Tawbuid | |
---|---|
Native to | Philippines |
Region | Mindoro |
Native speakers | 14,000 (2000) [1] |
Language codes | |
ISO 639-3 | Either: bnj – Eastern Tawbuid twb – Western Tawbuid |
Glottolog | bata1318 |
The Tawbuid language is a language spoken by Tawbuid Mangyans in the province of Mindoro in the Philippines. It is divided into eastern and western dialects. The Bangon Mangyans also speak the western dialect of Tawbuid.
The Tau-buid (or Tawbuid) Mangyans live in central Mindoro.
In Oriental Mindoro, Eastern Tawbuid (also known as Bangon) is spoken by 1,130 people in the municipalities of Socorro, Pinamalayan, and Gloria. [1]
In Occidental Mindoro, Western Tawbuid (also known as Batangan) is spoken by 6,810 people in the municipalities of Sablayan and Calintaan. [1]
Front | Central | Back | |
---|---|---|---|
Close | i | ɨ | u |
Mid | e | ɔ | |
Open | a |
Labial | Alveolar | Palatal | Velar | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Nasal | m | n | ŋ | ||
Plosive | voiceless | p | t | k | |
voiced | b | d | g | ||
Fricative | f | s | |||
Liquid | lateral | l | |||
flap | ɾ | ||||
Approximant | w | j |
Comparison with related languages shows a gradual loss of /k/ > /h/ > /Ø/. For example:
There is a residual /k/ in the 1st person singular, in the affix /ak-/, usually shortened in speech to /k-/. E.g. kadasug kban (or akban) 'I will arrive.'
There are no glottal phonemes, either /h/ or /ʔ/, in Tawbuid.
The glottal stop [ʔ] may be realized between adjacent identical vowels. Normally though, in connected speech, two adjacent vowels are either merged to form a lengthened vowel or differentiated by stress. For example:
Notice that in the above, the stress precedes the glottal, whereas without a glottal, the stress is in the normal position for that particular stress pattern. Vowels following /i/ and /u/ offer different interpretations as to whether a linking /y/ or /w/ is present. For example:
There is a remarkable absence of assimilation at the point of articulation of nasals with following sounds. For example:
/i/ close front spread
/e/ half close front spread
Established as a phoneme in contrast with /i/ by minimal pairs
Occurrence in similar environment:
Historically this was most likely /ay/. It is a common occurrence in languages around the world for /ay/ to become /e/, as in French and English. (Tagalog also exhibits this trend, with may being pronounced /me/ in connected speech.) Comparison of Tawbuid with related languages shows this:
Within Tawbuid, /ay/ and /e/ alternate with different grammatical forms of the same word.
/a/ open central unrounded
Vowel which occurs in syllable-initial, mid and final positions.
/o/ half-open back rounded
Established as a phoneme in contrast with /u/ by minimal pairs
As with /e/, this is probably a historical development of /aw/. A similar process occurred in English and French.
/u/ close back rounded
Syllable-initial, middle and final
/ɨ/ close central unrounded
Syllable-initial, middle and final
In orthography, the letter v is used. In the 1950s when the Reeds started writing the language, that was a convenient (and unused) letter on the typewriter. It is the least frequent vowel (>1%), and in fact the least frequent phoneme (>0.5%) in the language. It mostly occurs with /a/ or /ɨ/ in an adjacent syllable. In all but one word (tibanglvn) /a/ and /ɨ/ are the only vowels used. (One exception noted: the name of a river near Tundayaw is Guribvy.)
/b/ voiced bilabial plosive
Syllable-initial and final. For example:
/p/ voiceless bilabial plosive
Environment: syllable-initial (but rare word-initial) and final
Variants: [p] voiceless unaspirated bilabial plosive
Environment: syllable-initial
[pʰ] voiceless slightly aspirated bilabial plosive
Environment: word-final
/p/ is established as a phoneme in contrast with /f/ by the following:
There is at least one minimal pair:
/p/ is in contrastive distribution with /f/ under the following circumstances:
/d/ voiced alveolar plosive
Syllable-initial and final.
Realised as [t] before voiceless consonants, most frequently in the verb form CVd-root-an.
/t/ voiceless alveolar plosive
Environment: syllable-initial and final
Variants:
Environment: syllable-initial
Environment: word-final
/g/ voiced velar plosive
Environment: syllable-initial and final, or initial cluster.
Realised as [k] before voiceless consonants, for example in the verb prefix g-, and prefixes tag-, fag-.
/k/ voiceless velar plosive
Environment: syllable-initial and final
[k] voiceless unaspirated bilabial plosive
Environment: syllable-initial
[kʰ] voiceless slightly aspirated plosive
Environment: word-final
There is a tendency for the initial /k/ to be lost in Tawbuid compared to similar words in related languages. For example:
/f/ voiceless labiodental fricative
Environment: syllable-initial only. See comments on /p/ for contrastive features.
Rare in Austronesian languages. Historically related to Tagalog and other Philippine languages. /p/. For example:
/s/ voiceless alveolar fricative
Can occur in all syllable positions, and in the initial consonant cluster /st/. The affricate /ts/ is treated as a unit rather than two successive consonants.
/m/ bilabial nasal
Can occur in all syllable positions.
/n/ dental nasal
Environment: syllable-initial and final and syllabic
/ŋ/ velar nasal
Environment: syllable-initial and final and syllabic
/l/ voiced alveolar palatalized lateral
Environment: syllable-initial and final
/R/ voiced alveolar flap
Environment: syllable-initial and (rarely) final
/w/ voiced bilabial approximant
Environment: syllable-initial and final
/y/ voiced palatal approximant
Environment: syllable-initial and final
Stress patterns
Primary stress in Tawbuid is either final or penultimate. Most words are stressed unpredictably, and in some speakers, all syllables seem to be equally stressed. Modification in stress occurs in affective speech (see below).
Some syllable patterns have predictable stress. A word containing two adjacent syllables with CVC patterns are stressed on the second of those two syllables, whether final or not.
Words with two identical CVC patterns interrupted by /-ar-/ or /-al-/ are also stressed on the second of those two CVC syllables.
Where the final and penultimate syllables are open, and the vowels are the same, the stress is penultimate.
But when the vowels are different, stress can occur unpredictably.
A root word can change its stress when affixes are added, because affixes carry their own inherent stress.
In affective speech (utterances in which the speaker wishes to convey emotion), lengthening may change stress:
Secondary stress and tertiary stress
In words of more than three syllables there is a secondary and even a tertiary stress.
Accent
Within the Western Tawbuid region, there are distinctive accents as well as vocabulary preferences. Taking the rebuke lag katanya 'don't do that':
A rebuke or any utterance conveying a negative emotion is frequently said with lips rounded throughout.
Syllable patterns
V
Monosyllabic words are: e, o, u
Some words beginning with a vowel have a V syllable-initial pattern.
V-CV
C – in the case of the completed aspect prefix /n-/
VC
CVC
CCV
CVC with semivowels
In phonology, an allophone is one of multiple possible spoken sounds – or phones – used to pronounce a single phoneme in a particular language. For example, in English, the voiceless plosive and the aspirated form are allophones for the phoneme, while these two are considered to be different phonemes in some languages such as Central Thai. Similarly, in Spanish, and are allophones for the phoneme, while these two are considered to be different phonemes in English.
Unless otherwise noted, statements in this article refer to Standard Finnish, which is based on the dialect spoken in the former Häme Province in central south Finland. Standard Finnish is used by professional speakers, such as reporters and news presenters on television.
The Catalan phonology has a certain degree of dialectal variation. Although there are two standard varieties, one based on Central Eastern dialect and another one based on South-Western or Valencian, this article deals with features of all or most dialects, as well as regional pronunciation differences.
Mangyan is the generic name for the eight indigenous groups found in Mindoro each with its own tribal name, language, and customs. The total population may be around 280,001, but official statistics are difficult to determine under the conditions of remote areas, reclusive tribal groups and some having little if any outside world contact.
Sikkimese is a language of the Tibeto-Burman languages spoken by the Bhutia people in Sikkim in northeast India, parts of Koshi province in eastern Nepal, and Bhutan. It is one of the official languages of Sikkim.
Eastern Pomo, also known as Clear Lake Pomo, is a nearly extinct Pomoan language spoken around Clear Lake in Lake County, California by one of the Pomo peoples.
The Tarahumara language is a Mexican Indigenous language of the Uto-Aztecan language family spoken by around 70,000 Tarahumara (Rarámuri/Ralámuli) people in the state of Chihuahua, according to a 2002 census conducted by the government of Mexico.
Yanesha', also called Amuesha or Amoesha is a language spoken by the Amuesha people of Peru in central and eastern Pasco Region.
Taos is a Tanoan language spoken by several hundred people in New Mexico, in the United States. The main description of its phonology was contributed by George L. Trager in a (pre-generative) structuralist framework. Earlier considerations of the phonetics-phonology were by John P. Harrington and Jaime de Angulo. Trager's first account was in Trager (1946) based on fieldwork 1935-1937, which was then substantially revised in Trager (1948). The description below takes Trager (1946) as the main point of departure and notes where this differs from the analysis of Trager (1948). Harrington's description is more similar to Trager (1946). Certain comments from a generative perspective are noted in a comparative work Hale (1967).
Baiso or Bayso is a Lowland East Cushitic language belonging to the Omo–Tana subgroup, and is spoken in Ethiopia, in the region around Lake Abaya.
The phonology of the Zuni language as spoken in the southwestern United States is described here. Phonology is a branch of linguistics that studies how languages or dialects systematically organize their sounds.
This article is about the phonology and phonetics of the Estonian language.
Kensiu (Kensiw) is an Austroasiatic language of the Jahaic subbranch. It is spoken by a small community of 300 people in Yala Province in southern Thailand and also reportedly by a community of approximately 300 speakers in Western Malaysia in Perak and Kedah states. Speakers of this language are Negritos who are known as the Maniq people or Mani of Thailand. In Malaysia, they are counted among the Orang Asli.
The Bukid language, Binukid or Bukidnon, is an Austronesian language spoken by indigenous peoples of Northern Mindanao in the southern Philippines. The word Bukid means 'mountain' or 'highland' while Binukid means 'in the manner, or style, of the mountain or highland'. In Bukidnon province, it is referred to as Higaonon.
Wadiyara Koli is an Indo-Aryan language of the Gujarati group. It is spoken by the Wadiyara people, who originate from Wadiyar in Gujarat; many of whom are thought to have migrated to Sindh in the early twentieth century, following the onset of famine. The Wadiyara people are affiliated with the Bhil people and Koli people, but are generally more inclined towards associating themselves with the Koli; they are often regarded as a subgroup of the latter.
Avava (Navava), also known as Katbol, Tembimbe-Katbol, or Bangsa’ is an Oceanic language of central Malekula, Vanuatu. It has nasalized fricatives and a bilabial trill.
Adyghe is a language of the Northwest Caucasian family which, like the other Northwest Caucasian languages, is very rich in consonants, featuring many labialized and ejective consonants. Adyghe is phonologically more complex than Kabardian, having the retroflex consonants and their labialized forms.
Matlatzinca, or more specifically San Francisco Matlatzinca, is an endangered Oto-Manguean language of Western Central Mexico.[3] The name of the language in the language itself is pjiekak'joo.[4] The term "Matlatzinca" comes from the town's name in Nahuatl, meaning "the lords of the network." At one point, the Matlatzinca groups were called "pirindas," meaning "those in the middle."[5]
Medumba phonology is the way in which the Medumba language is pronounced. Medumba is a Bamileke language of Cameroon; the people who speak it originate from the Nde division of the West Region of the country. It deals with phonetics, phonotactics and their variation across different dialects of Medumba.