Miskito grammar

Last updated

The Miskito language, the language of the Miskito people of the Atlantic coast of Nicaragua and Honduras, is a member of the Misumalpan language family and also a strongly Germanic-influenced language.[ citation needed ] Miskito is as widely spoken in Honduras and Nicaragua as Spanish, it is also an official language in the Atlantic region of these countries. With more than 8 million speakers, Miskito has positioned in the second place in both countries after Spanish. Miskito is not only spoken in Central America, but in Europe (United Kingdom, Germany, Sweden, Netherlands, Spain, Switzerland, France and Italy), the USA, Canada and in many other Latin American countries. Miskito used to be a royal state language in the 16th to 19th dynasties of the Miskito Kingdom.

Contents

Miskito alphabet

The Miskito alphabet is the same as the English alphabet. It has 21 consonants and 5 vowels.

A (a), B (be), C (ce), D (de), E (e), F (ef), G (ge), H (ha), I (i), J (jei), K (ka), L (el), M (em), N (en), O (o), P (pi), Q (ku), R (ar), S (es), T (te), U (yoo), V (vee), W (dubilu), X (eks), Y (yei), Z (zet).

Phonology

Phonemes

Vowels
Short Long
Front Back Front Back
High i u î û
Low a â
Consonants
Bilabial Alveolar Palatal Velar Glottal
Nasals voiced m n ŋ ng
voiceless mh nh ŋ̊ ngh
Plosives voiceless p t k
voiced b d
Fricatives s h
Liquids voiced l , r
voiceless lh, rh
Semivowels j y w

Suprasegmentals

Word stress is generally on the first syllable of each word.

H has been included on the above consonant chart out of deference to the orthography and previous descriptions, but may in fact represent a suprasegmental feature rather than a consonantal phoneme (except in loanwords such as heven 'heaven'). Occurrence of h is restricted to the stressed syllable in a word, and its realization consists of the devoicing of adjacent vowel and consonant phonemes within that syllable. In spelling it is customary to place the letter h at the end of the syllable so affected.
  • lih 'turtle'
  • naha 'this'
  • pihni 'white'
  • banhta 'roof'
  • walhwal 'four'
  • banghkaia 'to fill'

Phonotactics

Syllables may have up to two consonants preceding the vowel nucleus, and two following it. This may be represented by the formula (C)(C)V(C)(C). Examples of monosyllabic words:

  • ba 'definite article'
  • ya? 'who?'
  • an? 'how many?'
  • wal 'two'
  • plun 'food'
  • puls! 'play!'
  • praks! 'close it!'

Within words of more than one syllable interior clusters may therefore contain more than two consonants (rarely more than three), but in such cases there is generally a morpheme boundary involved:

  • wamtla 'your house'
  • alkbia 'he will take it'

Simplification of underlying consonant clusters in verb forms takes place, with stem consonants disappearing when certain suffixes are added to verb stems of certain phonological shapes:

  • sab-aia 'perforate' → Imperative sa-s, Negative imperative sa-para, Future II 2 sa-ma, 3 sa-bia, Different-subject participle 3 sa-ka
  • atk-aia 'buy' → Imperative at-s, Negative imperative at-para, Future II 2 at-ma, 3 at-bia, Different-subject participle 3 at-ka

Noun phrase

Determiners and quantifiers

Common determiners and quantifiers
Some determinersSome quantifiers
  • ba definite article
  • na proximal definite article
  • kum indefinite article
  • kumkum plural indefinite article
  • naha 'this'
  • baha 'that'
  • naura 'close by'
  • bukra 'over there'
  • ani 'which'
  • dia 'what'
  • kumkum 'some'
  • uya, ailal, manis 'many'
  • wala 'other'
  • sut 'all'
  • an 'how many'
  • kumi, wan 'one'
  • wal, tu 'two'
  • yumhpa, tri 'three'
  • walhwal, for 'four'
  • matsip, faip 'five'

The demonstratives naha, baha, naura, bukra and the interrogative determiners ani and dia precede the noun they determine and require the ligature (see below).

  • naha araska 'this horse'
  • baha araska 'that horse'
  • ani araska? 'which horse?'

The indefinite article and most quantifiers follow the noun and do not require a ligature.

  • aras kum 'one horse, a horse'
  • aras kumkum 'some horses'
  • aras an? 'how many horses?'
  • aras yumhpa 'three horses'

The general article ba and the proximal article na stand at the end of the noun phrase and require no ligature. The proximal article expresses proximity.

  • aras ba 'the horse'
  • utla na 'the house'
  • papiki atkan araska na 'this horse that my father bought'

Optionally the article may be combined with other determiners or quantifiers, and with the ligature (which seems to convey a greater degree of definiteness).

  • naha araska na 'this horse'
  • baha araska ba 'that horse'
  • aras kum ba 'there is one horse'
  • araska ba 'the horse in question, the horse which...'

The determiners are used sometimes with pronouns to emphasize the subject in question.

  • Witin ba patkira sa. 'He is guilty.'

Ligature

Ligature is a term (with precedents in other languages) for describing a grammatical feature of Miskito traditionally referred to with less accuracy in the Miskito context as 'construct'. A ligature is a morpheme (often -ka) which occurs when a noun is linked to some other element in the noun phrase. In Miskito, most of the elements that require the presence of ligature are ones that precede the head noun:

Elements triggering linking
TypeExample
Determiners
  • baha araska 'that horse'
Adjectives
  • araskakarna ba 'the horse is strong'
Dependent possessors
  • Johan araska 'Johan's horse'
Relative clauses
  • Kati atkan araska ba 'the horse that Kati bought'

Ligature takes a variety of forms:

Forms of ligature
FormExamples
-ka suffix
  • aras 'horse' → araska
  • kabu 'sea' → kabuka
  • piuta 'snake' → piutka
-ika suffix
  • kipla 'rock' → kiplika
-ya suffix
  • tasba 'land' → tasbaya
  • tala 'blood' → talia
-a- INFIX
  • silak 'needle' → sialaka (< *si-a-laka)
  • utla 'house' → watla (< *w-a-tla)
-ka suffix + -a- INFIX
  • duri 'boat' → duarka (< *du-a-r-ka)
-ya suffix + -a- INFIX
  • sula 'deer' → sualia (< *su-a-l-ia)
irregular
  • plun 'food' → pata
  • diara 'thing' → dukia

Some nouns take no ligature morpheme; these mostly denote parts of the body (e.g. bila 'mouth', napa 'tooth', kakma 'nose') or kinship (e.g. lakra 'opposite-sex sibling'), although there is only an imperfect correlation between membership of this morphological class and semantic inalienability (see also relationals below).

Possession

A noun phrase possessor precedes the possessed noun with ligature (unless inalienable, see above).

  • Mark araska 'Mark's horse'
  • Mark napa 'Mark's tooth'

The possessor may be a personal pronoun if it is emphasized.

  • yang araski 'my horse'
  • mannan nampa 'your (pl.) tooth'

Such pronouns may be omitted. In either case, personal possessors are grammaticalized as morphological indices.

  • araski 'my/our (exc.) horse'
  • nampa 'your (sg./pl.) tooth'
Possessive indices
preposed particlesuffix forminfix form
1-i-i-'my, our (exc.)'
2-m-m-'your (sg./pl.)'
3ai'his, her, its, their'
1+2wan'our (inc.)'

Ai and wan precede the noun, with ligature unless inalienable.

(aras→araska)

(bila)

  • ai araska 'his... horse'
  • ai bila 'his mouth'
  • wan araska 'our (inc.) horse'
  • wan bila 'our (inc.) mouth'

The indices for first or second person are generally suffixed to the -ka or -ya ligature when either is present (with loss of final -a before -i):

(aras→araska)

(tasba→tasbaya)

  • araski 'my... horse'
  • tasbayi 'my land'
  • araskam 'your horse'
  • tasbayam 'your land'

Otherwise they are mostly infixed after the infixed ligature -a-:

(utla→watla:)

(sula→sualia:)

  • waitla 'my house'
  • suailia 'my deer'
  • wamtla 'your house'
  • suamlia 'your deer'

Nouns of the inalienable class (with no ligature) take the same possessive indices, which may again be either suffixed:

(bila)

  • bili 'my/our (exc.) mouth'
  • bilam 'your mouth'

...or infixed:

(napa)

  • naipa 'my/our (exc.) tooth'
  • nampa 'your tooth'

Some nouns infix in the first person but suffix in the second, and there are some other miscellaneous irregularities.

(kakma)

(duri→duarka)

  • kaikma 'my/our (exc.) nose'
  • duairka 'my/our (exc.) boat'
  • kangmkma 'your nose'
  • duarmka 'your boat'

The plural

Number is not a morphological category in Miskito. Plural number is indicated in noun phrases by the particle nani or -nan placed after the noun or pronoun. Nani is optional with numerals.

  • yang nani or yangnan 'we (exc.)'
  • baha nani or bahanan 'those'
  • naha watla nani na or naha watlanan na 'these houses'
  • uplika karna nani ba or upla karnanan ba 'the strong people'
  • aras (nani) yumhpa 'three horses'

Adjectives

Adjectives used attributively usually follow the head noun and do not require a ligature:

  • aras pihni ba 'the white horse'

but some (including past participles) precede it, in which case the noun, unless inalienable, takes its ligature:

  • aras karna ba 'the strong horse'
  • upla pruannan ba 'dead people'

Pronouns and adverbs

The personal pronouns differentiate three persons and also have an exclusive/inclusive distinction in the first person plural. The general plural morpheme nani or -nan is added to form plurals (except with yawan). Use of these pronouns is optional when person is indexed in the possessed form, relational or verb group.

Personal pronouns
SingularPlural

yang 'I/me'

man 'you'

witin (neutral) 'he/him, she/her, it'

yang nani or yangnan 'we/us (exclusive)'

man nani or mannan 'you'

witin nani or witinnan 'they/them'

yawan 'we/us (inclusive)'

The pronouns are not case-specific, and may, under comparable conditions, be marked by the same postpositions as other noun phrases.

Other pronouns and deictic adverbs
PronounsPlace adverbsOther adverbs
Demonstrative
  • naha 'this'
  • baha 'that'
  • nahara, naura 'here'
  • bahara, bukra 'there'
  • naku, nan 'like this'
  • baku, ban 'like that, so'
  • mahka, nanara, nawas 'now'
  • bara 'then'
Interrogative
  • ya? 'who?'
  • dia? 'what?'
  • dikia? 'what? (construct)'
  • ani? 'which one?'
  • anira? 'where?'
  • nahki? 'how?'
  • ahkia? 'when?'
Negative polarity
  • diara kumsin 'anything'
  • upla kumsin 'anybody'
  • kumsin 'any' (det.)
  • plis kumsin 'anywhere'
  • piu kumsin 'ever'

Postpositions

Dative complements are marked by the multi-purpose enclitic postposition ra, which is also a locative (doing duty for both dative and spatial meanings of English 'to', as well as 'in'). The same marker is also often used with direct objects.

  • Locative ra:Nikaragua ra auna. 'I am going to Nicaragua.' Honduras ra iwisna. 'I live in Honduras.'
  • Dative ra:Yang Joly ra buk kum yabri. 'I gave Joly a book.' "I Joly-ra book one gave.'
  • Accusative ra:Yang Joly ra kaikri. 'I saw Joly.' "I Joly-ra saw."

This and other postpositions are placed after the last element in a noun phrase, e.g.

  • aras ra 'to the horse'
  • aras pihni ra 'to the white horse'
  • aras pihni kum ra 'to a white horse'
Some postpositions
ra (enclitic)'to, in, at...'

(see above)

kat

wina

'to, as far as'

'from'

  • Bilwi wina Lempira kat 'from Bilwi to Lempira'
  • utla wina 'out of the house'
wal'with (general)'
  • Piter tuktika ba wal 'with Piter's child'
  • rais bins wal 'rice with beans'
  • baha lalahka wal 'with that money'
ni'with (instrumental)'
  • naha lalahka ni 'with this money'
  • bip taya ni '(made) of leather ("cow skin")'

Relationals

Relationals are quasi-nouns expressing some relationship (often spatial) to their possessor complement. Many of the relationals perceivably originate in locatives (in -ra) of nouns designating parts of the body employed metaphorically to convey spatial or other relations.

For example, utla bilara literally means 'in the mouth of the house'.

  • utla bila-ra 'inside the house'

Relationals index pronominal complements in the same way as nouns index their possessors.

  • ai bila-ra 'inside him/her/them'
  • (yang) bili-ra 'inside me'
  • (man) bilam-ra 'inside you'

Some examples of relationals in use:

  • Witin yang ninira balan. 'He came behind me.'
  • Man nani kainamra Bilwi ra wamna. 'I will go to Bilwi before you (pl.).'
  • Witin dur lamara takaskan. 'He paused near the door.'
  • Naha batilka utla bilara mangkaisna. 'I will put this bottle inside the house.'
  • Upla aihkika witin dukiara but munan. 'Most people voted for him.'
Some relationals
Spatial relationsOther relations
  • bila-ra 'in, inside'
  • pura(-ra) 'on, on top of'
  • mununhta-ra 'under'
  • kaina-ra 'in front of'
  • nina-ra 'behind'
  • tila-ra 'between, among'
  • lama-ra 'near'
  • dukia-ra 'for, about'
  • mapa-ra 'for, against, as regards'
  • watlika-ra 'instead of'
  • tawan 'because of'

Verbal groups

Overview

Verbs are conventionally cited with the infinitive suffix -aia. The stem of many such verbs (obtained by subtracting the infinitive ending) are monosyllabic (bal-, dim-, tak-, dauk-, kaik-, bri-, wi-, pi- etc.); a few are non-syllabic (e.g. w- 'go').

  • balaia 'come'
  • waia 'go'
  • dimaia 'go in'
  • takaia 'go out'
  • daukaia 'make'
  • munaia 'do'
  • briaia 'have'
  • kaikaia 'see'
  • aisaia 'speak'
  • wiaia 'tell'
  • walaia 'hear'
  • piaia 'eat'
  • yapaia 'sleep'

Finite forms include several tenses and moods, in each of which the person (but not number) of the subject is marked by suffixes. The tenses themselves have characteristic suffixes which combine with the subject-indexing suffixes.

In addition to synthetic (simple) tenses, there is also a considerable range of periphrastic (compound) tenses. These are formed with a non-finite form of the main verb followed by an auxiliary verb.

Some of the synthetic tenses represent original periphrastic tense structures that have become welded into single words. This helps to explain why there are two different forms each in the present, past and future. (The sample verb used is pulaia 'play', stem pul-, given here in the third-person form of each tense.)

III

Present tenses:

puluya

pulisa

Past tenses:

pulata

pulan

Future tenses:

pulaisa

pulbia

In addition to a subject index which form part of a verb's suffix, for transitive verbs the verb group includes an object index in the form of a preverbal particle marking the person (but not the number) of the object. The subject markers vary somewhat according to the tense, but the most usual forms are shown in the following table (see below for more details).

Subject and object indices
PersonSubject

suffixes

Object

particles

1-naai
2-mamai
3

-a

1+2wan

Presence of the personal pronouns (yang, man, witin, yawan, yang nani...) referring to the indexed subject or object is optional (i.e. there is pro-drop).

  • Mai kaikisna. 'I see you.'

The absence of an object index preceding a transitive verb signals a third person object:

  • Kaikisna. 'I see him/her/it/them'
  • Waitla kaikisna. 'I see my house.'

Other participant roles may be expressed by personal pronouns with the appropriate postpositions, e.g.

  • Yang ra tri paun aiks. 'Give me three pounds.'
  • Man wal aisaia want sna 'I want to talk to ("with") you.'

Forms for a third-person subject, in addition to indexing specific subjects that are equivalent to 'he', 'she', 'it', 'they' or 'we (inclusive)', are also used with transitive verbs to indicate a non-specific subject, thus providing a passive-like construction.

  • Ai kaikan. 'He (etc.) saw me' but also 'I was seen.'

To indicate that a verb has a plural subject, a finite auxiliary, banghwaia, may be added at the end of the verb group, preceded by a same-subject participle.

  • Maria ra kaiki banghwri. 'We (exc.) saw María.'

Conjugation

The stem of a verb is obtained by removing the -aia suffix from the infinitive. Most verb stems end in a consonant, and are conjugated as follows (our sample verb is pulaia 'play').

Regular verb
Present IPresent IIPast IPast IIFuture IFuture IIImperative
1pulunapulisnapulatnapulripulaisnapulamna
2pulumapulismapulatmapulrampulaismapulmapuls
3 and 1+2puluyapulisapulatapulanpulaisapulbia

Verbs whose stems end in i (bri- 'have', wi- 'tell', pi- 'eat', di- 'drink', swi- 'allow') vary from the above paradigm in a few minor points. Bal-aia 'come' and w-aia 'go', have an irregular Present I tense. The verb yabaia 'give' is anomalous in a different way by having irregularly derived non-third-person object-indexing forms. Finally, the most irregular verb of all is the defective and irregular kaia 'to be'.

i-stem (piaia 'eat')
Present IPresent IIPast IPast IIFuture IFuture IIImperative
1pisunapisnapisatnapiripiaisnapimna
2pisumapismapisatmapirampiaismapimapis
3 and 1+2pisuyapisapisatapinpiaisapibia
Balaia, waia
Present of balaia 'come'Present of waia 'go'
1aulnaauna
2aulmaauma
3 / 1+2aulaauya
Stems of yabaia 'give'
Object1231+2
Infinitiveaik-aia

'give me/us'

maik-aia

'give you'

yab-aia

'give him/her/it/them'

wank-aia

'give us (inc.)'

kaia 'be'
PresentPast IPast IIFuture IFuture IIImperative
1snakatnakaprikaisnakamna
2smakatmakapramkaismakamabas
3 / 1+2sakatakankaisakabia

Use of tenses

Present I expresses that an action is happening or about to happen at the time of speaking.

  • Yang Miskitu aisuna. 'I am speaking Miskito.'
  • Yang naha minit takuna. 'I am about to go out this minute.'

Present II is a general present, indistinctly progressive or habitual.

  • Yang Miskitu aisisna. 'I speak Miskito.'

Past I is a perfect.

  • Yang kuirku ba dakakatna. 'I have fed the pigs.'

As the nucleus of a main clause Past II is a simple aorist past. Connected to a following verb in a past or present tense within a switch reference chain, it functions as the different-subject participle (see below).

  • Yang Meri ra kaikri. 'I saw Meri.'
  • Yang buk nani ba sakri witin Meri ra yaban. 'I found the books and he gave them to Meri.' (or 'I having found the books...')

Future I expresses that an event is imminent.

  • Jon pruaisa. 'Jon is going to die.'

Future II is a general future. It is also used as an irrealis in subordinate clauses.

  • Man naha apilka pima kaka, man pruma. 'If you eat this apple, you will die.'
  • Yawan anira wabia? 'Where shall we go?'
  • Jon wantkan Meri balbia. 'Jon wanted Meri to come.'

The second-person imperative ends in -s; its negative (prohibitive) counterpart ending in -para. A gentler order may be expressed using the Past II second-person form (ending in -ram). The first-person inclusive plural imperative ('Let's...') ends in -p(i).

  • Sturi nani kumkum yang nani ra ai wis! 'Tell us some stories!'
  • Baku pali saura ai munpara. 'Do not treat me like that!'
  • Umpira ai kaiks! 'Have pity on me!'

Switch reference and non-finite verb forms

Switch reference participlesNegative participlePast participleInfinitive
Same subjectSame subject anteriorDifferent subject
past/present (= Past II)
Different subject future
Regular1pulipulisipulripulrikapulraspulanpulaia
2pulram
3 / 1+2pulanpulka
kaia 'to be'1sikaprikaprikakankaia
2kapram
3 / 1+2kankaka

The switch reference participles are used in verb or clause chains sharing the same subject; only the last verb adopts a finite tense form.

  • Utla wina taki kauhri. 'I fell coming out of the house.' ("Coming out of the house — I fell.")

These participles are also used in many compound verbs and periphrastic formations.

  • bri balaia 'to bring' ("to have and come")
  • puli kan 'he was playing'

The anterior participle further expresses that an event occurred before that expressed by the following verb.

  • Watla ra dimisi witin wal aisari. 'After entering the house I spoke to him.' ("Having entered the house — I spoke with him."

The different-subject participle in -ka signals a change of subject between it and the following verb, and is used when the latter is in a future tense.

  • Paul buk nani ba sakka witin Slilma ra yabia. 'When Pauli finds the books, hej will give them to Slilma.'

When the subject of the different subject participle is first or second person, the ending is -rika if the main verb is future.

  • Man yarika takbia. 'You will let it out.' ("You letting, it will get out.")

When the last verb of a different-subject chain is in the present or past tense, the preceding verb must be in the Past II tense.

  • Paul buk nani ba sakan witin Slilma ra yaban. 'When Pauli found the books, hej gave them to Slilma.'

The negative participle can be followed by a finite form of kaia to express any person-tense combination; alternatively these categories may be left implicit by omitting the auxiliary.

  • Man ai kaikras kapram. 'You did not see me.'
  • Man ai kaikras. 'You do/did/will not see me.'

The past participle, identical in form to the third-person of Past II, is used: (a) as a passive adjective;

  • Satail ba bip tawa ni daukan kan. 'The saddle is made of cow's hide.'

(b) in a periphrastic passive construction with kaia as auxiliary;

  • Yang kupran kapri. 'I was beaten.'

(c) in an idiomatic construction with daukaia 'make'.

  • Mai kaikan ai daukisa. 'I would like to see you.' ("Seen you makes me.")

The uses of the infinitive: (a) approximates that of infinitives in many European languages:

  • Aikuki la dauki banghwan tasba pis kum atkaia. 'They made a joint agreement to purchase a piece of land.'
  • Pedro ai muihni nani aikuki aisaia wan. 'Pedro went and spoke with his brothers.'
  • Diara sut brisma, dia mita wark pliki waia? 'You have everything, why go and look for work?'

(b) include several modal constructions.

  • Yang wamtla ra waia want sna. 'I want to go to your house.'
  • Yang wamtla ra waia sna. 'I have to go to your house.'
  • Yang wamtla ra waia kapri. 'I should have gone to your house.'

Note: Given the differences in terminology, the following comparative table for names of non-finite forms used in this article, Salamanca's Miskito school grammar and Green's Lexicographic Study of Ulwa (a related language with similar categories) may be found useful:

This articleSalamancaGreen
same subject simultaneous participle'gerundio''proximate'
same subject anterior participle'transgresivo'
different subject future participle'conexivo''obviative'

Periphrastic tenses

The range of aspectual, modal and other notions that can be expressed is enlarged considerably by the availability of various periphrastic constructions in which a verb acting as auxiliary is placed after the main verb. The conjugated component can take a variety of tenses, including periphrastic ones, and the periphrases themselves may often be combined; thus chains of several auxiliaries are possible. Some representative examples of such periphrases follow:

Puli kapri 'I was playing' consists of the same-subject participle of pulaia followed by the first person of Past II of kaia 'to be', "playing was-I".

  • puli kapri 'I was playing' ("playing was-I")

Pulaia sna 'I am to play, I have to play' consists of kaia after an infinitive.

  • pulaia sna 'I am to play' ("play-to am-I")

This construction with the auxiliary in Past II can express an impossible condition: pulaia kapri 'I should have played' or 'I would have played'.

  • pulaia kapri 'I should/would have played' ("play-to was-I")

Combining the infinitive with other auxiliary verbs we obtain other modal constructions.

  • pulaia want sna 'I want to play' ("play-to want am-I")

The particle sip, with an anomalous distribution, is used in expressions of possibility and ability.

  • Witin sip sa utla kum paskaia. 'He can build a house.'
  • Witin sip utla kum paskras sa. 'He cannot build a house.'

Another type of construction consists of a conjugated main verb followed by a third-person form of kaia. Various tense sequences for the two verbs are possible and convey a range of nuances. Past perfect and future perfect can be expressed by placing both verbs in Past II or future II respectively.

  • pulri kan 'I had played'
  • pulamna kabia 'I shall have played'

By compounding the past perfect construction again with sa, and then kaka for 'if' (itself really a form of kaia), we obtain an unfulfilled hypothetical clause.

  • pulri kan sa kaka 'if I had played'

Syntax

Word order

In Miskito sentences the verb (or verb group) regularly comes last. The subject, if expressed as a noun phrase, normally precedes objects and other constituents. In these examples the verb is in bold.

  • Jon pruaisa. 'Jon is going to die.'
  • (Yang) Honduras ra iwisna. 'I live in Honduras.'
  • (Yang) Meri ra kaikri. 'I saw Meri.'
  • Baha ya daukan? 'Who did this?'
  • (Yang) Meri ra buk kum yabri. 'I gave Meri a book.'
  • Mairin ba tuktan ra li ni tahbisa. 'The woman is bathing the baby with water.'
  • Yang ra tri paun aik. 'Give me three pounds.'

However, long or heavier constituents (here in bold) may follow the verb.

  • Yang witin ra yabaia wantsna. 'I want to give it to him/her.'
  • Naha mani champianshipka ba ya wintakbia ni? 'Who do you think is going to win the championship this year?'

Demonstrative and interrogative determiners, the possessive proclitics ai and wan, and certain adjectives, precede the noun, which takes the ligature in these cases.

  • baha araska 'that horse'
  • ani araska? 'which horse?'
  • ai araska 'his/her horse'
  • aras karna ba 'the strong horse'

Articles and quantifiers follow nouns.

  • aras ba 'the horse'
  • aras kum 'a/one horse'
  • aras uya 'many horses'
  • aras an? 'how many horses?'

Adpositions and relationals follow the noun phrase.

  • aras ba wal 'with the horse'
  • aras ba kainara 'in front of the horse'

Auxiliaries follow main verbs.

  • puli kapri 'I was playing'
  • pulri kan 'I had played'
  • pulaia want sna 'I want to play'

The object proclitics ai, mai and wan precede the main verb.

  • mai kaikisna 'I see you'

The negative particle apia follows future-tense verbs, but precedes forms of kaia 'to be'.

  • Ai kaikma apia. 'You will not see me.'
  • Baha watla tawanki ra apia sa. 'That house is not in my village.'

In compound verbs, the conjugated element comes last.

  • aisi kaikri 'I read it' ("speaking I-saw")
  • bri aulna 'I am bringing it' ("having I-am-coming")
  • want sna 'I want it'
  • win takbia 'he/she/it will win'

Sentence particles follow the verb.

  • Man balma ki? 'Will you come?'
  • Ya win takbia ni? 'I wonder who will win!'

In subordination structures the rule that places subordinate elements first is frequently overridden by a tendency to place long and heavy constituents last.

  • Watla ra dimisi witin wal aisari. 'After entering the house I spoke to him.'
  • Piter buknan ba sakan tem, Slilma ra yabri. 'When Piter found the books, I gave them to Slilma.'
  • Johan wantkan Kati balbia. 'Johan wanted Kati to come.'

Relative clauses precede the head.

  • Meri atkan araska ba 'the horse that Meri bought'

Complement and circumstantial clauses may precede or follow the main clause.

  • Plawar abalkaisna brid daukaia mata. 'I am going to mix flour in order to make bread.'
  • Witin plun atkaia auya kan bara, ai yaptika ba balan. 'When he was about to buy food, his mother came.'

Propositional structure

While no systematic case marking differentiates formally between subjects and objects, there exist (apart from word order) certain option for achieving disambiguation.

One is to mark animate direct objects with the postposition ra.

  • Meri ra kaikri. 'I saw Meri.'

Another is to identify the agent of a transitive verb with the postposition mita. Since mita always occurs with agents of transitive verbs it might be viewed as a proto-ergative marker.

  • Puisin mita ikan. 'The poison killed him/her.'
  • Piter mita Meri wamtlara brih wan. 'Piter brought Meri to your house.'

Yet another way to identify the subject is for it to participate in a verbal periphrasis. Outwardly, the 'particle' bui is placed after such subjects. Bui is the same-subject participle of buaia 'get up', so the semantic route of this grammaticalization is, for example, from 'Who will get up and remove it?' to 'Who (subject) will remove it?' The use of bui allows an object to precede a subject (for topicalization) without this leading to ambiguity. Bui almost always occurs with subjects of transitive verbs and so may again be understood as a proto-ergative marker.

  • Prari bui duri abakan. 'The hurricane sunk the canoe.'
  • bui ai kangban? 'Who touched me?'

Information structure

A system of specialized postpositions is used to identify topics and focused constituents:

Lika is a particle that may follow a sentence constituent identifying it as sentence topic.

  • Baku lika yang maipara an man mampara sin aitani kabia. 'That way, it will be good for me and for you too.'
  • Yang nini lika Juan. 'My name is Juan.'
  • Pedro mahka wan, bara María lika takaskan. 'Pedro left, and/but María stayed.'

Sika may be placed after a definite noun phrase to foreground it; its effect is similar to that of focus clefting in English.

  • Naha sika diara nani na dawanka kabia. 'This is who is going to own these things.'
  • Witin sika yaptiki. 'She is my mother.'

Valency

Most verbs are built up from a monosyllabic lexical root ending in a vowel or a single consonant, to which an extension or stem consonant is very often added. The extensions correlate with transitivity: transitive stems have either -k- or -b- (unpredictably), while intransitive stems have -w-. There is also a valency-decreasing verb-prefix ai- which, added to transitive stems, produces unergative, reflexive, reciprocal or middle verbs. See the section on Derivation (below) for examples.

Miskito has periphrastic causative expressions using one or another of the causation verbs yabaia 'give', munaia 'make', swiaia 'let'. In these constructions, the verb of causation is subordinated to the verb of action.

  • Pedro tuktika ra swika pulbia. 'Pedro will let the child play.' "P. letting the child, (it) will play"

Negation

To negate a verb, the invariable negative participle in -ras is used either alone or followed by an auxiliary specifying tense and person.

  • Man ai kaikras. 'You do/did/will not see me.'
  • Man ai kaikras kapram. 'You did not see me.'

For the future tenses only, another option is to place apia after the future verb form.

  • Man ai kaikma apia. or Man ai kaikras kama. 'You will not see me.'

The second person imperative has its own special negative form, with the verbal suffix -para.

  • Baku yang nanira ai wipara. 'Do not speak to us like that!'

The verb kaia, having no negative participle, is negated by a preposed apia.

  • Yang aitani apia sna. 'I am not worthy.'

'Nothing', 'nobody' and so on are expressed using indefinite words, generally accompanied by sin 'also, even', usually in combination with negative verb forms.

  • Upla kumi sin balras. 'Nobody came.'
  • Yang upla kumi sin ra kaikras. 'I didn't see anybody.'
  • Muihki upla kumi ra sin diara wiras. 'My brother did not tell anybody anything.'
  • Pyu kumi sin sîka nît apia kaka dipara. 'Never take medicine if you do not need it.'

Questions

Question words
  • ya 'who'
  • dia 'what'
  • ani 'which'
  • an 'how many'
  • anira 'where'
  • ahkia 'when'
  • nahki 'how'
  • diakan 'why'

The sentence-final particle ki may, optionally, be used in either yes-no or wh-questions.

  • Man balma ki? 'Will you come?'
  • Buk an brisma ki? 'How many books do you have?'

With or without ki, in wh-questions the interrogative element either stands at the beginning of the question...

  • Ya baha daukan? 'Who did this?'
  • Diakan man baku lukisma ki? 'Why do you think so?'

...or immediately precedes the verb.

  • Yawan anira wabia? 'Where shall we go?'
  • Inska ba wal dia daukamna ki? 'What shall I do with the fish?'

'who' as the agentive subject of a question may be followed by the bui marker (see above).

  • Ya bui ai kangban? 'Who touched me?'

Indirect questions may be followed by saba (or sapa).

  • Witin wan dia daukan saba kaikaia. 'He went to see what he had done.'

In yes–no questions sentence-final ki is optional. Such questions may be answered with au 'yes' or apia 'no'.

  • Man sma ki? — Au, yang sika. 'Is it you? — Yes, it's me.'
  • Man nani naha sut kaikisma? 'Do you (pl.) see all this?'

Sentence mood particles

Sentence-final mood particles
  • bika surprise, exclamation
  • ni 'I wonder'
  • ki question, surprise

Mood particles may be placed at the end of a sentence (i.e. following the verb). See the example of ki above.

  • Yang baku sma bika! 'Why, you are like me!'
  • Wimna kaka laubia ni? 'I wonder if he'll get angry if I tell him.'

Coordinating conjunctions

Coordinating conjunctions
  • bara, bamna, an 'and'
  • apia kaka, o, ar 'or'
  • sakuna, kuna 'but'
  • Kumi ba sirpi kan bara wala ba tara kan. 'One was small and the other was big.'
  • Juan an Pedro talia sa. 'Juan and Pedro are similar.'
  • Ai aisika, apia kaka ai yaptika wal aisaia sa. 'It is necessary to talk to his father or his mother.'
  • Witin aisan sakuna yang tanka briras. 'He spoke but I didn't understand.'

Relative clauses

There are two major constructions which may be used to form relative clauses in Miskito, the 'external head' strategy and the 'internal head' strategy.

In the external head strategy there is no subordination marker of any kind and the relative clause precedes the head noun, which takes a ligature, beside which it usually has an article too.

  • Sarah atkan watla ba Bilwi ra sa. 'The house that Sarah bought is in Bilwi.'
  • Naha tawanka ra truk kum bri uplika manis bara sa. 'In this town there are a lot of people who own a car.'

If the head is not expressed, an article following the relative clause serves to identify and delimit it.

  • Aras ra alkan nani ba bui asiki ra brih wan. 'Those who caught the horse took it to my father.'

In the internal head construction, the head noun is not extracted from the place it underlyingly occupies in the relative clause, which is bounded by an article as in headless external head clauses.

  • Sarah watla atkan ba Bilwi ra sa. 'The house that Sarah bought is in Bilwi.' (as if to say: "The Sarah bought the house is in Bilwi.")

In the 'headless' counterpart of the internal construction, the place of the head within the relative clause is occupied by an interrogative pronoun.

  • Dia makama ba, yang maikamna. 'I will give you what you ask.'

Complement clauses

A complement clause may bear no subordination marker but merely be followed by the article ba functioning in practice as a nominalizer.

  • Yang nahwala waitna kum ra ikan ba nu takri. 'I have heard the news that a man was killed yesterday.'

Indirect questions end in saba (i.e. sa 'is' + ba article).

  • Witin wan dia daukan saba kaikaia. 'He went to see what he had done.'

The tense of complement clauses does not follow that of the matrix clause, but directly expresses a time relation in reference to the matrix.

  • Witin nani walan Pedro ba raya sa. 'They heard that Pedro was alive.' ("...that P. is alive")

Complement clauses that have no autonomous time reference ('irrealis') take Future II.

  • Jon Sarah balbia wantkan. 'Jon wanted Sarah to come.'

Conditional and concessive clauses

Conditional ('if') clauses add kaka and precede the consequence clause. (Kaka is the third-person different subject participle of kaia 'be', literally "it being (the case that)".)

  • Man naha apilka pima kaka, man pruma. 'If you eat this apple, you will die.'
  • Yang naha tasba wina katna kaka, aiklabaia kapri. 'If I were from this land, I would fight.'
  • Witin nahwala sula kum kaikan kan sa kaka, ikaia kan. 'If he had seen a deer yesterday, he would have killed it.'

Concessive ('although') clauses may end in sin 'also, either, even', or in sakuna 'but'.

  • Aisikam nani balbia apia, yang witin nani ra bik takamna sin. 'Your parents will not come, even if I beg them to.'
  • Piter wintakaisa, witin saura pali sa sakuna. 'Piter will win, even though he is very bad.'

Circumstantial clauses

Circumstantial clauses generally end in a subordinating conjunction of some sort. Sometimes the article ba precedes the conjunction, which may take the form of a preposition...

  • * Witin plun atkaia auya kan bara, ai yaptika ba balan. 'When he was about to buy food, his mother came.' (bara = ba + ra)

a relational...

  • Baha daukaia dukiara diara manis nit sa. 'In order to do that, many things are needed.'
  • Plawar abalkaisna brid daukaia mata. 'I am going to mix flour in order to make bread.'
  • Plun piras kainara ai mihta sikban. 'Before eating food he washed his hands.'

or a noun.

  • Yang buk kum aisi kaiki kapri taim, man bal dimram. 'When I was reading a book, you came in.'

Lexicon

General

As regards origin, the Miskito lexicon consists of the following principal components:

Derivation

Some derivational affixes:

AffixFunctionMeaningExamples
-ira suffix(1) adjectives from nouns (with ligature)abundance
  • tawa 'hair' → taw-ira 'hairy'
  • kipla 'rock' → kipl-ikakipl-ik-ira 'rocky'
(2) adjectives from nominalized adjectives in -(i)kasuperlative
  • karna 'strong' → karn-ikakarn-ik-ira 'very strong'
  • sirpi 'small' → sirpi-kasirpi-k-ira 'very small'
  • tara 'big' → tar-katar-k-ira 'very big'
-s suffixadjectives from nouns (with ligature)privative, '-less'
  • napa 'tooth' → napa-s 'toothless'
  • tangni 'flower' → tangni-katangni-ka-s 'flowerless'
  • walpa 'stone' → walpa-yawalpa-ya-s 'stoneless'
-(i)ka suffixnouns from adjectivesabstract nouns, '-ness' (cf. ligature)
  • karna 'strong' → karn-ika 'strength'
  • ingni 'bright' → ingni-ka 'brightness'
-(i)ra suffixnouns from adjectivesabstract nouns, '-ness'
  • sirpi 'small' → sirpi-ra 'smallness'
  • siksa 'black' → siks-ira 'blackness'
-aika suffixnouns from verbs(1) instrument
  • pahb-aia 'sweep' → pahb-aika 'broom'
(2) place
  • plap-aia 'run' → plap-aika 'track'
-anka suffixnouns from verbsaction (nominalized past participle)
  • pahb-aia 'sweep' → pahb-anka 'act of sweeping'
-ra suffixnouns from verbsaction
  • plap-aia 'run' → plap-ra 'running'
reduplication + -ra suffixnouns from verbs(1) agent, '-er'
  • plap-aia 'run' → pla-plap-ra 'runner'
(2) undergoer
  • raw-aia 'get better, be cured' → ra-raw-ra 'patient'
-b- or -k- suffix(1) verbs from verb rootstransitive verb
  • dak-b-aia 'cut (tr.)'
  • ra-k-aia 'cure (tr.)'
(2) verbs from adjective roots
  • rat-ni 'wet (adj.)' → rat-b-aia 'wet (tr.)'
-w- suffixverbsintransitive verb
  • dak-w-aia 'break (intr.)'
  • ra-w-aia 'be cured'
(2) verbs from adjective roots
  • ing-ni 'bright' → ing-w-aia 'shine'
ai- prefixintransitive verbs from transitivesreflexive or middle
  • sak-b-aia 'stretch (sth.) out' → ai-sak-b-aia 'lie down'
  • srung-k-aia 'cover' → ai-srung-k-aia 'cover oneself'

Lexical compounds

Miskito has a large number of light-verb constructions or compound verbs which consist of two words but express meanings that are lexically determined for the construction as a whole, e.g.

  • aisikaikaia 'read' ("speak and see")
  • bribalaia 'bring' ("have and come")
  • bilawalaia 'obey' ("hear word")
  • kupiabaikaia 'get angry' ("split heart")

A similar construction is used in verbs that are loans from English: the borrowed lexeme is an invariable element (help, wark, want...) followed by a Miskito verb, e.g.

  • helpmunaia 'help' ("do help")
  • warktakaia 'work' ("go out work")
  • wantkaia 'want' ("be want")

Nominal compounds are much less common.

  • bip mairin 'cow' ("beef/bovine female")

Typological overview

Phonology

Phoneme inventory

The Miskito phoneme inventory includes four vowels (a, e, i, o, u)[ clarification needed ], apparently with phonemic length playing a part. Consonant series include voiced and voiceless plosives, voiced nasals and semivowels, two liquids and the fricative s. Orthographic h apparently represents a suprasegmental feature.

Other aspects

Syllables consist of a vowel nucleus preceded and followed by a maximum of two consonant: (C)(C)V(C)(C). Word stress is normally on the first syllable and not distinctive.

Morphology

Inflectional and derivational morphology are of moderate complexity and predominantly suffixing, together with the use of infixes in the nominal paradigm.

Nominal morphology

The nominal morphological categories are ligature and person (but not number) of the possessor, the exponents of which have suffix and infix allophones, except for third person and first person inclusive possessor indices, which are preposed particles. Plural number is indicated by a postpositive particle.

Verbal morphology

In the verbal morphology, tense, mood and person (of the subject) are marked by suffixes (and sometimes fused into portmanteau suffix forms). Object indices of transitive verbs are represented by particles preceding the verb (third person is zero). Number is not marked in these subject and object indices, but a plural subject may be indicated through a verbal periphrasis serving this function.

Syntax

Word order

Word order
  • object + verb
  • determiner (except article) + head
  • head + article
  • head + quantifier
  • adjective + head / head + adjective
  • possessor + head
  • relative clause + head
  • head + postposition
  • verb + auxiliary
  • sentence + particle
  • subordinate clause + subordinator

Sentence order is predominantly SOV. Auxiliaries follow main verbs. Sentence particles are sentence-final. Within the noun phrase, most determiners precede the head, but articles follow it, as do quantifiers. Adjectives may either precede or follow the head noun. Possessors precede possessed, and relative clauses precede their head. The ligature morpheme generally occurs on the noun whenever this is preceded by one of the items mentioned, and also when it takes a possessive index. Postpositional structures are found.

Head or dependent marking

HEAD-marking constructions
  • subject + VERB
  • object + VERB
  • possessor + POSSESSED
  • noun + RELATIONAL

Miskito is consistently head-marking. There is pro-drop for both subject and object (i.e. subject and object pronouns are commonly omitted). The finite verb's subject argument is indexed for person (not for number) on the verb. Transitive verbs also index their object through pre-verbal particles (zero for third person). A maximum of one such object index is possible. If a transitive verb has both a patient and a recipient, the latter is not indexed and appears as a postpositional phrase (indirect object).

The expression of nominal possessive or genitive relations is similarly head-marking: the head (i.e. the possessed) is marked with indices indicating the person of the dependent (the possessor), the noun phrase expressing which is either omitted normally if pronominal (a pro-drop phenomenon) or precedes the head, e.g. arask-i 'my horse' (or yang arask-i), araska 'his horse' (zero-marked possessor), Juan araska 'Juan's horse' (cf. aras 'horse' without ligature).

Adpositions

Other relations between a verb and its noun phrase complements or adjuncts are expressed by means of postpositional structures or relational constructions. Postpositions are invariable and follow the noun phrase, e.g. Nicaragua ra 'in/to Nicaragua'. A relational construction has the internal form of a possessive construction (above), except that the place of the head noun is occupied by a quasi-noun called a relational; the latter is often followed by a postpositon. E.g. nin-i-ra (or yang ninira) 'behind me', nina-ra (or witin ninara) 'behind him', Juan nina-ra 'behind Juan', where the relational nina imitates a possessed noun.

Predication, sentence types and compound and complex sentences

There is a copula with an irregular and defective conjugational paradigm.

Negation is achieved through various constructions. One is the use of the verb's negative participle, which is invariable for person and tense; another is through use of a negative particle apia which follows verbs (in the future only), but precedes the copula. Yes–no questions have no special grammatical marking as such, but all kinds of questions are optionally followed by the sentence particle ki. Other sentence particles express different modal nuances.

Verbs or whole clauses may be conjoined by juxtaposition, all but the last verb in the chain adopting the form of a switch reference participle. These vary in form depending on whether the following verb has the same or a different subject, and also depending on certain tense or aspect relations, and on the person of the subject in the case of different-subject participles.

Besides these widely used constructions, clauses may also be linked by coordinating conjunctions, and subordinate clauses may be marked by a clause-final subordinator.

See also

Related Research Articles

Infinitive is a linguistics term for certain verb forms existing in many languages, most often used as non-finite verbs. As with many linguistic concepts, there is not a single definition applicable to all languages. The name is derived from Late Latin [modus] infinitivus, a derivative of infinitus meaning "unlimited".

Hurrian is an extinct Hurro-Urartian language spoken by the Hurrians (Khurrites), a people who entered northern Mesopotamia around 2300 BC and had mostly vanished by 1000 BC. Hurrian was the language of the Mitanni kingdom in northern Mesopotamia and was likely spoken at least initially in Hurrian settlements in modern-day Syria.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Greenlandic language</span> Inuit language spoken in Greenland

Greenlandic is an Eskimo–Aleut language with about 57,000 speakers, mostly Greenlandic Inuit in Greenland. It is closely related to the Inuit languages in Canada such as Inuktitut. It is the most widely spoken Eskimo–Aleut language. In June 2009, the government of Greenland, the Naalakkersuisut, made Greenlandic the sole official language of the autonomous territory, to strengthen it in the face of competition from the colonial language, Danish. The main variety is Kalaallisut, or West Greenlandic. The second variety is Tunumiit oraasiat, or East Greenlandic. The language of the Inughuit of Greenland, Inuktun or Polar Eskimo, is a recent arrival and a dialect of Inuktitut.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Crow language</span> Missouri Valley Siouan language of Montana, US

Crow is a Missouri Valley Siouan language spoken primarily by the Crow Nation in present-day southeastern Montana. The word Apsáalooke translates to "Children of the Large Beaked Bird", which was later incorrectly translated into English as 'Crow'. It is one of the larger populations of American Indian languages with 4,160 speakers according to the 2015 US Census.

Neo-Mandaic, also known as Modern Mandaic, sometimes called the "ratna", is the modern reflex of the Mandaic language, the liturgical language of the Mandaean religious community of Iraq and Iran. Although severely endangered, it survives today as the first language of a small number of Mandaeans in Iran and in the Mandaean diaspora. All Neo-Mandaic speakers are multilingual in the languages of their neighbors, Arabic and Persian, and the influence of these languages upon the grammar of Neo-Mandaic is considerable, particularly in the lexicon and the morphology of the noun. Nevertheless, Neo-Mandaic is more conservative even in these regards than most other Neo-Aramaic languages.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Tzeltal language</span> Mayan language of Mexico

Tzeltal or Tseltal is a Mayan language spoken in the Mexican state of Chiapas, mostly in the municipalities of Ocosingo, Altamirano, Huixtán, Tenejapa, Yajalón, Chanal, Sitalá, Amatenango del Valle, Socoltenango, Las Rosas, Chilón, San Juan Cancuc, San Cristóbal de las Casas and Oxchuc. Tzeltal is one of many Mayan languages spoken near this eastern region of Chiapas, including Tzotzil, Chʼol, and Tojolabʼal, among others. There is also a small Tzeltal diaspora in other parts of Mexico and the United States, primarily as a result of unfavorable economic conditions in Chiapas.

Swampy Cree is a variety of the Algonquian language, Cree. It is spoken in a series of Swampy Cree communities in northern Manitoba, central northeast of Saskatchewan along the Saskatchewan River and along the Hudson Bay coast and adjacent inland areas to the south and west, and Ontario along the coast of Hudson Bay and James Bay. Within the group of dialects called "West Cree", it is referred to as an "n-dialect", as the variable phoneme common to all Cree dialects appears as "n" in this dialect.

The grammar of Classical Nahuatl is agglutinative, head-marking, and makes extensive use of compounding, noun incorporation and derivation. That is, it can add many different prefixes and suffixes to a root until very long words are formed. Very long verbal forms or nouns created by incorporation, and accumulation of prefixes are common in literary works. New words can thus be easily created.

Tsez, also known as Dido, is a Northeast Caucasian language with about 15,000 speakers spoken by the Tsez, a Muslim people in the mountainous Tsunta District of southwestern Dagestan in Russia. The name is said to derive from the Tsez word for 'eagle', but this is most likely a folk etymology. The name Dido is derived from the Georgian word დიდი, meaning 'big'.

Ixcatec is a language spoken by the people of the Mexican village of Santa María Ixcatlan, in the northern part of the state of Oaxaca. The Ixcatec language belongs to the Popolocan branch of the Oto-manguean language family. It is believed to have been the second language to branch off from the others within the Popolocan subgroup, though there is a small debate over the relation it has to them.

This rather technical article provides a typological sketch of the Pipil language. Another related article outlines Pipil grammar in fuller detail. The distinctive purpose of the present article is to single out those specific features of Nawat linguistic structure that are relevant to this language's general typological classification and characterization, answering the question: What major features make this language similar to or different from other languages? Most of the assertions in this article are generalizations from information found in the Pipil grammar article.

This article provides a grammar sketch of the Nawat or Pipil language, an endangered language spoken by the Pipils of western El Salvador and Nicarao people of Nicaragua. It belongs to the Nahua group within the Uto-Aztecan language family. There also exists a brief typological overview of the language that summarizes the language's most salient features of general typological interest in more technical terms.

Classical Kʼicheʼ was an ancestral form of today's Kʼicheʼ language, which was spoken in the highland regions of Guatemala around the time of the 16th-century Spanish conquest of Guatemala. Classical Kʼicheʼ has been preserved in a number of historical Mesoamerican documents, lineage histories, missionary texts, and dictionaries. Most famously, it is the language in which the renowned highland Maya mythological and historical narrative Popol Vuh is written. Another historical text of partly similar content is the Título de Totonicapán.

The Rama language is one of the indigenous languages of the Chibchan family spoken by the Rama people on the island of Rama Cay and south of lake Bluefields on the Caribbean coast of Nicaragua. Other indigenous languages of this region include Miskito and Sumu. Rama is one of the northernmost languages of the Chibchan family.

The Wuvulu-Aua language is an Austronesian language which is spoken on the Wuvulu and Aua Islands and in the Manus Province of Papua New Guinea.

The verb is one of the most complex parts of Basque grammar. It is sometimes represented as a difficult challenge for learners of the language, and many Basque grammars devote most of their pages to lists or tables of verb paradigms. This article does not give a full list of verb forms; its purpose is to explain the nature and structure of the system.

Bororo (Borôro), also known as Boe, is the sole surviving language of a small family believed to be part of the Macro-Jê languages. It is spoken by the Bororo, hunters and gatherers in the central Mato Grosso region of Brazil.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Paya language</span> Chibchan language of Honduras

Pech or Pesh is a Chibchan language spoken in Honduras. It was formerly known as Paya, and continues to be referred to in this manner by several sources, though there are negative connotations associated with this term. It has also been referred to as Seco. There are 300 speakers according to Yasugi (2007). It is spoken near the north-central coast of Honduras, in the Dulce Nombre de Culmí municipality of Olancho Department.

Mehek is a Tama language spoken by about 6300 people in a somewhat mountainous area along the southern base of the Torricelli Mountains in northwestern Papua New Guinea. Mehek is spoken in six villages of Sandaun Province: Nuku, Yiminum, Mansuku, Yifkindu, Wilwil, and Kafle. Mehek is most closely related to Pahi, with 51% lexical similarity, and spoken approximately 20 kilometers to the southwest. Mehek is a fairly typical Papuan language, being verb-final, having a relatively simple phonology, and agglutinative morphology. There is very little published information about Mehek. The literacy rate in Tok Pisin, spoken by nearly everyone, is 50-75%. Mehek is not written, so there is no literacy in Mehek. Tok Pisin is primarily used in the schools, with 50% children attending. There is also a sign language used by the large number of deaf people in the Mehek community.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Daakaka language</span> Austronesian language spoken in Vanuatu

Daakaka is a native language of Ambrym, Vanuatu. It is spoken by about one thousand speakers in the south-western corner of the island.

References