The grammar of the Otomi language displays a mixture of elements of synthetic and analytic structures. Particularly the phrase-level morphology is synthetic, whereas the sentence-level is analytic. [1] Simultaneously, the language is head-marking in terms of its verbal morphology, but not in its nominal morphology, which is more analytic. Otomi recognizes three large open word classes of nouns, verbs, and particles. There is a small closed class of property words, variously analyzed as adjectives or stative verbs. [2]
According to the most-common analysis, the Otomi language has two kinds of bound morphemes, proclitics and affixes. Proclitics differ from affixes mainly in their phonological characteristics – they are marked for tone and block nasal harmony. [3] Some authors consider proclitics to be better analyzed as prefixes. [4] [5] Orthographically, the standard orthography writes proclitics as separate words, whereas affixes are written joined to their host root. Most affixes are suffixes and with few exceptions occur only on verbs, whereas the proclitics occur both in nominal and verbal paradigms. Proclitics mark the categories of definiteness and number, person, negation, tense and aspect – often fused in a single proclitic. Suffixes mark direct and indirect objects, as well as clusivity (the distinction between inclusive and exclusive we), number, location and affective emphasis. Historically, as in other Oto-Manguean languages, the basic word order is verb–subject–object (VSO), but some dialects tend towards subject–verb–object (SVO) word order, probably under influence from Spanish. [1] Possessive constructions use the order possessed-possessor but modificational constructions use modifier-head order.
The phonemic orthography employed for writing Otomi in this article is the one used by Lastra (1992 and 2005). It includes tones in order to be maximally informative, but many practical orthographies used by Otomi speakers do not include information about tone. The symbols used to write the tones in the examples are acute accent /´/ for high tone, and circumflex accent /^/ for ascending tone; low tone is left unmarked. The symbols used for the four nasal vowels are /į, ę, ą, ų/. The consonant symbols: /c/ denotes IPA [t͡s], /y/ denotes IPA [j]. The remaining symbols are from the IPA with their original values.
The pronominal system of most Otomi varieties distinguish four persons: first inclusive and exclusive, second and third, and three numbers singular, dual and plural. The system below is from the Toluca dialect. [6]
Singular | Dual | Plural | |
---|---|---|---|
1st-person incl. | nugó "I" | nugó-bé "you and I" | nugó-hé "I and you guys" |
1st-person excl. | nugó-wí "we two (not you)" | nugó-hɨ´ "We all (not you)" | |
2nd-person | nukʔígé "you" | nukʔígé-wí "you two" | nukʔígé-gɨ´ "you guys" |
3rd-person | gégé "she/he/it" | nugégé-wí "the two of them" | nugégé-hɨ´ "they" |
Otomi nouns are inflected for possession, and for diminutive. The particular pattern of possessive inflection is widespread throughout the Mesoamerican Linguistic Area. A possessed noun is prefixed with a morpheme agreeing in person with the possessor. If the possessor is plural or dual, the noun is also marked with a suffix agreeing with the possessor's number. Below is given the inflectional paradigm for the word /ngų́/ "house" in the dialect of Toluca. [7]
Singular | Dual | Plural | |
---|---|---|---|
1st-person excl. | mą-ngų́ "my house" | mą-ngų́-bé "Our house (me and him/her)" | mą-ngų́-hé "Our house (me and them)" |
1st-person incl. | mą-ngų́-wí "Our house (me and you)" | mą-ngų́-hɨ́ "Our house (me and you and them)" | |
2nd-person | ri-ngų́ "your house" | ri-ngų́-wí "you two's house" | ri-ngų́-hɨ́ "you guys' house" |
3rd-person | rʌ-ngų́ "her/his/its house" | yʌ-ngų́-wí "the house of the two of them" | yʌ-ngų́-hɨ́ "their house" |
To express plurality of a possessed noun, a periphrastic construction is used.
kʔɨ
those
mą-ngų́
I-house
"my houses"
Possession can also be emphatic, in which case it adds an emphatic suffix -gó (first person) -gé (second person) or -gégé (third person) and adds as a prefix the word mɛhti "possession".
ni
that
rʌ
the
ʔbɛ̂cʔé
basket
mą-mɛ́hti-gó-ní
I-possession-me-that
"that basket is mine"
ni
that
rʌ
the
ʔbɛ̂cʔé
basket
rʌ-mɛ́hti-gégé-ní
He/she-possession-her/him-that
"that basket is his/hers"
The diminutive is inflected by a prefix ci-
ci-nú
DIM-house
"little house"
Plurality of nouns is expressed with articles preceding the noun, rʌ "the (singular)" or yʌ "the (dual/plural)":
Singular | Dual | Plural |
---|---|---|
rʌ ngų́ "the house" | yʌ yóho ngų́ "the two houses" | yʌ ngų́ "the houses" |
In addition to the simple plural/singular articles, the Classical Otomi language as described by Cárceres distinguished honorific and pejorative articles. In classical Otomi, the articles were ąn "neutral singular", e (neutral and honorific plural), nø̂ (pejorative singular) yo (pejorative plural) and o (honorific singular). [8]
On verbs, all of the categories of person of subject, tense, aspect and mood are marked by the means of a single prefix on each verb. [9] In Otomi of Toluca and of Ixtenco, the categories distinguished are present, preterit, perfect, imperfect, future, pluperfect, two different subjunctives, present and past continuative and imperative. Mezquital Otomi has additional moods. [10] On transitive verbs, person of the object is inflected by a suffix. If either subject or object is dual or plural, it is shown with a plural suffix following the object suffix.
The structure of the Otomi verb is as follows:
Person of subject/T/A/M | Misc. prefix (e.g. adverbial) | Root | Object suffix | Plural/Dual suffix |
The present tense prefixes are di- (first person), gi- (second person), and i- (third person).
Singular | Dual | Plural | |
---|---|---|---|
1st-person excl. | di-nú "I see" | di-nú-bé "we see (me and him/her)" | di-nú-hé "we see (me and them)" |
1st-person incl. | di-nú-wí "We see(me and you)" | di-nú-hɨ´ "We see (me and you and them)" | |
2nd-person | gi-nú "you see" | gi-nú-wí "You two see" | gi-nú-hɨ´ "You guys see" |
3rd-person | i-nú "she/he/it sees" | i-nú-wí "the two of them see" | i-nú-hɨ´ "they see" |
The preterit uses the prefixes do-, go- and bi-, perfect uses to-, ko-, ʃi-, imperfect uses dimá, gimá, mi, future uses go-, gi- and da- and pluperfect tamą-, kimą-, kamą-. All tenses use the same suffixes for dual and plural numbers and clusivity as the present tense; from here on only the singular forms will be given. The difference between preterit and imperfect is similar to the distinction between the preterit in Spanish habló "he spoke (punctual)" and the imperfect hablaba "he spoke/He used to speak/he was speaking (non-punctual)".
Preterit | Perfect | Imperfect | Future tense | Pluperfect | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
1st-person singular | do-nú "I saw (punctual)" | to-nú "I have seen" | dimá-nú "I saw (non-punctual)" | go-nú "I will see" | tamą-nú "I had seen" |
2nd-person singular | go-nú "you saw (punctual)" | ko-nú "you have seen" | gimá-nú "you saw (non-punctual)" | gi-nú "you will see" | kimą-nú "you had seen" |
3rd-person singular | bi-nú "she/he/it saw (punctual)" | ʃi-nú "she/he/it has seen" | mi-nú "you saw (non-punctual)" | da-nú "she/he/it will see" | kamą-nú "she/he/it had seen" |
In Toluca Otomi, the semantic differences between the two subjunctive forms (A and B) are not easily defined; according to Lastra, sometimes subjunctive B has a meaning that is more recent in time than subjunctive A. Both have the meaning of something counterfactual. However, in other Otomi varieties, e.g. Otomi of Ixtenco Tlaxcala, the distinction between the two forms is one of subjunctive vs. irrealis. [10] The past and present progressive are similar in meaning to English was and is X-ing respectively. The imperative is for issuing direct orders.
Subjunctive A | Subjunctive B | Present progressive | Past progressive | Imperative | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
1st-person singular | (n)gwa-nú "I would have seen" | kwa-nú "I would have seen" | drʌ-nú "I am seeing" | ndrʌ-nú "I was seeing" | * |
2nd-person singular | (n)gwi-nú "you would have seen" | kwi-nú "you would have seen" | grʌ-nú "you are seeing" | dgrʌ-nú "you were seeing" | nú "See!" |
3rd-person singular | (n)di-nú "she/he/it would have seen" | trʌ-nú "she/he/it would have seen" | rʌ-nú "she/he/it is seeing" | mbrʌ-nú "she/he/it was seeing" | * |
Verbs expressing movement towards the speaker such as ʔįhį "come" use a different set of prefixes for marking person/T/A/M. These prefixes can also be used with other verbs to express "to do something while coming this way". In Toluca Otomi, mba- is the third-person singular imperfect prefix for movement verbs.
mba-tųhų
3SG.MOV.IPFV-sing
"he came singing" [11]
To form predicates from nouns, the subject prefixes are simply added to the noun root:
drʌ-môkhá
1SG.PRS.CONT-priest
"I am a priest" [11]
Transitive verbs are inflected for agreement with their objects by means of suffixes, while using the same subject prefixes as the intransitive verbs to agree with their agents. However, in all dialects a few intransitive verbs take the object suffix instead of the subject prefix; usually these intransitive verbs are stative, i.e. describing a state. This has led to the interpretation that in Otomi, morphosyntactic alignment is split between active–stative and accusative systems. [12]
In Toluca Otomi, the object suffixes are -gí (first person), -kʔí (second person) and -bi (third person), but the vowel /i/ may harmonize to /e/ when suffixed to a root containing /e/. The first-person suffix is realized as -kí after sibilants and after certain verb roots, and -hkí when used with certain other verbs. The second-person object suffix may sometimes metathesise to -ʔkí. The third-person suffix also has the allomorphs -hpí/-hpé, -pí, -bí, and sometimes third-person objects are marked with a zero morpheme.
1st-person object | 2nd-person object | 3rd-person object |
---|---|---|
bi-ñús-kí he/past-write-1.OBJ "he wrote me" | bi-ñús-kʔí he/past-write-2.OBJ "he wrote you" | bi-kré-bi he/past-believe-3.OBJ "he believed it" |
bi-nú-gí he/past-see-1.OBJ "he saw me" | bi-nú-kʔí he/past-see-2.OBJ "he saw you" | bi-hkwáhti-bí he/she/past-hit-3.OBJ "she/he hit him/her" |
Plural and dual number of objects is marked by the same suffixes as the subject, in some cases leading to ambiguity about the respective numbers of subject and object. With object suffixes of the first or second persons sometimes the verbal root changes, often dropping final vowels.
dual object/subject | plural object/subject |
---|---|
bi- he/PAST- ñaʃ cut hair -kʔí -you -wí -DU "the two of them cut your hair" or | bi- he/PAST- ñaʃ cut hair -kí -you -hɨ´ -PL "they cut my hair" or |
A class of words that describe properties or states has been described either as adjectives [13] or as stative verbs. [14] This word class consists of words with a meaning of attributing a property to an entity, e.g. "the man is tall", "the house is old". Within this class, some roots use the normal subject/T/A/M prefixes, while others always use the object suffixes to encode the person of the patient or subject. The use of the same suffixes that are used to encode the patient/objects of transitive verbs to encode the patient/subject of the predicate has been interpreted as a trait of split intransitivity. [12] This phenomenon occurs in all dialects, but which stative verbs the object prefixes, and how many take, vary between dialects. In Toluca Otomi, most stative verbs are conjugated using a set of suffixes similar to the object/patient suffixes and a third-person subject prefix, while only a few use the present continuative subject prefixes. The following examples of the two kinds of stative verb conjugation in Toluca Otomi. [13]
with patient/object suffix | with subject/agent prefix |
---|---|
rʌ- it/PRS- nô- fat- hkʔí me "I am fat" | drʌ- I/PRS/CONT- dôtʔî short "I am short" |
Most Otomi varieties also allow different kinds of adverbial meanings to be inflected on the verb.
From Toluca Otomi, examples of adverbial affixes are:
kʔʌ
rʌ
ʃùa
ya
bì-pɛphí
"Juan is working now (I see)" (Indicative the Juan now bí-work)
bɨ
when
ga-thô
ga-pass.by
rʌ-tá mbrʌ-mí-thó
3/PAST/CONT-sit-COMPL
"When his father came by he was already sitting"
do-ndɨ-chú
1/PRF-ndɨ-scared
"I got really scared"
Other affixes express inchoative aspect, instrumental function or purpose. There is also a suffix with the meaning of "meanwhile". [13]
Some dialects have SVO as the most frequent word order, for example Otomi of Toluca [15] and of San Ildefonso, Querétaro, [16] but other dialects such as Mezquital Otomi have VSO as the basic, pragmatically unmarked word order. [17] Proto-Otomi is also thought to have had VSO order as verb-initial order is the most frequent basic word order in other Oto-Manguean languages. It has been reported that some Otomi dialects are shifting from verb initial to a subject-initial basic word order under influence from Spanish. [18] Interrogative particles occur sentence initially conditional classes follow the conclusion. Relative clauses follow the noun. [13]
The Fur language is a Nilo-Saharan language spoken by the Fur of Darfur in Western Sudan. It is part of a broader family of languages known as the Fur languages.
Persian grammar is the grammar of the Persian language, whose dialectal variants are spoken in Iran, Afghanistan, Caucasus, Uzbekistan and Tajikistan. It is similar to that of many other Indo-European languages. The language became a more analytic language around the time of Middle Persian, with fewer cases and discarding grammatical gender. The innovations remain in Modern Persian, which is one of the few Indo-European languages to lack grammatical gender, even in pronouns.
The Ojibwe language is an Algonquian American Indian language spoken throughout the Great Lakes region and westward onto the northern plains. It is one of the largest American Indian languages north of Mexico in terms of number of speakers, and exhibits a large number of divergent dialects. For the most part, this article describes the Minnesota variety of the Southwestern dialect. The orthography used is the Fiero Double-Vowel System.
This article describes the grammar of the standard Tajik language as spoken and written in Tajikistan. In general, the grammar of the Tajik language fits the analytical type. Little remains of the case system, and grammatical relationships are primarily expressed via clitics, word order and other analytical constructions. Like other modern varieties of Persian, Tajik grammar is almost identical to the classic Persian grammar, although there are differences in some verb tenses.
Otomi is an Oto-Pamean language spoken by approximately 240,000 indigenous Otomi people in the central altiplano region of Mexico. Otomi consists of several closely related languages, many of which are not mutually intelligible. The word Hñähñu has been proposed as an endonym, but since it represents the usage of a single dialect, it has not gained wide currency. Linguists have classified the modern dialects into three dialect areas: the Northwestern dialects are spoken in Querétaro, Hidalgo and Guanajuato; the Southwestern dialects are spoken in the State of Mexico; and the Eastern dialects are spoken in the highlands of Veracruz, Puebla, and eastern Hidalgo and villages in Tlaxcala and Mexico states.
This article provides a grammar sketch of the Nawat or Pipil language, an endangered language spoken by the Pipils of western El Salvador and Nicarao people of Nicaragua. It belongs to the Nahua group within the Uto-Aztecan language family. There also exists a brief typological overview of the language that summarizes the language's most salient features of general typological interest in more technical terms.
Pohnpeian is a Micronesian language spoken as the indigenous language of the island of Pohnpei in the Caroline Islands. Pohnpeian has approximately 30,000 (estimated) native speakers living in Pohnpei and its outlying atolls and islands with another 10,000-15,000 (estimated) living off island in parts of the US mainland, Hawaii and Guam. It is the second-most widely spoken native language of the Federated States of Micronesia.
This article discusses the grammar of the Western Lombard (Insubric) language. The examples are in Milanese, written according to the Classical Milanese orthography.
The verb is one of the most complex parts of Basque grammar. It is sometimes represented as a difficult challenge for learners of the language, and many Basque grammars devote most of their pages to lists or tables of verb paradigms. This article does not give a full list of verb forms; its purpose is to explain the nature and structure of the system.
Somali is an agglutinative language, using many affixes and particles to determine and alter the meaning of words. As in other related Afroasiatic languages, Somali nouns are inflected for gender, number and case, while verbs are inflected for persons, number, tenses, and moods.
Munsee is an endangered language of the Eastern Algonquian subgroup of the Algonquian language family, itself a branch of the Algic language family.
Temoaya Otomi, also known as Toluca Otomi or Otomi of San Andrés Cuexcontitlan, is a variety of the Otomi language spoken in Mexico by ca. 37,000 people in and around the municipality of Temoaya, and in three communities within the municipality of Toluca: San Andrés Cuexcontitlán, San Pablo Autopan and San Cristobal Huichochitlan. The two varieties are quite different. The speakers themselves call the language Ñatho. Lastra (2001) classifies it as a southwestern dialect along with the dialects of Mexico state. Lastra also notes that the endangered Otomí dialect of San Felipe in eastern Michoacán is most similar to the Otomí spoken in San Andrés Cuexcontitlan.
Mingrelian is a Kartvelian language that is mainly spoken in the Western Georgian regions Samegrelo and Abkhazia. In Abkhazia the number of Mingrelian speakers declined dramatically in the 1990s as a result of heavy ethnic cleansing of ethnic Georgians, the overwhelming majority of which were Mingrelians.
This article discusses the conjugation of verbs in a number of varieties of Catalan, including Old Catalan. Each verbal form is accompanied by its phonetic transcription. Widely used dialectal forms are included, even if they are not considered standard in either of the written norms: those of the Institut d'Estudis Catalans and the Acadèmia Valenciana de la Llengua. Other dialectal forms exist, including those characteristic of minor dialects such as Ribagorçan and Algherese and transitional forms of major dialects.
Mortlockese, also known as Mortlock or Nomoi, is a language that belongs to the Chuukic group of Micronesian languages in the Federated States of Micronesia spoken primarily in the Mortlock Islands. It is nearly intelligible with Satawalese, with an 18 percent intelligibility and an 82 percent lexical similarity, and Puluwatese, with a 75 percent intelligibility and an 83 percent lexical similarity. The language today has become mutually intelligible with Chuukese, though marked with a distinct Mortlockese accent. Linguistic patterns show that Mortlockese is converging with Chuukese since Mortlockese now has an 80 to 85 percent lexical similarity.
This article describes the grammar of the Old Irish language. The grammar of the language has been described with exhaustive detail by various authors, including Thurneysen, Binchy and Bergin, McCone, O'Connell, Stifter, among many others.
Matlatzinca, or more specifically San Francisco Matlatzinca, is an endangered Oto-Manguean language of Western Central Mexico. The name of the language in the language itself is pjiekak'joo. The term "Matlatzinca" comes from the town's name in Nahuatl, meaning "the lords of the network." At one point, the Matlatzinca groups were called "pirindas," meaning "those in the middle."
Swahili is a Bantu language which is native to or mainly spoken in the East African region. It has a grammatical structure that is typical for Bantu languages, bearing all the hallmarks of this language family. These include agglutinativity, a rich array of noun classes, extensive inflection for person, tense, aspect and mood, and generally a subject–verb–object word order.
The grammar of the Massachusett language shares similarities with the grammars of related Algonquian languages. Nouns have gender based on animacy, based on the Massachusett world-view of what has spirit versus what does not. A body would be animate, but the parts of the body are inanimate. Nouns are also marked for obviation, with nouns subject to the topic marked apart from nouns less relevant to the discourse. Personal pronouns distinguish three persons, two numbers, inclusive and exclusive first-person plural, and proximate/obviative third-persons. Nouns are also marked as absentative, especially when referring to lost items or deceased persons.
Early Romani is the latest common predecessor of all forms of the Romani language. It was spoken before the Roma people dispersed throughout Europe. It is not directly attested, but rather reconstructed on the basis of shared features of existing Romani varieties. Early Romani is thought to have been spoken in the Byzantine Empire between the 9th-10th and 13th-14th centuries.