Roviana language

Last updated
Roviana
Native to Solomon Islands
Regionnorth central New Georgia Island
Native speakers
(9,900 cited 1999) [1]
L2 speakers: 16,000 (1987) [1]
Austronesian
Language codes
ISO 639-3 rug
Glottolog rovi1238
This article contains IPA phonetic symbols. Without proper rendering support, you may see question marks, boxes, or other symbols instead of Unicode characters. For an introductory guide on IPA symbols, see Help:IPA.

Roviana is a member of the North West Solomonic branch of Oceanic languages. It is spoken around Roviana and Vonavona lagoons at the north central New Georgia in the Solomon Islands. It has 10,000 first-language speakers and an additional 16,000 people mostly over 30 years old speak it as a second language (Raymond 2005). In the past, Roviana was widely used as a trade language and further used as a lingua franca, especially for church purposes in the Western Province, but now it is being replaced by the Solomon Islands Pijin. Published studies on Roviana include: Ray (1926), Waterhouse (1949) and Todd (1978) contain the syntax of Roviana. Corston-Oliver (1996 & 2002) discuss ergativity in Roviana. Todd (2000) and Ross (1988) discuss the clause structure in Roviana. Schuelke (2020) discusses grammatical relations and syntactic ergativity in Roviana. [2]

Contents

Phonology and orthography

Consonants

Labial Alveolar Velar Glottal
Nasal m m n n ŋ ng
Plosive voiced ᵐb b ⁿd d ᵑɡ q
voiceless p p t t k k
Fricative voiced β v z z ɣ g
voiceless s s h h
Rhotic r r
Lateral l l

The Roviana alphabet is based on the Latin alphabet and consists of the above letters.

allophones:[h]~[ɦ] ([+voiced]) / V_V → /huhuβe/[huɦuβe] 'bathing'

[ŋ]~[ɲ] / _V [-back] → /ŋiɾa/[ɲiɾa] 'strong'

/r/ is lightly trilled in unstressed syllables and strongly trilled in stressed syllables.

Vowels

FrontBack
Highiu
Mideo
Lowa

V → V: / stressed Vs

V → Ṽ / _N

[a]~[ə] / _V → /leana/[leəna]

Phonotactics

(C) V

(C represents a single consonant and V represents a monophthong or diphthong.)

Diphthongs

There are five diphthongs; /ei/, /ai/, /ae/, /au/, and /oi/

The majority of lexical morphemes consist of two or three syllables. Lexical morphemes consisting of four syllables or a single syllable are uncommon whereas morphemes consisting of more than four syllables have never occurred.

Stress

Stress is not contrastive.

  1. Roots of one syllable, with the exception of prepositions and articles;
    1. /ˈla/ 'go', /ˈmae/ 'come'
  2. Roots of two syllables are stressed on the initial syllables;
    1. /ˈzama/ 'talk', /ˈtalo/ 'taro'
  3. Roots of more than two syllables are stressed on the first and second syllables;
    1. /ˈeˈhara/ 'blood', /ˈsiˈɡareti/ 'cigarette'

The nominalising infix in occurs within the first syllable of the root, it always receives stress;

/ˈɣani/ 'eat', /ˈɣiˈnani/ 'food'

All material which precedes the root (prefixes and reduplicated material) is assigned stress as if it were a single root;

/ˈβari-ˈpera/ 'fight', /ˈhabo-ˈhabotu-ana/ 'chair'

The transitive suffix /-i/ takes stress;

/ˈseke-ˈi-a/ 'hit him/her/it'

Other suffixes, however, do not take stress and are ignored in determining the placement of stress. Material following the root is not treated as a unit for the purpose of stress assignment;

/ˈdoɣoˈr-i-ɣami/ 'see us (EXClusive)'

The suffix /-ɣami/ does not receive stress.

Stress is assigned independently to each root in a compound:

/βetu/+/lotu//ˈβetuˈlotu/ 'church ('pray' + 'house')'

Grammar

Roviana word order is verb–subject–object (VSO).

Pronouns

PersonAbsoluteErgativeFocalEnglish
1st personaraurauarauI, me
gitagitagitawe (incl)
gamigamigamiwe(excl)
2nd personagoigoiagoiyou (sg)
gamugamugamuyou (pl)
3rd personasasaasdas/he/it
sariniriarinithey

Pronominal suffixes

singularplural
1st personexclusive-qu-mami
inclusive-da
2nd person-mu-mia
3rd person-na-di

These are suffixed to direct/inalienable possessions such as kin terms and parts of the body.

lima-na

hand-3SG

lima-na

hand-3SG

'his/her/its hand'

tama-qu

father-1SG

tama-qu

father-1SG

'my father'

Preposed possessor

singularplural
1st personexclusivequamami
inclusivenada
2nd personmuamia
3rd personnanadia

These are suffixed to indirect or alienable possessions:

nana

POSS:3SG

hore

canoe

nana hore

POSS:3SG canoe

'his/her canoe'

mia

POSS:2SG

popoa

home

mia popoa

POSS:2SG home

'their home'

Postposed possessor

singularplural
1st personexclusivetaqatani/tami
inclusivetani
2nd persontamutani/tamu
3rd persontaditadi

These are suffixed to a second kind of indirect or alienable possessions:

Hie

This

sa

DEF

lose

room

tanisa

POSS:3PL

Hie sa lose tanisa

This DEF room POSS:3PL

'This is his/her room'

singularplural
1st personexclusivegequgemami
inclusivegada
2nd persongemugemi
3rd persongenagedi

The possessive for food is prefixed ge or ga:

gemi

POSS:2PL

ginani

food

gemi ginani

POSS:2PL food

'your food'

singularplural
1st personexclusiveequemami
inclusiveeda
2nd personemuemi
3rd personenaedi

The possessive for desire is prefixed o or e:

equ

POSS:1SG

puta

sleep

equ puta

POSS:1SG sleep

'I want to sleep'

Interrogative pronouns

Inter.PronounsEnglish
eseiwho
ariseiwho (pl)
esei ri karawho (of two persons)
teseiwhose
sa/na sawhat
sa sariwhat (pl)
savanawhich
na sa ri karawhich (of two things)

Indefinite pronouns

Indef.PronounsEnglish
keke tiea man
ke tieany man
isa si kekeanother
keke nana koburuone of his/her kids
kaiqa puleothers/some more
ka visavisa/kaiqasome/few
loke tieno one
votiki zinamadifferent language
loke toŋanothing/none

Demonstrative pronouns

Dem. pronounsE.g. sentence/questionEnglish
hie/si hie/hierasa tie hie/sa si hie? /hiera sa qua vetuthis man/what is this?/this is my house
hoi/sana/asaasa sa vineki hoi/na tie sana/asa se Mariathat's the girl there/that man/that's Maria
hire/si hirehire mua buka/tamu goi si hire?these are your books/are these yours?
hiroihiroi mua bukathose are your books

Nouns

There are two classes of nouns in Roviana. The first includes kin terms, body parts and some local nouns. These are used with suffixed personal pronouns such as:

tinaqu – 'my mother'
mata-na – 'her/his /its eyes'

Nouns of the second class are used with separate possessive words such as:

qua buka – 'my book'
nana vetu – 'his/her/its house'

Local nouns are formed from verbs by the suffix ana. They denote a place where an action is performed:

habotu - 'sit'
habo-habotu-ana - 'chair'

Nouns are formed from verbs & adjectives by the infix in. When the verb or adjective begins with a vowel, in is prefixed:

ene - 'to walk'
inene - 'a journey'

When the verb or adjective begins a consonant in is infixed after the first consonant:

kera - 'sing'
kinera -'song'

A noun can also be formed by in from the causative or reciprocal forms of verbs:

gila -'to know'
vagila - 'to show'
vinagila-gila - 'a sign'

Demonstratives

Articles in Roviana occur before the noun, marking the noun phrase as common or proper. Roviana has definite and indefinite articles.

The indefinite article is na:

na

INDEF

nana

POSS:3SG

buka

book

na nana buka

INDEF POSS:3SG book

'his/her book'

na can also be exchanged with sa:

sa

hore

sa hore

'the canoe'

na and sa may also be applied with plural nouns:

na

INDEF

tie

person

habahuala

poor

-di

POSS:3PL

na tie habahuala -di

INDEF person poor POSS:3PL

'the poor people'

The definite article is sa:

sa

DEF

dia

POSS:3PL

vetu

house

sa dia vetu

DEF POSS:3PL house

'their house'

The personal articles are the non- absolutive e and absolutive se. E is commonly used with a proper noun in the subjective case, se in the objective:

Dogoria

saw

rau

1SG

se

ABS

Nate

Nate

rane

rane

sarere

sarere

lahe.

lahe

Dogoria rau se Nate rane sarere lahe.

saw 1SG ABS Nate rane sarere lahe

'I saw Nate last Saturday.'

Syntax

Imperative and interrogative sentences

Imperative sentences

An actor can optionally be omitted (1); otherwise there is no structural difference from a declarative clause (2).

(1)

La

go

(si

ABS

goi).

2SG

La (si goi).

go ABS 2SG

'Go!'

(2)

Va-mae-a

CAUS-come-3SG

sa

DEF

magu.

knife

Va-mae-a sa magu.

CAUS-come-3SG DEF knife

'Give me the knife!'

Interrogative sentences

Yes–no questions are structurally identical to declaratives, but have a distinct rising intonation. The two single word answers are uve 'yes' and lokari 'no'.

Wh-questions or information questions contain an interrogative phrase in focus position (i.e. clause initial) and optionally is followed by the focal particle si; for example,

ae 'where?'
esei 'who?'
kavisa 'how many/much?'
sa 'what?'
vea 'how?, why?'

Esei

who

poza-mu

name-2SG

si

FOC

agoi?

2SG:FOC

Esei poza-mu si agoi?

who name-2SG FOC 2SG:FOC

'What is your name?' (lit. 'Who is your name?')

Interrogative morphemes are frequently preceded by the disjunctive particle na;

na vea ke 'why?'
na sa 'what?'

Complex sentences

Coordination

Coordination is marked by a conjunction between the two clauses; the conjunction belongs with the second clause;

ba 'but'
ke 'so, thus'
me(ke) 'and' (me is far more common in texts)
na 'or'
pude 'purposive'
tiqe 'then'

Gina

maybe

tourism

tourism

kamahire

now

kote

FUT

sage

go.up

mae

come

ba

but

lopu

NEG

ta-gilana.

PASS-know

Gina tourism kamahire kote sage mae ba lopu ta-gilana.

maybe tourism now FUT go.up come but NEG PASS-know

'Maybe tourism will pick up, but we don't know.'

Subordination

Three major classes are relative clauses, complement clauses and adverbial clauses.

Relative clauses

Relative clauses follow the head N and are introduced by the invariant relative clause marker sapu. They may only be formed on A, S and O and on the argument nominal of a verbless clause. A more detailed explanation is below.

Complement clauses

Complement clauses are introduced by the subordinator sapu; otherwise, they are no different from main clauses. Complement clauses occur after verbs of cognition, speech or perception, whereas subordinate clauses (with the exception of relative clauses) occur in focus position;

Lopu

NEG

hiva-ni-a

like-TR-3SG

ri

3PL

sapu

COMP

tangin-i-a

hold-TR-3SG

rau

1SG

sa

DEF

vineki

girl

Lopu hiva-ni-a ri sapu tangin-i-a rau sa vineki

NEG like-TR-3SG 3PL COMP hold-TR-3SG 1SG DEF girl

'They didn't like me holding the girl.' (lit.'They didn't like it, that I was holding the girl.')

Complement clauses are considered to be intermediate between main and subordinate clauses. In texts, complement clauses in Roviana are rare. Direct quotation is more frequent than subordination to higher predicates of information, while epistemic modals (e.g. gina 'maybe', tu 'EMPH) are often used rather than subordination to higher predicates of cognition (ergativity).

Adverbial clauses

Adverbial clauses occur in focus position and never contain new mentions in core argument positions. They are introduced by a subordinator and followed by the focal particle si, a consequence of being in focus position;

beto 'after'
pude 'if'
totoso 'while, when'

Ke

so

beto

after

vagi

gather

ri

3PL

sarina

DEF:PL

⟨in⟩avoso

NOM⟩know

si

FOC

la

go

buna-i-a

bomb-TR-3SG

ri

3PL

sa

DEF

vasina

place

asa.

that

Ke beto vagi ri sarina ⟨in⟩avoso si la buna-i-a ri sa vasina asa.

so after gather 3PL DEF:PL ⟨NOM⟩know FOC go bomb-TR-3SG 3PL DEF place that

'So after they had gathered all the information, they went and bombed that place.'

Subordination is extremely limited in Roviana. Subordinate clauses never contain other subordinate clauses, nor do they contain relative clauses. Similarly, relative clauses do not contain either subordinate clauses or relative clauses.

Ergativity

The subject of an intransitive verb has the same morphological marker as a direct object, and a different morphological marker from the subject of a transitive verb.

A – transitive subject, O – transitive direct object, S – intransitive subject, respectively.

Whether Roviana is an ergative language or not is argumentative, however; relative clauses in this language can be categorised by ergativity, so it can be described as an ergative language.

Relative clauses

Relative clauses in Roviana follow the head N and are introduced by an invariant relative marker sapu. The coreferent of the N in the matrix clause is never overt within the relative clause. This feature may be according to whether the notional coreferent within the relative clause is A, S or O.

Relative clauses on A

Relative clauses on A use clausal nominalisation. The notional A has no overt realisation. The nominalised verb in a relative clause on A carries a suffix 'NSUF', which is also used to index the possessor in possessives;

sa

DEF

huda

tree

noma-na

big-3SG.NSUF

sa huda noma-na

DEF tree big-3SG.NSUF

'the big tree'

When the O in the relative clause is a proper N, it is marked with the article e;

Hierana

this

sa

DEF

koreo

boy

sapu

REL

tupa-na

punch-3SG.NSUF

e

ART

Zone.

John

Hierana sa koreo sapu tupa-na e Zone.

this DEF boy REL punch-3SG.NSUF ART John

'This is the boy that punched John.'

Relative clauses on S

Given that the coreferent in the relative clause does not have overt realisation;

Hierana

this

sa

DEF

tie

man

sapu

REL

kote

FUT

taloa.

leave

Hierana sa tie sapu kote taloa.

this DEF man REL FUT leave

'This is the man who is going away.'

Relative clauses on O

In relative clauses on O, A is overt in the relative clause and full verbal morphology is used to index the O. The nominal suffixes are not used in relative clauses on O;

Hierana

this

sa

DEF

koreo

boy

sapu

REL

tupa-i-a

punch-TR-3SG.DO

e

ART

Zone.

John

Hierana sa koreo sapu tupa-i-a e Zone.

this DEF boy REL punch-TR-3SG.DO ART John

'This is the boy that John punched.'

In the context of a relative clause which is by definition subordinate, e is glossed simply ART, since it is used with proper Ns which are A or O. These following two examples have got e; the first one is on A whereas the second one is on O.

Hierana

this

sa

DEF

koreo

boy

sapu

REL

tupa-na

punch-3SG.NSUF

e

ART

Zone.

John

Hierana sa koreo sapu tupa-na e Zone.

this DEF boy REL punch-3SG.NSUF ART John

'This is the boy that punched John.'

Hierana

this

sa

DEF

koreo

boy

sapu

REL

tupa-i-a

punch-TR-3SG.DO

e

ART

Zone.

John

Hierana sa koreo sapu tupa-i-a e Zone.

this DEF boy REL punch-TR-3SG.DO ART John

'This is the boy that John punched.'

'When' clauses

'When' clauses are introduced by the subordinator totoso 'time' or the syncopated form totso, but they do not specify the precise nature of the temporal relation involved;

Totso

time

koa

stay

goi

you.SG

pa

PREP

korapa

inside

tropic

tropic

si

FOC

kaqu

must

pezaku

wash.hands

lamo

always

si

ABS

goi.

you.SG

Totso koa goi pa korapa tropic si kaqu pezaku lamo si goi.

time stay you.SG PREP inside tropic FOC must wash.hands always ABS you.SG

'When you stay in the tropics, you must always wash your hands.'

'After' clauses

The event of an 'after' clause is introduced by the subordinator beto 'finish' and temporally precedes the event of the matrix clause to which it is syntactically subordinate;

Ke

so

beto

finish

vag-i

gather-TR

ri

they

sarina

DEF.PL

⟨in⟩avoso

NOM⟩know

si

FOC

la

go

buna-i-a

bomb-TR-3SG.DO

ri

they.ERG

sa

DEF

vasina

place

asa.

that

Ke beto vag-i ri sarina ⟨in⟩avoso si la buna-i-a ri sa vasina asa.

so finish gather-TR they DEF.PL ⟨NOM⟩know FOC go bomb-TR-3SG.DO they.ERG DEF place that

'So after they had gathered all the information, they went and bombed that place.'

'Contemporaneous' clauses

'Contemporaneous' clauses have imperfective aspect, usually accompanied by reduplication of the verb, with the meaning 'While ...-ing' or 'As ...-ing';

En-ene

REDUP-walk

ri

they

la

go

hoirana

there

si

FOC

tutuvi-a

meet-3SG.DO

ri

they.ERG

se

ABS

Manue.

Possum

En-ene ri la hoirana si tutuvi-a ri se Manue.

REDUP-walk they go there FOC meet-3SG.DO they.ERG ABS Possum

'As they were walking along, they met Possum.'

Conditionals

In a conditional, the protasis is a subordinate clause. As with the subordinate clauses, there is a neutral system of case marking;

...ba

but

pude

if

gore

go.down

vura

come.out

mae

come

sa

it

si

FOC

kote

FUT

taloa

leave

si

ABS

rau.

I

...ba pude gore vura mae sa si kote taloa si rau.

but if go.down come.out come it FOC FUT leave ABS I

'...but if it works out, I'll leave.'

Footnotes

  1. 1 2 Roviana at Ethnologue (18th ed., 2015) (subscription required)
  2. Schuelke, Peter (2020). Grammatical Relations and Syntactic Ergativity in Roviana: A little-described language of the Solomon Islands. Honolulu, HI, USA: University of Hawaiʻi at Mānoa.

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Fijian language</span> Austronesian language of Fiji

Fijian is an Austronesian language of the Malayo-Polynesian family spoken by some 350,000–450,000 ethnic Fijians as a native language. The 2013 Constitution established Fijian as an official language of Fiji, along with English and Fiji Hindi and there is discussion about establishing it as the "national language". Fijian is a VOS language.

Taba is a Malayo-Polynesian language of the South Halmahera–West New Guinea group. It is spoken mostly on the islands of Makian, Kayoa and southern Halmahera in North Maluku province of Indonesia by about 20,000 people.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Dameli language</span> Indo-Aryan language spoken in Pakistan

Dameli (دَميلي), also Damia, Damɛ̃ḍī, Dāmia bāṣa or Gidoj, is an Indo-Aryan language of the Dardic subgroup spoken by approximately 5,000 people in the Domel Town, in the Chitral District of Khyber-Pakhtunkhwa province of Pakistan.

The Nafsan language, also known as South Efate or Erakor, is a Southern Oceanic language spoken on the island of Efate in central Vanuatu. As of 2005, there are approximately 6,000 speakers who live in coastal villages from Pango to Eton. The language's grammar has been studied by Nick Thieberger, who has produced a book of stories and a dictionary of the language.

Manam is a Kairiru–Manam language spoken mainly on the volcanic Manam Island, northeast of New Guinea.

Tamambo, or Malo, is an Oceanic language spoken by 4,000 people on Malo and nearby islands in Vanuatu. It is one of the most conservative Southern Oceanic languages.

Cavineña is an indigenous language spoken on the Amazonian plains of northern Bolivia by over 1,000 Cavineño people. Although Cavineña is still spoken, it is an endangered language. Guillaume (2004) states that about 1200 people speak the language, out of a population of around 1700. Nearly all Cavineña are bilingual in Spanish.

Hoava is an Oceanic language spoken by 1000–1500 people on New Georgia Island, Solomon Islands. Speakers of Hoava are multilingual and usually also speak Roviana, Marovo, Solomon Islands Pijin, English.

Ughele is an Oceanic language spoken by about 1200 people on Rendova Island, located in the Western Province of the Solomon Islands.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Bangime language</span> Language isolate of southeastern Mali

Bangime is a language isolate spoken by 3,500 ethnic Dogon in seven villages in southern Mali, who call themselves the bàŋɡá–ndɛ̀. Bangande is the name of the ethnicity of this community and their population grows at a rate of 2.5% per year. The Bangande consider themselves to be Dogon, but other Dogon people insist they are not. Bangime is an endangered language classified as 6a - Vigorous by Ethnologue. Long known to be highly divergent from the (other) Dogon languages, it was first proposed as a possible isolate by Blench (2005). Heath and Hantgan have hypothesized that the cliffs surrounding the Bangande valley provided isolation of the language as well as safety for Bangande people. Even though Bangime is not closely related to Dogon languages, the Bangande still consider their language to be Dogon. Hantgan and List report that Bangime speakers seem unaware that it is not mutually intelligible with any Dogon language.

Biak, also known as Biak-Numfor, Noefoor, Mafoor, Mefoor, Nufoor, Mafoorsch, Myfoorsch and Noefoorsch, is an Austronesian language of the South Halmahera-West New Guinea subgroup of the Eastern Malayo-Polynesian languages.

Farefare or Frafra, also known by the regional name of Gurenne (Gurene), is a Niger–Congo language spoken by the Frafra people of northern Ghana, particularly the Upper East Region, and southern Burkina Faso. It is a national language of Ghana, and is closely related to Dagbani and other languages of Northern Ghana, and also related to Mossi, also known as Mooré, the national language of Burkina Faso.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Iatmul language</span> Ndu language spoken in Papua New Guinea

Iatmul is the language of the Iatmul people, spoken around the Sepik River in the East Sepik Province, northern Papua New Guinea. The Iatmul, however, do not refer to their language by the term Iatmul, but call it gepmakudi.

Mav̋ea is an Oceanic language spoken on Mavea Island in Vanuatu, off the eastern coast of Espiritu Santo. It belongs to the North–Central Vanuatu linkage of Southern Oceanic. The total population of the island is approximately 172, with only 34 fluent speakers of the Mav̋ea language reported in 2008.

Tawala is an Oceanic language of the Milne Bay Province, Papua New Guinea. It is spoken by 20,000 people who live in hamlets and small villages on the East Cape peninsula, on the shores of Milne Bay and on areas of the islands of Sideia and Basilaki. There are approximately 40 main centres of population each speaking the same dialect, although through the process of colonisation some centres have gained more prominence than others.

Lewo is an Oceanic language spoken on Epi Island, in Vanuatu.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Vamale language</span> Austronesian language spoken in New Caledonia

Vamale (Pamale) is a Kanak language of northern New Caledonia. The Hmwaeke dialect, spoken in Tiéta, is fusing with Haveke and nearly extinct. Vamale is nowadays spoken in Tiendanite, We Hava, Téganpaïk and Tiouandé. It was spoken in the Pamale valley and its tributaries Vawe and Usa until the colonial war of 1917, when its speakers were displaced.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Toʼabaita language</span> Malaita language of the Solomon Islands

Toʼabaita, also known as Toqabaqita, Toʼambaita, Malu and Maluʼu, is a language spoken by the people living at the north-western tip of Malaita Island, of South Eastern Solomon Islands. Toʼabaita is an Austronesian language.

Neveʻei, also known as Vinmavis, is an Oceanic language of central Malekula, Vanuatu. There are around 500 primary speakers of Neveʻei and about 750 speakers in total.

Longgu (Logu) is a Southeast Solomonic language of Guadalcanal, but originally from Malaita.

References