Paranormal

Last updated

Paranormal events are purported phenomena described in popular culture, folk, and other non-scientific bodies of knowledge, whose existence within these contexts is described as being beyond the scope of normal scientific understanding. [1] [2] [3] [4] Notable paranormal beliefs include those that pertain to extrasensory perception (for example, telepathy), spiritualism and the pseudosciences of ghost hunting, cryptozoology, and ufology. [5]

Contents

Proposals regarding the paranormal are different from scientific hypotheses or speculations extrapolated from scientific evidence because scientific ideas are grounded in empirical observations and experimental data gained through the scientific method. In contrast, those who argue for the existence of the paranormal explicitly do not base their arguments on empirical evidence but rather on anecdote, testimony, and suspicion. The standard scientific models give the explanation that what appears to be paranormal phenomena is usually a misinterpretation, misunderstanding, or anomalous variation of natural phenomena. [6] [7] [8]

Etymology

The term paranormal has existed in the English language since at least 1920. [9] [10] The word consists of two parts: para and normal. The definition implies that the scientific explanation of the world around us is normal and anything that is above, beyond, or contrary to that is para.

Paranormal subjects

On the classification of paranormal subjects, psychologist Terence Hines said in his book Pseudoscience and the Paranormal (2003):

The paranormal can best be thought of as a subset of pseudoscience. What sets the paranormal apart from other pseudosciences is a reliance on explanations for alleged phenomena that are well outside the bounds of established science. Thus, paranormal phenomena include extrasensory perception (ESP), telekinesis, ghosts, poltergeists, life after death, reincarnation, faith healing, human auras, and so forth. The explanations for these allied phenomena are phrased in vague terms of "psychic forces", "human energy fields", and so on. This is in contrast to many pseudoscientific explanations for other nonparanormal phenomena, which, although very bad science, are still couched in acceptable scientific terms. [11]

Ghost hunting

Ghost hunting is the investigation of locations that are reportedly haunted by ghosts. Typically, a ghost-hunting team will attempt to collect evidence supporting the existence of paranormal activity.

In traditional ghostlore, and fiction featuring ghosts, a ghost is a manifestation of the spirit or soul of a person. [12] Alternative theories expand on that idea and include belief in the ghosts of deceased animals. Sometimes the term "ghost" is used synonymously with any spirit or demon; [13] however, in popular usage the term typically refers to the spirit of a deceased person.

The belief in ghosts as souls of the departed is closely tied to the concept of animism, an ancient belief that attributed souls to everything in nature. [14] As the 19th-century anthropologist George Frazer explained in his classic work, The Golden Bough (1890), souls were seen as the 'creature within' which animated the body. [15] Although the human soul was sometimes symbolically or literally depicted in ancient cultures as a bird or other animal, it was widely held that the soul was an exact reproduction of the body in every feature, even down to the clothing worn by the person. This is depicted in artwork from various ancient cultures, including such works as the ancient Egyptian Book of the Dead (c.1550 BCE), which shows deceased people in the afterlife appearing much as they did before death, including the style of dress.

Ufology

The possibility of extraterrestrial life is not, in itself, a paranormal subject. Many scientists are actively engaged in the search for unicellular life within the Solar System, carrying out studies on the surface of Mars and examining meteors that have fallen to Earth. [16] Projects such as SETI are conducting an astronomical search for radio activity that would show evidence of intelligent life outside the Solar System. [17] Scientific theories of how life developed on Earth allow for the possibility that life also developed on other planets. The paranormal aspect of extraterrestrial life centers largely around the belief in unidentified flying objects (UFOs) and the phenomena said to be associated with them.

Early in the history of UFO culture, believers divided themselves into two camps. The first held a rather conservative view of the phenomena, interpreting them as unexplained occurrences that merited serious study. They began calling themselves "ufologists" in the 1950s and felt that logical analysis of sighting reports would validate the notion of extraterrestrial visitation. [14] [18]

The second camp held a view that coupled ideas of extraterrestrial visitation with beliefs from existing quasi-religious movements. Typically, these individuals were enthusiasts of occultism and the paranormal. Many had backgrounds as active Theosophists or spiritualists, or were followers of other esoteric doctrines. In contemporary times, many of these beliefs have coalesced into New Age spiritual movements. [14] [18]

Both secular and spiritual believers describe UFOs as having abilities beyond what are considered possible according to known aerodynamic constraints and physical laws. The transitory events surrounding many UFO sightings preclude any opportunity for the repeat testing required by the scientific method. Acceptance of UFO theories by the larger scientific community is further hindered by the many possible hoaxes associated with UFO culture. [19]

Cryptozoology

Cryptozoology is a pseudoscience and subculture that aims to prove the existence of entities from the folklore record, such as Bigfoot, chupacabras, or Mokele-mbembe. Cryptozoologists refer to these entities as cryptids, a term coined by the subculture.

Paranormal research

Approaching the paranormal from a research perspective is often difficult because of the lack of acceptable physical evidence from most of the purported phenomena. By definition, the paranormal does not conform to conventional expectations of nature. Therefore, a phenomenon cannot be confirmed as paranormal using the scientific method because, if it could be, it would no longer fit the definition. (However, confirmation would result in the phenomenon being reclassified as part of science.) Despite this problem, studies on the paranormal are periodically conducted by researchers from various disciplines. Some researchers simply study the beliefs in the paranormal regardless of whether the phenomena are considered to objectively exist. This section deals with various approaches to the paranormal: anecdotal, experimental, and participant-observer approaches and the skeptical investigation approach.

Anecdotal approach

Charles Fort, 1920. Fort is perhaps the most widely known collector of paranormal stories. Fort charles 1920.jpg
Charles Fort, 1920. Fort is perhaps the most widely known collector of paranormal stories.

An anecdotal approach to the paranormal involves the collection of stories told about the paranormal.

Charles Fort (1874–1932) is perhaps the best-known collector of paranormal anecdotes. Fort is said to have compiled as many as 40,000 notes on unexplained paranormal experiences, though there were no doubt many more. These notes came from what he called "the orthodox conventionality of Science", which were odd events originally reported in magazines and newspapers such as The Times and scientific journals such as Scientific American , Nature and Science . From this research Fort wrote seven books, though only four survive: The Book of the Damned (1919), New Lands (1923), Lo! (1931) and Wild Talents (1932); one book was written between New Lands and Lo!, but it was abandoned and absorbed into Lo!

Reported events that he collected include teleportation (a term Fort is generally credited with coining); poltergeist events; falls of frogs, fishes, and inorganic materials of an amazing range; crop circles; unaccountable noises and explosions; spontaneous fires; levitation; ball lightning (a term explicitly used by Fort); unidentified flying objects; mysterious appearances and disappearances; giant wheels of light in the oceans; and animals found outside their normal ranges (see phantom cat). He offered many reports of OOPArts, the abbreviation for "out of place" artifacts: strange items found in unlikely locations. He is perhaps the first person to explain strange human appearances and disappearances by the hypothesis of alien abduction and was an early proponent of the extraterrestrial hypothesis.

Fort is considered by many as the father of modern paranormalism, which is the study of the paranormal.

The magazine Fortean Times continues Charles Fort's approach, regularly reporting anecdotal accounts of the paranormal.

Such anecdotal collections, lacking the reproducibility of empirical evidence, are not amenable to scientific investigation. The anecdotal approach is not a scientific approach to the paranormal because it leaves verification dependent on the credibility of the party presenting the evidence. Nevertheless, it is a common approach to investigating paranormal phenomena.

Parapsychology

Participant of a Ganzfeld experiment which proponents say may show evidence of telepathy. Ganzfeld.jpg
Participant of a Ganzfeld experiment which proponents say may show evidence of telepathy.

Experimental investigation of the paranormal has been conducted by parapsychologists. J. B. Rhine popularized the now famous methodology of using card-guessing and dice-rolling experiments in a laboratory in the hopes of finding evidence of extrasensory perception. [20] However, it was revealed that Rhine's experiments contained methodological flaws and procedural errors. [21] [22] [23]

In 1957, the Parapsychological Association was formed as the preeminent society for parapsychologists. In 1969, they became affiliated with the American Association for the Advancement of Science. [24] Criticisms of the field were focused in the creation (in 1976) of the Committee for the Scientific Investigation of Claims of the Paranormal (now called the Committee for Skeptical Inquiry) and its periodical, the Skeptical Inquirer . [25] Eventually, more mainstream scientists became critical of parapsychology as an endeavor, and statements by the National Academies of Science and the National Science Foundation cast a pall on the claims of evidence for parapsychology. Today, many cite parapsychology as an example of a pseudoscience. [26] [27] Parapsychology has been criticized for continuing investigation despite being unable to provide convincing evidence for the existence of any psychic phenomena after more than a century of research. [28] [29]

By the 2000s, the status of paranormal research in the United States had greatly declined from its height in the 1970s, with the majority of work being privately funded and only a small amount of research being carried out in university laboratories. In 2007, Britain had a number of privately funded laboratories in university psychology departments. [30] Publication remained limited to a small number of niche journals, [30] and to date there have been no experimental results that have gained wide acceptance in the scientific community as valid evidence of the paranormal. [30]

Participant-observer approach

A ghost hunter taking an EMF reading, which proponents claim may be connected to paranormal activity Nightshot2.png
A ghost hunter taking an EMF reading, which proponents claim may be connected to paranormal activity

While parapsychologists look for quantitative evidence of the paranormal in laboratories, a great number of people immerse themselves in qualitative research through participant-observer approaches to the paranormal. Participant-observer methodologies have overlaps with other essentially qualitative approaches, including phenomenological research that seeks largely to describe subjects as they are experienced, rather than to explain them. [31] [ page needed ]

Participant observation suggests that by immersing oneself in the subject that is being studied, a researcher is presumed to gain understanding of the subject. Criticisms of participant observation as a data-gathering technique are similar to criticisms of other approaches to the paranormal, but also include an increased threat to the scientific objectivity of the researcher, unsystematic gathering of data, reliance on subjective measurement, and possible observer effects (i.e. observation may distort the observed behavior). [32] [ page needed ] Specific data-gathering methods, such as recording EMF (electromagnetic field) readings at haunted locations, have their own criticisms beyond those attributed to the participant-observer approach itself.

Participant observation, as an approach to the paranormal, has gained increased visibility and popularity through reality television programs like Ghost Hunters , and the formation of independent ghost hunting groups that advocate immersive research at alleged paranormal locations. One popular website for ghost hunting enthusiasts lists over 300 of these organizations throughout the United States and the United Kingdom. [33]

Skeptical scientific investigation

James Randi was a well-known investigator of paranormal claims. James Randi crop.jpg
James Randi was a well-known investigator of paranormal claims.

Scientific skeptics advocate critical investigation of claims of paranormal phenomena: applying the scientific method to reach a rational, scientific explanation of the phenomena to account for the paranormal claims, taking into account that alleged paranormal abilities and occurrences are sometimes hoaxes or misinterpretations of natural phenomena. A way of summarizing this method is by the application of Occam's razor, which suggests that the simpler solution is usually the correct one. [34]

The Committee for Skeptical Inquiry (CSI), formerly the Committee for the Scientific Investigation of Claims of the Paranormal (CSICOP), is an organization that aims to publicize the scientific, skeptical approach. It carries out investigations aimed at understanding paranormal reports in terms of scientific understanding, and publishes its results in the Skeptical Inquirer magazine.

CSI's Richard Wiseman draws attention to possible alternative explanations for perceived paranormal activity in his article, The Haunted Brain. While he recognizes that approximately 15% of people believe they have experienced an encounter with a ghost, he reports that only 1% report seeing a full-fledged ghost while the rest report strange sensory stimuli, such as seeing fleeting shadows or wisps of smoke, or the sensation of hearing footsteps or feeling a presence. Wiseman makes the claim that, rather than experiencing paranormal activity, it is activity within our own brains that creates these strange sensations. [7]

Michael Persinger proposed that ghostly experiences could be explained by stimulating the brain with weak magnetic fields. [7] Swedish psychologist Pehr Granqvist and his team, attempting to replicate Persinger's research, determined that the paranormal sensations experienced by Persinger's subjects were merely the result of suggestion, and that brain stimulation with magnetic fields did not result in ghostly experiences. [7]

Oxford University Justin Barrett has theorized that "agency"—being able to figure out why people do what they do—is so important in everyday life, that it is natural for our brains to work too hard at it, thereby detecting human or ghost-like behavior in everyday meaningless stimuli. [7]

James Randi, an investigator with a background in illusion, felt that the simplest explanation for those claiming paranormal abilities is often trickery, illustrated by demonstrating that the spoon bending abilities of psychic Uri Geller can easily be duplicated by trained stage magicians. [35] [36] He was also the founder of the James Randi Educational Foundation and its million dollar challenge that offered a prize of US$1,000,000 to anyone who could demonstrate evidence of any paranormal, supernatural or occult power or event, under test conditions agreed to by both parties. [37] Despite many declarations of supernatural ability, the prize was never claimed.

Psychology

In "anomalistic psychology", paranormal phenomena have naturalistic explanations resulting from psychological and physical factors which have sometimes given the impression of paranormal activity to some people, in fact, where there have been none. [38] The psychologist David Marks wrote that paranormal phenomena can be explained by magical thinking, mental imagery, subjective validation, coincidence, hidden causes, and fraud. [6] According to studies some people tend to hold paranormal beliefs because they possess psychological traits that make them more likely to misattribute paranormal causation to normal experiences. [39] [40] Research has also discovered that cognitive bias is a factor underlying paranormal belief. [41] [42]

Chris French founder of the Anomalistic Psychology Research Unit. Chris French World Skeptics Congress Berlin 2012.jpg
Chris French founder of the Anomalistic Psychology Research Unit.

Many studies have found a link between personality and psychopathology variables correlating with paranormal belief. [43] [44] [45] Some studies have also shown that fantasy proneness correlates positively with paranormal belief. [46]

Bainbridge (1978) and Wuthnow (1976) found that the most susceptible people to paranormal belief are those who are poorly educated, unemployed or have roles that rank low among social values. The alienation of these people due to their status in society is said to encourage them to appeal to paranormal or magical beliefs. [47] [48]

Research has associated paranormal belief with low cognitive ability, low IQ and a lack of science education. [49] [50] Intelligent and highly educated participants involved in surveys have proven to have less paranormal belief. [51] [52] [53] Tobacyk (1984) and Messer and Griggs (1989) discovered that college students with better grades have less belief in the paranormal. [54] [55]

In a case study (Gow, 2004) involving 167 participants the findings revealed that psychological absorption and dissociation were higher for believers in the paranormal. [56] Another study involving 100 students had revealed a positive correlation between paranormal belief and proneness to dissociation. [57] A study (Williams et al. 2007) discovered that "neuroticism is fundamental to individual differences in paranormal belief, while paranormal belief is independent of extraversion and psychoticism". [58] A correlation has been found between paranormal belief and irrational thinking. [59] [60]

In an experiment Wierzbicki (1985) reported a significant correlation between paranormal belief and the number of errors made on a syllogistic reasoning task, suggesting that believers in the paranormal have lower cognitive ability. [61] A relationship between narcissistic personality and paranormal belief was discovered in a study involving the Australian Sheep-Goat Scale. [62]

De Boer and Bierman wrote:

In his article 'Creative or Defective' Radin (2005) asserts that many academics explain the belief in the paranormal by using one of the three following hypotheses: Ignorance, deprivation or deficiency. 'The ignorance hypothesis asserts that people believe in the paranormal because they're uneducated or stupid. The deprivation hypothesis proposes that these beliefs exist to provide a way to cope in the face of psychological uncertainties and physical stressors. The deficiency hypothesis asserts that such beliefs arise because people are mentally defective in some way, ranging from low intelligence or poor critical thinking ability to a full-blown psychosis' (Radin). The deficiency hypothesis gets some support from the fact that the belief in the paranormal is an aspect of a schizotypical personality (Pizzagalli, Lehman and Brugger, 2001). [63]

A psychological study involving 174 members of the Society for Psychical Research completed a delusional ideation questionnaire and a deductive reasoning task. As predicted, the study showed that "individuals who reported a strong belief in the paranormal made more errors and displayed more delusional ideation than skeptical individuals". There was also a reasoning bias which was limited to people who reported a belief in, rather than experience of, paranormal phenomena. The results suggested that reasoning abnormalities may have a causal role in the formation of paranormal belief. [64]

Research has shown that people reporting contact with aliens have higher levels of absorption, dissociativity, fantasy proneness and tendency to hallucinate. [65]

Findings have shown in specific cases that paranormal belief acts as a psychodynamic coping function and serves as a mechanism for coping with stress. [66] Survivors from childhood sexual abuse, violent and unsettled home environments have reported to have higher levels of paranormal belief. [67] [68] A study of a random sample of 502 adults revealed paranormal experiences were common in the population which were linked to a history of childhood trauma and dissociative symptoms. [69] Research has also suggested that people who perceive themselves as having little control over their lives may develop paranormal beliefs to help provide an enhanced sense of control. [70]

Gender differences in surveys on paranormal belief have reported women scoring higher than men overall and men having greater belief in UFOs and extraterrestrials. [71] [72] Surveys have also investigated the relationship between ethnicity and paranormal belief. In a sample of American university students (Tobacyk et al. 1988) it was found that people of African descent have a higher level of belief in superstitions and witchcraft while belief in extraterrestrial life forms was stronger among people of European descent. [73] Otis and Kuo (1984) surveyed Singapore university students and found Chinese, Indian and Malay students to differ in their paranormal beliefs, with the Chinese students showing greater skepticism. [74]

According to American surveys analysed by (Bader et al. 2011) African Americans have the highest belief in the paranormal and while the findings are not uniform the "general trend is for whites to show lesser belief in most paranormal subjects". [75]

Polls show that about fifty percent of the United States population believe in the paranormal. Robert L. Park says a lot of people believe in it because they "want it to be so". [76]

A 2013 study that utilized a biological motion perception task discovered a "relation between illusory pattern perception and supernatural and paranormal beliefs and suggest that paranormal beliefs are strongly related to agency detection biases". [40]

A 2014 study discovered that schizophrenic patients have more belief in psi than healthy adults. [77]

Neuroscience

Some scientists have investigated possible neurocognitive processes underlying the formation of paranormal beliefs. [78] In a study (Pizzagalli et al. 2000) data demonstrated that "subjects differing in their declared belief in and experience with paranormal phenomena as well as in their schizotypal ideation, as determined by a standardized instrument, displayed differential brain electric activity during resting periods." [79] Another study (Schulter and Papousek, 2008) wrote that paranormal belief can be explained by patterns of functional hemispheric asymmetry that may be related to perturbations during fetal development. [80]

It was also realized that people with higher dopamine levels have the ability to find patterns and meanings where there are not any. This is why scientists have connected high dopamine levels with paranormal belief. [81]

Criticism

Some scientists have criticized the media for promoting paranormal claims. In a report by Singer and Benassi in 1981, they wrote that the media may account for much of the near universality of paranormal belief, as the public are constantly exposed to films, newspapers, documentaries and books endorsing paranormal claims while critical coverage is largely absent. [82] According to Paul Kurtz "In regard to the many talk shows that constantly deal with paranormal topics, the skeptical viewpoint is rarely heard; and when it is permitted to be expressed, it is usually sandbagged by the host or other guests." Kurtz described the popularity of public belief in the paranormal as a "quasi-religious phenomenon", a manifestation of a transcendental temptation, a tendency for people to seek a transcendental reality that cannot be known by using the methods of science. Kurtz compared this to a primitive form of magical thinking. [83]

Terence Hines has written that on a personal level, paranormal claims could be considered a form of consumer fraud as people are "being induced through false claims to spend their money—often large sums—on paranormal claims that do not deliver what they promise" and uncritical acceptance of paranormal belief systems can be damaging to society. [84]

Belief polls

While the existence of paranormal phenomena is controversial and debated passionately by both proponents of the paranormal and by skeptics, surveys are useful in determining the beliefs of people in regards to paranormal phenomena. These opinions, while not constituting scientific evidence for or against, may give an indication of the mindset of a certain portion of the population (at least among those who answered the polls). The number of people worldwide who believe in parapsychological powers has been estimated to be 3 to 4 billion. [85]

A survey conducted in 2006 by researchers from Australia's Monash University [86] sought to determine the types of phenomena that people claim to have experienced and the effects these experiences have had on their lives. The study was conducted as an online survey with over 2,000 respondents from around the world participating. The results revealed that around 70% of the respondents believe to have had an unexplained paranormal event that changed their life, mostly in a positive way. About 70% also claimed to have seen, heard, or been touched by an animal or person that they knew was not there; 80% have reported having a premonition, and almost 50% stated they recalled a previous life. [86]

Polls were conducted by Bryan Farha at Oklahoma City University and Gary Steward of the University of Central Oklahoma in 2006. They found fairly consistent results compared to the results of a Gallup poll in 2001. [87]

Percentage of U.S. citizens polled
PhenomenaFarha-Steward (2006) Gallup (2001)Gallup (2005) [88]
BeliefUnsureDisbeliefBeliefUnsureDisbeliefBeliefUnsureDisbelief
Psychic, Spiritual healing 56261854192655 [lower-alpha 1] 1726
ESP 283933502027412532
Haunted houses 402535421641371646
Demonic possession 40283241164142 [lower-alpha 2] 1344
Ghosts 392734381744321948
Telepathy 243442362635312742
Extraterrestrials visited Earth in the past 173449332738242451
Clairvoyance and Prophecy 243343322345262450
Mediumship 162955282646212355
Astrology 172657281852251955
Witches 261955261559211266
Reincarnation 152857252054202059

A survey by Jeffrey S. Levin, associate professor at Eastern Virginia Medical School, found that more than two thirds of the United States population reported having at least one mystical experience. [87] [89]

A 1996 Gallup poll estimated that 71% of the people in the U.S. believed that the government was covering up information about UFOs. A 2002 Roper poll conducted for the Sci Fi channel reported that 56% thought UFOs were real craft and 48% that aliens had visited the Earth. [87]

A 2001 National Science Foundation survey found that 9% of people polled thought astrology was very scientific, and 31% thought it was somewhat scientific. About 32% of Americans surveyed stated that some numbers were lucky, while 46% of Europeans agreed with that claim. About 60% of all people polled believed in some form of Extra-sensory perception and 30% thought that "some of the unidentified flying objects that have been reported are really space vehicles from other civilizations." [90]

In 2017 the Chapman University Survey of American Fears asked about seven paranormal beliefs and found that "the most common belief is that ancient advanced civilizations such as Atlantis once existed (55%). Next was that places can be haunted by spirits (52%), aliens have visited Earth in our ancient past (35%), aliens have come to Earth in modern times (26%), some people can move objects with their minds (25%), fortune tellers and psychics can survey the future (19%), and Bigfoot is a real creature. Only one-fourth of respondents didn't hold at least one of these beliefs." [91]

Paranormal challenges

In 1922, Scientific American offered two US$2,500 offers: (1) for the first authentic spirit photograph made under test conditions, and (2) for the first psychic to produce a "visible psychic manifestation". Harry Houdini was a member of the investigating committee. The first medium to be tested was George Valiantine, who claimed that in his presence spirits would speak through a trumpet that floated around a darkened room. For the test, Valiantine was placed in a room, the lights were extinguished, but unbeknownst to him his chair had been rigged to light a signal in an adjoining room if he ever left his seat. Because the light signals were tripped during his performance, Valiantine did not collect the award. [92] The last to be examined by Scientific American was Mina Crandon in 1924.

Since then, many individuals and groups have offered similar monetary awards for proof of the paranormal in an observed setting. These prizes have a combined value of over $2.4 million. [93]

The James Randi Educational Foundation offered a prize of a million dollars to a person who could prove that they had supernatural or paranormal abilities under appropriate test conditions. Several other skeptic groups also offer a monied prize for proof of the paranormal, including the largest group of paranormal investigators, the Independent Investigations Group, which has chapters in Hollywood; Atlanta; Denver; Washington, D.C.; Alberta, B.C.; and San Francisco. The IIG offers a $100,000 prize and a $5,000 finders fee if a claimant can prove a paranormal claim under 2 scientifically controlled tests. Founded in 2000 no claimant has passed the first (and lower odds) of the test. [94]

See also

Paranormal

By location

Authors

Skepticism

Notes

  1. Also includes the effect of placebo through "power of the human mind to heal the body". [88]
  2. Some may have taken this metaphorically. [88]

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Clairvoyance</span> Claimed form of extrasensory perception

Clairvoyance is the claimed ability to acquire information that would be considered impossible to get through scientifically proven sensations, thus classified as extrasensory perception, or "sixth sense". Any person who is claimed to have such ability is said to be a clairvoyant.

Extrasensory perception (ESP), also known as a sixth sense, or cryptaesthesia, is a claimed paranormal ability pertaining to reception of information not gained through the recognized physical senses, but sensed with the mind. The term was adopted by Duke University botanist J. B. Rhine to denote psychic abilities such as intuition, telepathy, psychometry, clairvoyance, clairaudience, clairsentience, empathy and their trans-temporal operation as precognition or retrocognition.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Parapsychology</span> Study of paranormal and psychic phenomena

Parapsychology is the study of alleged psychic phenomena and other paranormal claims, for example, those related to near-death experiences, synchronicity, apparitional experiences, etc. Criticized as being a pseudoscience, the majority of mainstream scientists reject it. Parapsychology has also been criticised by mainstream critics for claims by many of its practitioners that their studies are plausible despite a lack of convincing evidence after more than a century of research for the existence of any psychic phenomena.

Parapsychology is a field of research that studies a number of ostensible paranormal phenomena, including telepathy, precognition, clairvoyance, psychokinesis, near-death experiences, reincarnation, and apparitional experiences.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Society for Psychical Research</span> UK nonprofit organisation

The Society for Psychical Research (SPR) is a nonprofit organisation in the United Kingdom. Its stated purpose is to understand events and abilities commonly described as psychic or paranormal. It describes itself as the "first society to conduct organised scholarly research into human experiences that challenge contemporary scientific models." It does not, however, since its inception in 1882, hold any corporate opinions: SPR members assert a variety of beliefs with regard to the nature of the phenomena studied.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Telepathy</span> Psychic ability

Telepathy is the purported vicarious transmission of information from one person's mind to another's without using any known human sensory channels or physical interaction. The term was first coined in 1882 by the classical scholar Frederic W. H. Myers, a founder of the Society for Psychical Research (SPR), and has remained more popular than the earlier expression thought-transference.

Precognition is the purported psychic phenomenon of seeing, or otherwise becoming directly aware of, events in the future.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Psychic</span> Person claiming extrasensory perception abilities

A psychic is a person who claims to use powers rooted in parapsychology such as extrasensory perception (ESP) to identify information hidden from the normal senses, particularly involving telepathy or clairvoyance, or who performs acts that are apparently inexplicable by natural laws, such as psychokinesis or teleportation. Although many people believe in psychic abilities, the scientific consensus is that there is no proof of the existence of such powers, and describes the practice as pseudoscience. The word "psychic" is also used as an adjective to describe such abilities.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Ganzfeld experiment</span> Pseudoscientific test for extrasensory perception (ESP)

A ganzfeld experiment is an assessment used by parapsychologists that they contend can test for extrasensory perception (ESP) or telepathy. In these experiments, a "sender" attempts to mentally transmit an image to a "receiver" who is in a state of sensory deprivation. The receiver is normally asked to choose between a limited number of options for what the transmission was supposed to be and parapsychologists who propose that such telepathy is possible argue that rates of success above the expectation from randomness are evidence for ESP. Consistent, independent replication of ganzfeld experiments has not been achieved, and, in spite of strenuous arguments by parapsychologists to the contrary, there is no validated evidence accepted by the wider scientific community for the existence of any parapsychological phenomena. Ongoing parapsychology research using ganzfeld experiments has been criticized by independent reviewers as having the hallmarks of pseudoscience.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Remote viewing</span> Pseudoscientific concept

Remote viewing (RV) is the practice of seeking impressions about a distant or unseen subject, purportedly sensing with the mind. Typically a remote viewer is expected to give information about an object, event, person or location that is hidden from physical view and separated at some distance. Physicists Russell Targ and Harold Puthoff, parapsychology researchers at Stanford Research Institute (SRI), are generally credited with coining the term "remote viewing" to distinguish it from the closely related concept of clairvoyance. According to Targ, the term was first suggested by Ingo Swann in December 1971 during an experiment at the American Society for Psychical Research in New York City.

A debunker is a person or organization that exposes or discredits claims believed to be false, exaggerated, or pretentious. The term is often associated with skeptical investigation of controversial topics such as UFOs, claimed paranormal phenomena, cryptids, conspiracy theories, alternative medicine, religion, or exploratory or fringe areas of scientific or pseudoscientific research.

Dean Radin investigates phenomena in parapsychology. Following a bachelor and master's degree in electrical engineering and a PhD in educational psychology Radin worked at Bell Labs, as a researcher at Princeton University and the University of Edinburgh, and was a faculty member at University of Nevada, Las Vegas. He then became Chief Scientist at the Institute of Noetic Sciences (IONS) in Petaluma, California, USA, later becoming the president of the Parapsychological Association. He is also co-editor-in-chief of the journal Explore: The Journal of Science and Healing. Radin's ideas and work have been criticized by scientists and philosophers skeptical of paranormal claims. The review of Radin's first book, The Conscious Universe, that appeared in Nature charged that Radin ignored the known hoaxes in the field, made statistical errors and ignored plausible non-paranormal explanations for parapsychological data.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Chris French</span> Psychologist specialising in paranormal beliefs (born 1956)

Christopher (Chris) Charles French is a British psychologist who is prominent in the field of anomalistic psychology, with a focus on the psychology of paranormal beliefs and anomalous experiences. In addition to his academic activities, French frequently appears on radio and television to provide a skeptical perspective on paranormal claims.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Russell Targ</span> American physicist, parapsychologist, and author

Russell Targ is an American physicist, parapsychologist, and author who is best known for his work on remote viewing.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Stanley Krippner</span>

Stanley Krippner is an American psychologist and parapsychologist. He received a B.S. degree from the University of Wisconsin–Madison in 1954 and M.A. (1957) and Ph.D. (1961) degrees from Northwestern University.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Ghost hunting</span> Investigating reportedly haunted locations for ghosts

Ghost hunting is the process of investigating locations that are purportedly haunted by ghosts. Typically, a ghost-hunting team will attempt to collect evidence supporting the existence of paranormal activity.

Erlendur Haraldsson was a professor emeritus of psychology on the faculty of social science at the University of Iceland. He published in various psychology and psychiatry journals. In addition, he published parapsychology books and authored a number of papers for parapsychology journals.

<i>Flim-Flam!</i> 1980 book by James Randi

Flim-Flam! Psychics, ESP, Unicorns, and Other Delusions is a 1980 book by magician and skeptic James Randi about paranormal, occult, and pseudoscience claims. The foreword is by science fiction author Isaac Asimov. Randi explores topics which he says that scientists and the media are too willing to promote without skepticism and proper expertise.

In psychology, anomalistic psychology is the study of human behaviour and experience connected with what is often called the paranormal, with few assumptions made about the validity of the reported phenomena.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Telekinesis</span> Influencing of objects without physical interaction

Telekinesis is a hypothetical psychic ability allowing an individual to influence a physical system without physical interaction. Experiments to prove the existence of telekinesis have historically been criticized for lack of proper controls and repeatability. There is no reliable evidence that telekinesis is a real phenomenon, and the topic is generally regarded as pseudoscience.

References

  1. "Belief in the Paranormal or Pseudoscience". Nsf.gov. Archived from the original on 4 February 2012. Retrieved 7 March 2012.
  2. "Paranormal". The Free Dictionary. Retrieved 3 February 2008.
  3. "Paranormal". Dictionary.com . Ask.com.
  4. "Paranormal". Merriam-Webster Dictionary . Merriam-Webster. August 2023.
  5. Gordon, Stuart (1993). The Paranormal: An Illustrated Encyclopedia. Trafalgar Square. ISBN   978-0-7472-3603-0.
  6. 1 2 Marks, D. F. (April 1988). "The Psychology of Paranormal Beliefs". Experientia. 44 (4): 332–7. doi:10.1007/BF01961272. PMID   3282908. S2CID   20803932.
  7. 1 2 3 4 5 Richard Wiseman (2011). "The Haunted Brain". www.csicop.org. Retrieved 7 January 2019.
  8. Schmaltz, Rodney M.; Lilienfeld, Scott O. (17 April 2014). "Hauntings, Homeopathy, and the Hopkinsville Goblins: Using Pseudoscience to Teach Scientific Thinking". Frontiers in Psychology . 5: 336. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00336 . PMC   4028994 . PMID   24860520.
  9. "Paranormal". In odisha
  10. "Paranormal". In Dictionary.com.
  11. Hines, Terence (2003). Pseudoscience and the Paranormal. Prometheus Books. p. 20. ISBN   978-1-57392-979-0.
  12. Parapsychological Association. "Paranormal". Glossary of key words frequently used in parapsychology. Archived from the original on 11 January 2011. Retrieved 13 December 2006.
  13. "Ghost". The Free Dictionary. Retrieved 13 December 2006.
  14. 1 2 3 Shepard, Leslie A.; Melton, J. Gordon, eds. (1996). "Animism". Encyclopedia of Occultism & Parapsychology (4th ed.). Gale Research & Cengage Learning. ISBN   978-0-8103-5487-6.
  15. Frazer, James George (1993). The Golden Bough: A Study in Magic and Religion. Herts: Wordsworth Editions Ltd. p. 178. ISBN   978-1-85326-310-1.
  16. "NASA Scientists To Discuss Search For Extraterrestrial Life". SpaceDaily.com. 11 December 2003. Retrieved 15 June 2014.
  17. Freudenrich, Craig (8 May 2001). "How SETI Works". HowStuffWorks . Retrieved 15 June 2014.
  18. 1 2 Denzler, Brenda (2001). The Lure of the Edge: Scientific Passions, Religious Beliefs, and the Pursuit of UFOs. University of California Press. pp. 67–. ISBN   978-0-520-22432-2.
  19. Priest, Susanna H. (2010). Encyclopedia of Science and Technology Communication. SAGE Publications. pp. 918–. ISBN   978-1-4129-5920-9.
  20. Hines, Terence (2003). pp. 119–120.
  21. Gulliksen, Harold O. (January 1938). "Extra-Sensory Perception: What Is It?". American Journal of Sociology . 43 (4): 623–634. doi:10.1086/217775. S2CID   145317132. Investigating Rhine's methods, we find that his mathematical methods are wrong and that the effect of this error would in some cases be negligible and in others very marked. We find that many of his experiments were set up in a manner which would tend to increase, instead of to diminish, the possibility of systematic clerical errors; and lastly, that the ESP cards can be read from the back.
  22. Wynn, Charles; Wiggins, Arthur (2001). Quantum Leaps in the Wrong Direction: Where Real Science Ends... and Pseudoscience Begins. Joseph Henry Press. p. 156. ISBN   978-0-309-07309-7. In 1940, Rhine coauthored a book, Extrasensory Perception After Sixty Years in which he suggested that something more than mere guess work was involved in his experiments. He was right! It is now known that the experiments conducted in his laboratory contained serious methodological flaws. Tests often took place with minimal or no screening between the subject and the person administering the test. Subjects could see the backs of cards that were later discovered to be so cheaply printed that a faint outline of the symbol could be seen. Furthermore, in face-to-face tests, subjects could see card faces reflected in the tester's eyeglasses or cornea. They were even able to (consciously or unconsciously) pick up clues from the tester's facial expression and voice inflection. In addition, an observant subject could identify the cards by certain irregularities like warped edges, spots on the backs, or design imperfections.
  23. Hines, Terence (2003). p. 122. "The procedural errors in the Rhine experiments have been extremely damaging to his claims to have demonstrated the existence of ESP. Equally damaging has been the fact that the results have not replicated when the experiments have been conducted in other laboratories."
  24. Irwin, Harvey J.; Watt, Caroline A. (2007). An Introduction to Parapsychology (5th ed.). McFarland. p. 249. ISBN   978-0-7864-3059-8.
  25. Kurtz, Paul (September–October 2006). "Summing Up Thirty Years of the Skeptical Inquirer". Skeptical Inquirer . 30 (5): 13–19.
  26. Stenger, Victor J. (1990). p. 192. "Today, parapsychology is widely regarded as a pseudoscience.... Over a century it has been tainted by fraud, incompetence, and a general unwillingness to accept the verdict of conventional scientific method."
  27. Pigliucci, Massimo; Boudry, Maarten (2013). Philosophy of Pseudoscience: Reconsidering the Demarcation Problem. University Of Chicago Press. p. 158. ISBN   978-0-226-05196-3. Many observers refer to the field as a 'pseudoscience'. When mainstream scientists say that the field of parapsychology is not scientific, they mean that no satisfying naturalistic cause-and-effect explanation for these supposed effects has yet been proposed and that the field's experiments cannot be consistently replicated.
  28. Hines, Terence (2003). p. 144. "It is important to realize that, in one hundred years of parapsychological investigations, there has never been a single adequate demonstration of the reality of any psi phenomenon."
  29. Cordón, Luis A. (2005). Popular Psychology: An Encyclopedia. Greenwood Press. p. 182. ISBN   978-0-313-32457-4. The essential problem is that a large portion of the scientific community, including most research psychologists, regards parapsychology as a pseudoscience, due largely to its failure to move beyond null results in the way science usually does. Ordinarily, when experimental evidence fails repeatedly to support a hypothesis, that hypothesis is abandoned. Within parapsychology, however, more than a century of experimentation has failed even to conclusively demonstrate the mere existence of paranormal phenomenon, yet parapsychologists continue to pursue that elusive goal.
  30. 1 2 3 Oling-Smee, L (1 March 2007). "The lab that asked the wrong questions". Nature . 446 (7131): 10–1. Bibcode:2007Natur.446...10O. doi: 10.1038/446010a . PMID   17330012.
  31. Husserl, Edmund (1970). Logical Investigations. Routledge Kegan Paul Books. ISBN   978-0-7100-6618-3.
  32. Becker, Howard S. (1993). Problems of Inference and Proof in Participant Observation (Bobbs Merrill Reprint ed.). New York: Irvington. ISBN   978-0-8290-3493-6.
  33. "Paranormal Groups". Ghostvillage.com. Archived from the original on 2 January 2007. Retrieved 15 June 2014.
  34. Caso, Alvaro (January–February 2002). "Three Skeptics' Debate Tools Examined". Skeptical Inquirer . 26 (1): 37–41.
  35. Alcock, James E. (1981). Parapsychology-Science Or Magic?: A Psychological Perspective. Pergamon Press. pp. 139–140. ISBN   978-0-08-025773-0. It was through the diligent efforts of conjurer James ('The Amazing') Randi that Geller was finally, at least in most people's eyes, exposed. Randi demonstrated that he could by ordinary conjuring means duplicate Geller's feats. His perseverance in investigating and unveiling the circumstances of many of Geller's more spectacular performances (including the discovery of confederates who aided Geller when necessary) made it very difficult for anyone with any degree of critical thought to continue to accept Geller's claims.
  36. Hurley, Patrick J. (2010). A Concise Introduction to Logic (11th ed.). Wadsworth Publishing. p. 626. ISBN   978-0-8400-3416-8. In fact Geller was just a clever trickster who duped his audiences. Geller's trickery was exposed in large measure by the magician James Randi. After watching videotapes of Geller's performances, Randi discovered how Geller performed his tricks, and in no time he was able to perform every one of them himself. Sometimes Geller would prepare a spoon or key beforehand by bending it back and forth several times to the point where it was nearly ready to break. Later, by merely stroking it gently, he could cause it to double over. On other occasions Geller, or his accomplices, would use sleight-of-hand maneuvers to substitute bent objects in the place of straight ones.
  37. "The Million Dollar Challenge". James Randi Education Foundation.
  38. Holt, Nicola; Simmonds-Moore, Christine; Luke, David; French, Christopher C. (2012). Anomalistic Psychology (Palgrave Insights in Psychology). Palgrave Macmillan. ISBN   978-0-230-30150-4.
  39. Wiseman, Richard; Watt, Caroline (2006). "Belief in psychic ability and the misattribution hypothesis: A qualitative review" (PDF). British Journal of Psychology. 97 (3): 323–38. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.537.3311 . doi:10.1348/000712605X72523. PMID   16848946.
  40. 1 2 van Elk, Michiel (September 2013). "Paranormal believers are more prone to illusory agency detection than skeptics". Consciousness and Cognition . 22 (3): 1041–46. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2013.07.004. PMID   23933505. S2CID   32087344.
  41. French, C. C.; Wilson, K. (2007). "Chapter 1: Cognitive factors underlying paranormal beliefs and experiences". In Sala, S. Della (ed.). Tall Tales About the Mind and Brain: Separating Fact From Fiction. Oxford University Press. pp. 3–22. ISBN   978-0-19-856877-3.
  42. Willard, Aiyana K.; Norenzayan, Ara (November 2013). "Cognitive biases explain religious belief, paranormal belief, and belief in life's purpose". Cognition . 129 (2): 379–91. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2013.07.016. PMID   23974049. S2CID   18869844.
  43. Rattet, Shelley L.; Bursik, Krisanne (August 2001). "Investigating the personality correlates of paranormal belief and precognitive experience". Personality and Individual Differences. 31 (3): 433–444. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00148-3.
  44. Wiseman, Richard; Greening, Emma; Smith, Matthew (2003). "Belief in the paranormal and suggestion in the seance room" (PDF). British Journal of Psychology. 94 (3): 285–297. CiteSeerX   10.1.1.528.2693 . doi:10.1348/000712603767876235. ISSN   2044-8295. PMID   14511544.
  45. Wolfradt, Uwe (July 1997). "Dissociative experiences, trait anxiety and paranormal beliefs". Personality and Individual Differences. 23 (1): 15–19. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(97)00043-3.
  46. Irwin, Harvey J. (2009). The Psychology of Paranormal Belief: A Researcher's Handbook. University Of Hertfordshire Press. p. 89. ISBN   978-1-902806-93-8.
  47. Bainbridge, William Sims (Winter 1978). "Chariots of the gullible". Skeptical Inquirer . 3 (2): 33–48.
  48. Wuthnow, Robert (June 1976). "Astrology and Marginality". Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion . 15 (2): 157–168. doi:10.2307/1385359. JSTOR   1385359.
  49. Otis, Laura P.; Alcock, James E. (1982). "Factors affecting extraordinary belief". The Journal of Social Psychology . 118 (1): 77–85. doi:10.1080/00224545.1982.9924420.
  50. Smith, Matthew D.; Foster, Christa L.; Stovin, Gordon (March 1998). "Intelligence and paranormal belief: Examining the role of context". Journal of Parapsychology . 62 (1): 65–77.
  51. Blum, Stuart H.; Blum, Lucille H. (1 August 1974). "Do's and Dont's: An Informal Study of some Prevailing Superstitions". Psychological Reports . 35 (1, Pt 2): 567–71. doi:10.2466/pr0.1974.35.1.567. S2CID   144097942.
  52. Jahoda, Gustav (June 1970). "Supernatural Beliefs and Changing Cognitive Structures among Ghanaian University Students". Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology . 1 (2): 115–130. doi:10.1177/002202217000100202. S2CID   145138079.
  53. Killen, Pat; Wildman, Robert W.; Wildman, Robert W. II (1 June 1974). "Superstitiousness and Intelligence". Psychological Reports. 34 (3, Pt 2): 1158. doi:10.2466/pr0.1974.34.3c.1158. S2CID   145559260.
  54. Tobacyk, Jerome (1 February 1984). "Paranormal Belief and College Grade Point Average". Psychological Reports. 54 (1): 217–8. doi:10.2466/pr0.1984.54.1.217. S2CID   147487003.
  55. Messer, Wayne S.; Griggs, Richard A. (1 December 1989). "Student belief and involvement in the paranormal and performance in introductory psychology". Teaching of Psychology . 16 (4): 187–191. doi:10.1207/s15328023top1604_4. S2CID   145512239.
  56. Gow, Kathryn M.; Lang, Tracey; Chant, David (2004). "Fantasy proneness, paranormal beliefs and personality features in out-of-body experiences". Contemporary Hypnosis. 21 (3): 107–125. doi:10.1002/ch.296.
  57. Irwin, Harvey J. (1 December 1994). "Paranormal Belief and Proneness to Dissociation". Psychological Reports. 75 (3, Pt 1): 1344–46. doi:10.2466/pr0.1994.75.3.1344. PMID   7892403. S2CID   37685766.
  58. Williams, Emyr; Francis, Leslie J.; Robbins, Mandy (15 August 2007). "Personality and paranormal belief: a study among adolescents" (PDF). Pastoral Psychology. 56 (1): 9–14. doi:10.1007/s11089-007-0094-x. S2CID   31448730.
  59. Tobacyk, Jerome; Milford, Gary (1983). "Belief in paranormal phenomena: Assessment instrument development and implications for personality functioning". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology . 44 (5): 1029–1037. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.44.5.1029.
  60. Roig, Miguel; Bridges, K. Robert; Renner, Catherine H.; Jackson, Cheryl R. (February 1998). "Belief in the paranormal and its association with irrational thinking controlled for context effects". Personality and Individual Differences. 24 (2): 229–236. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(97)00162-1.
  61. Wierzbicki, Michael (1985). "Reasoning errors and belief in the paranormal". The Journal of Social Psychology . 125 (4): 489–494. doi:10.1080/00224545.1985.9713529.
  62. Roe, Chris A.; Morgan, Claire L. (1 April 2002). "Narcissism and Belief in the Paranormal". Psychological Reports. 90 (2): 405–411. doi:10.2466/pr0.2002.90.2.405. PMID   12061576. S2CID   36206016.
  63. de Boer, Rémi; Bierman, Dick J. (January 2006). "The roots of paranormal belief: Divergent associations or real paranormal experiences?" (PDF). Proceedings of the 49th Annual Convention of the Parapsychological Association: 283–298.
  64. Lawrence, Emma; Peters, Emmanuelle R. (November 2004). "Reasoning in believers in the paranormal". The Journal of Nervous & Mental Disease . 192 (11): 727–733. doi:10.1097/01.nmd.0000144691.22135.d0. PMID   15505516. S2CID   22487053.
  65. French, C. C.; Santomauro, Julia; Hamilton, Victoria; Fox, Rachel; Thalbourne, M. A. (November 2008). "Psychological aspects of the alien contact experience" (PDF). Cortex . 44 (10): 1387–1395. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2007.11.011. PMID   18635162. S2CID   6174823. Archived from the original (PDF) on 26 September 2020. Retrieved 26 September 2020.
  66. Perkins, Stefanie L.; Allen, Rhiannon (May 2006). "Childhood Physical Abuse and Differential Development of Paranormal Belief Systems". The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease . 194 (5): 349–355. doi:10.1097/01.nmd.0000217832.85665.c5. PMID   16699384. S2CID   21530392.
  67. French, C. C., & Kerman, M. K. (1996). "Childhood trauma, fantasy proneness and belief in the paranormal". Paper presented to the 1996 London Conference of the British Psychological Society, Institute of Education, University of London, 17–18 December 1996.
  68. Lawrence, Tony; Edwards, Claire; Barraclough, Nicholas; Church, Sarah; Hetherington, Francesca (August 1995). "Modelling childhood causes of paranormal belief and experience: Childhood trauma and childhood fantasy". Personality and Individual Differences. 19 (2): 209–215. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(95)00034-4.
  69. Ross, Christopher A.; Joshi, Smita (June 1992). "Paranormal Experiences in the General Population". The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease . 180 (6): 357–361. doi:10.1097/00005053-199206000-00004. PMID   1593270. S2CID   21459764.
  70. Blackmore, S. J.; Trościanko, T. (November 1985). "Belief in the paranormal: probability judgements, illusory control and the 'chance baseline shift'". British Journal of Psychology. 76 (4): 459–468. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1985.tb01969.x.
  71. Clarke, Dave (1991). "Belief in the paranormal: a New Zealand survey". Journal of the Society for Psychical Research . 57 (823): 412–425.
  72. Rice, Tom W. (March 2003). "Believe It Or Not: Religious and Other Paranormal Beliefs in the United States". Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion . 42 (1): 95–106. doi:10.1111/1468-5906.00163.
  73. Tobacyk, Jerome; Nagot, Ed; Miller, Mark (June 1988). "Paranormal Beliefs and Locus of Control: A Multidimensional Examination". Journal of Personality Assessment . 52 (2): 241–246. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa5202_5.
  74. Otis, Laura P.; Kuo, Eddie C. (1984). "Extraordinary beliefs among students in Singapore and Canada". The Journal of Psychology . 116 (2): 215–226. doi:10.1080/00223980.1984.9923639.
  75. Bader, Christopher D.; Mencken, F. Carson; Baker, Joseph (2011). Paranormal America: Ghost Encounters, UFO Sightings, Bigfoot Hunts, and Other Curiosities in Religion and Culture. NYU Press. pp. 57–58. ISBN   978-0-8147-9135-6.
  76. Chang, Kenneth (11 November 2003). "Do Paranormal Phenomena exist?". The New York Times. Retrieved 4 October 2013.
  77. Shiah, YJ; Wu, YZ; Chen, YH; Chiang, SK (April 2014). "Schizophrenia and the paranormal: More psi belief and superstition, and less déjà vu in medicated schizophrenic patients". Comprehensive Psychiatry . 55 (3): 688–692. doi:10.1016/j.comppsych.2013.11.003. PMID   24355706.
  78. Raz, Amir; Hines, T. M.; Fossella, John; Castro, Daniella (September 2008). "Paranormal experience and the COMT dopaminergic gene: A preliminary attempt to associate phenotype with genotype using an underlying brain theory". Cortex. 44 (10): 1336–41. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2007.07.011. PMID   18814869. S2CID   6070468.
  79. Pizzagalli, Diego; Lehmann, Dietrich; et al. (22 December 2000). "Brain electric correlates of strong belief in paranormal phenomena: intracerebral EEG source and regional Omega complexity analyses". Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging . 100 (3): 139–154. doi:10.1016/S0925-4927(00)00070-6. PMID   11120441. S2CID   3098128.
  80. Schulter, Günter; Papousek, Ilona (August 2008). "Believing in paranormal phenomena: Relations to asymmetry of body and brain". Cortex. 44 (10): 1326–35. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2007.08.022. PMID   18706541. S2CID   21140138.
  81. Phillips, Helen. "Paranormal beliefs linked to brain chemistry". New Scientist. Retrieved 4 October 2013.
  82. Singer, Barry; Benassi, Victor A. (January–February 1981). "Occult Beliefs: Media distortions, social uncertainty, and deficiencies of human reasoning seem to be at the basis of occult beliefs". American Scientist . 69 (1): 49–55.
  83. Kurtz, Paul (2001). Skepticism and Humanism: The New Paradigm. Transaction Publishers. p. 63. ISBN   978-0-7658-0051-0.
  84. Hines, Terence (2003). p. 38.
  85. Pigliucci, Massimo; Boudry, Maarten (2013). p. 155.
  86. 1 2 ABC Science Online (17 November 2006). "'Spooky survey' gets big response". ABC News . Archived from the original on 24 November 2006. Retrieved 15 June 2014.
  87. 1 2 3 "Smart People See Ghosts", Brad Steiger, Fate Magazine, April 2006, p. 52-56; the unusual thing found by Farha and Steward was that belief in the supernatural increased with education level, contrary to many other surveys. However, that aspect of their study is not being used here.
  88. 1 2 3 Moore, David W. (16 June 2005). "Three in Four Americans Believe in Paranormal". Gallup . Archived from the original on 19 September 2017. Retrieved 19 September 2013.
  89. USA Today, 12 January 1994
  90. "Science and Technology: Public Attitudes and Understanding – Public Knowledge About S&T; Chapter 7 of Science and Engineering Indicators 2004". National Science Board (National Science Foundation). Archived from the original on 18 August 2015. Retrieved 7 March 2012.
  91. "Chapman Survey 2017: Advanced Ancient Civilizations, Spirits, Alien Visits Remain Top Paranormal Beliefs" . Skeptical Inquirer . 42 (1): 5. January–February 2018.
  92. "Randi $1,000,000 paranormal challenge". The Skeptic's Dictionary. Retrieved 3 February 2008.
  93. Larsen, Claus (September 2003). "Get Rich Quick or Save the World". Skeptic Report. Archived from the original on 23 March 2007. Retrieved 7 March 2007.
  94. Independent Investigations Group. "Investigations" . Retrieved 11 April 2012.

Further reading