy of neurological origin". Brain. 127 (2): 243–258. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh040 . PMID   14662516.
  113. Blanke, O.; Ortigue, S.; Landis, T.; Seeck, M. (2002). "Stimulating illusory own-body perceptions" (PDF). Nature. 419 (6904): 269–270. Bibcode:2002Natur.419..269B. doi:10.1038/419269a. PMID   12239558. S2CID   4427138. Archived from the original (PDF) on August 30, 2017. Retrieved August 25, 2014.
  114. Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience at Ecole Polytechnique Federale de Lausanne: http://lnco.epfl.ch/
  115. Jan Holden; Jeff Long; Jason MacLurg. "Out-of-Body Experiences: All in the Brain?". Vital Signs. 21 (3). Retrieved April 26, 2020 via International Association for Near-Death Studies.
  116. Blanke O, Mohr C, Michel CM, Pascual-Leone A, Brugger P, Seeck M, Landis T, Thut G (2005). "Linking out-of-body experience and self processing to mental own-body imagery at the temporoparietal junction". Journal of Neuroscience. 25 (3): 550–557. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2612-04.2005 . PMC   6725328 . PMID   15659590.
  117. Arzy, S.; Thut, G.; Mohr, C.; Michel, C.M.; Blanke, O. (2006). "Neural basis of embodiment: Distinct contributions of temporoparietal junction and extrastriate body area". Journal of Neuroscience. 26 (31): 8074–8081. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0745-06.2006. PMC   6673771 . PMID   16885221.
  118. Blanke, O.; Arzy, S. (2005). "The out-of-body experience: Disturbed self-processing at the temporo-parietal junction". Neuroscientist. 11 (1): 16–24. doi:10.1177/1073858404270885. PMID   15632275. S2CID   8172076.
  119. Lenggenhager, Bigna; Tadi, Tej; Metzinger, Thomas; Blanke, Olaf (2007). "Video Ergo Sum: Manipulating Bodily Self-Consciousness" (PDF). Science. 317 (5841): 1096–1099. Bibcode:2007Sci...317.1096L. doi:10.1126/science.1143439. PMID   17717189. S2CID   22240455. Archived (PDF) from the original on August 23, 2017.
  120. Ehrsson HH (2007). "The Experimental Induction of Out-of-Body Experiences". Science. 317 (5841): 1048. Bibcode:2007Sci...317.1048E. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.455.9381 . doi:10.1126/science.1142175. PMID   17717177. S2CID   18673240.
  121. First out-of-body experience induced in laboratory setting Archived June 30, 2017, at the Wayback Machine , August 23, 2007, EurekAlert!
  122. ""Out of Body. Be Back Soon.", EnlightenmentNext, Feb–April 2008". Enlightennext.org. Archived from the original on December 3, 2008. Retrieved October 6, 2011.
  123. Parnia S; Waller D. G; Yeates R; Fenwick P. (2001). "A Qualitative and Quantitative Study of the Incidence, Features and Aetiology of Near-Death Experiences in Cardiac Arrest Survivors". Resuscitation. 48 (2): 149–156. doi:10.1016/s0300-9572(00)00328-2. PMID   11426476.
  124. Keith Augustine. (2008). "Hallucinatory Near-Death Experiences". Internet Infidels. Retrieved 2014-06-03.
  125. Augustine, Keith (2019). "Near-Death Experiences are Not Evidence for Either Theism or Atheism" (PDF). Theism and Atheism: Opposing Arguments in Philosophy. Macmillan Reference USA. pp. 594–596. ISBN   978-0-0286-6445-3. Archived (PDF) from the original on October 31, 2020. Retrieved February 14, 2019.
  126. French, Chris (2005). "Near-Death Experiences in Cardiac Arrest Survivors". The Boundaries of Consciousness: Neurobiology and Neuropathology. Progress in Brain Research. Vol. 150. pp. 351–367. doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(05)50025-6. ISBN   9780444518514. PMID   16186035.
  127. 1 2 3 Jane Dreaper. (2008). "Study into near-death experiences". BBC News. Retrieved 2014-06-03.
  128. "AWARE Study Update 2014". Horizon Research Foundation. Archived from the original on March 7, 2014.
  129. Bowman, Lee (December 20, 2013). "Scientists looking closer at what happens when body dies; edge closer to new understanding". WEWS-TV. Scripps Howard News Service. Archived from the original on December 23, 2013. Retrieved May 24, 2014.
  130. Mays, Robert George; Mays, Suzanne B. (December 1, 2015). "Explaining Near-Death Experiences: Physical or Non-Physical Causation?" (PDF). Journal of Near-Death Studies. 33 (3): 225–149. doi:10.17514/JNDS-2015-33-3-p125-149.. ISSN   0891-4494. S2CID   55010580. Archived from the original (PDF) on March 8, 2021. Retrieved April 26, 2020.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: ignored DOI errors (link)
  131. Parnia et al. (2014). "AWARE—AWAreness during REsuscitation—A prospective study". Resuscitation.
  132. "One not too impressive study does not prove life after death". Doubtful News. October 7, 2014. Archived from the original on December 16, 2014.
  133. Hill, Sharon (October 8, 2014). "No, this study is not evidence for "life after death"". randi.org. James Randi Educational Foundation.
  134. UK Clinical Trials Gateway. Primary Trial ID Number 17129, entitled "AWARE II (AWAreness during REsuscitation) A Multi-Centre Observational Study of the Relationship between the Quality of Brain Resuscitation and Consciousness, Neurological, Functional and Cognitive Outcomes following Cardiac Arrest" Last updated May 3, 2016. Page archived May 9, 2016
  135. "AWARE NDE Study | Psi Encyclopedia". psi-encyclopedia.spr.ac.uk. Retrieved September 30, 2019.
  136. Parnia, Sam; Keshavarz Shirazi, Tara; Patel, Jignesh; Tran, Linh; Sinha, Niraj; O'Neill, Caitlin; Roellke, Emma; Mengotto, Amanda; Findlay, Shannon; McBrine, Michael; Spiegel, Rebecca; Tarpey, Thaddeus; Huppert, Elise; Jaffe, Ian; Gonzales, Anelly M. (July 7, 2023). "AWAreness during REsuscitation - II: A multi-center study of consciousness and awareness in cardiac arrest". Resuscitation. 191: 109903. doi: 10.1016/j.resuscitation.2023.109903 . ISSN   0300-9572. PMID   37423492.
  137. Smith, A. M.; Messier, C. (2014). "Voluntary Out-of-Body Experience: An fMRI Study". Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 8: 70. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00070 . PMC   3918960 . PMID   24575000.
  138. Laureys, Steven. (2005). The Boundaries of Consciousness: Neurobiology and Neuropathology, Volume 150 (Progress in Brain Research). Elsevier Science. ISBN   978-0444518514

Further reading

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Clairvoyance</span> Claimed form of extrasensory perception

Clairvoyance is the claimed ability to acquire information that would be considered impossible to get through scientifically proven sensations, thus classified as extrasensory perception, or "sixth sense". Any person who is claimed to have such ability is said to be a clairvoyant.

In the psychology subfield of oneirology, a lucid dream is a type of dream in which the dreamer realizes that they are dreaming while dreaming. The capacity to have lucid dreams is a trainable cognitive skill. During a lucid dream, the dreamer may gain some amount of volitional control over the dream characters, narrative, or environment, although this control of dream content is not the salient feature of lucid dreaming. An important distinction is that lucid dreaming is a distinct type of dream from other types of dreams such as prelucid dreams and vivid dreams, although prelucid dreams are a precursor to lucid dreams, and lucid dreams are often accompanied with enhanced dream vividness. Lucid dreams are also distinct state from other lucid boundary sleep states such as lucid hypnagogia or lucid hypnopompia.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Parapsychology</span> Study of paranormal and psychic phenomena

Parapsychology is the study of alleged psychic phenomena and other paranormal claims, for example, those related to near-death experiences, synchronicity, apparitional experiences, etc. Criticized as being a pseudoscience, the majority of mainstream scientists reject it. Parapsychology has also been criticised by mainstream critics for claims by many of its practitioners that their studies are plausible despite a lack of convincing evidence after more than a century of research for the existence of any psychic phenomena.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Ganzfeld experiment</span> Pseudoscientific test for extrasensory perception (ESP)

A ganzfeld experiment is an assessment used by parapsychologists that they contend can test for extrasensory perception (ESP) or telepathy. In these experiments, a "sender" attempts to mentally transmit an image to a "receiver" who is in a state of sensory deprivation. The receiver is normally asked to choose between a limited number of options for what the transmission was supposed to be and parapsychologists who propose that such telepathy is possible argue that rates of success above the expectation from randomness are evidence for ESP. Consistent, independent replication of ganzfeld experiments has not been achieved, and, in spite of strenuous arguments by parapsychologists to the contrary, there is no validated evidence accepted by the wider scientific community for the existence of any parapsychological phenomena. Ongoing parapsychology research using ganzfeld experiments has been criticized by independent reviewers as having the hallmarks of pseudoscience.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Susan Blackmore</span> British writer and academic (born 1951)

Susan Jane Blackmore is a British writer, lecturer, sceptic, broadcaster, and a visiting professor at the University of Plymouth. Her fields of research include memetics, parapsychology, consciousness, and she is best known for her book The Meme Machine. She has written or contributed to over 40 books and 60 scholarly articles and is a contributor to The Guardian newspaper.

Paranormal events are purported phenomena described in popular culture, folk, and other non-scientific bodies of knowledge, whose existence within these contexts is described as being beyond the scope of normal scientific understanding. Notable paranormal beliefs include those that pertain to extrasensory perception, spiritualism and the pseudosciences of ghost hunting, cryptozoology, and ufology.

An altered state of consciousness (ASC), also called an altered state of mind or mind alteration, is any condition which is significantly different from a normal waking state. By 1892, the expression was in use in relation to hypnosis, though there is an ongoing debate as to whether hypnosis is to be identified as an ASC according to its modern definition. The next retrievable instance, by Max Mailhouse from his 1904 presentation to conference, however, is unequivocally identified as such, as it was in relation to epilepsy, and is still used today. In academia, the expression was used as early as 1966 by Arnold M. Ludwig and brought into common usage from 1969 by Charles Tart. It describes induced changes in one's mental state, almost always temporary. A synonymous phrase is "altered state of awareness".

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Astral projection</span> Interpretation of out-of-body experiences

Astral projection is a term used in esotericism to describe an intentional out-of-body experience (OBE) that assumes the existence of a subtle body, known as the astral body or body of light, through which consciousness can function separately from the physical body and travel throughout the astral plane.

Robert Allan Monroe was an American radio broadcasting executive who became known for his ideas about altered states of consciousness and for founding The Monroe Institute which continues to promote those ideas. His 1971 book Journeys Out of the Body is credited with popularizing the term "out-of-body experience".

Hypnagogia is the transitional state from wakefulness to sleep, also defined as the waning state of consciousness during the onset of sleep. Its opposite state is described as hypnopompia – the transitional state from sleep into wakefulness. Mental phenomena that may occur during this "threshold consciousness" phase include hypnagogic hallucinations, lucid dreaming, and sleep paralysis.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Charles Tart</span> American psychologist and parapsychologist

Charles T. Tart is an American psychologist and parapsychologist known for his psychological work on the nature of consciousness, as one of the founders of the field of transpersonal psychology, and for his research in parapsychology.

Autoscopy is the experience in which an individual perceives the surrounding environment from a different perspective, from a position outside of their own body. Autoscopy comes from the ancient Greek autós and skopós.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Stanley Krippner</span>

Stanley Krippner is an American psychologist and parapsychologist. He received a B.S. degree from the University of Wisconsin–Madison in 1954 and M.A. (1957) and Ph.D. (1961) degrees from Northwestern University.

A near-death experience (NDE) is a profound personal experience associated with death or impending death, which researchers describe as having similar characteristics. When positive, which the great majority are, such experiences may encompass a variety of sensations including detachment from the body, feelings of levitation, total serenity, security, warmth, joy, the experience of absolute dissolution, review of major life events, the presence of a light, and seeing dead relatives. When negative, such experiences may include sensations of anguish, distress, a void, devastation, and seeing hellish imagery.

In psychology, anomalistic psychology is the study of human behaviour and experience connected with what is often called the paranormal, with few assumptions made about the validity of the reported phenomena.

Peter Brooke Cadogan Fenwick is a neuropsychiatrist and neurophysiologist who is known for his studies of epilepsy and end-of-life phenomena.

Sam Parnia is a British associate professor of Medicine at the NYU Langone Medical Center where he is also director of research into cardiopulmonary resuscitation. In the United Kingdom, he is director of the Human Consciousness Project at the University of Southampton. Parnia is known for his work on near-death experiences and cardiopulmonary resuscitation.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Consciousness after death</span> Common metaphysical theme in society and culture

Consciousness after death is a common theme in society and culture, and the belief in some form of life after death is a feature of many religions. However, scientific research has established that the physiological functioning of the brain, the cessation of which defines brain death, is closely connected to mental states.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Karlis Osis</span> Latvian parapsychologist

Karlis Osis was a Latvian-born parapsychologist who specialised in exploring deathbed phenomena and life after death.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Olaf Blanke</span> Swiss and German physician, neurologist and neuroscientist

Olaf Blanke is a Swiss and German physician, neurologist and neuroscientist. He holds the Bertarelli Foundation Chair in Cognitive Neuroprosthetics at the École Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne (EPFL). He directs the Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience at the Brain Mind Institute of EPFL and is professor of Neurology at Geneva University Hospitals. Blanke is known for his research on the neurological bases of self-consciousness and out-of-body experiences.