Roviana | |
---|---|
Native to | Solomon Islands |
Region | north central New Georgia Island |
Native speakers | (9,900 cited 1999) [1] L2 speakers: 16,000 (1987) [1] |
Austronesian
| |
Language codes | |
ISO 639-3 | rug |
Glottolog | rovi1238 |
Roviana is a member of the North West Solomonic branch of Oceanic languages. It is spoken around Roviana and Vonavona lagoons at the north central New Georgia in the Solomon Islands. It has 10,000 first-language speakers and an additional 16,000 people mostly over 30 years old speak it as a second language (Raymond 2005). In the past, Roviana was widely used as a trade language and further used as a lingua franca, especially for church purposes in the Western Province, but now it is being replaced by the Solomon Islands Pijin. Published studies on Roviana include: Ray (1926), Waterhouse (1949) and Todd (1978) contain the syntax of Roviana. Corston-Oliver (1996 & 2002) discuss ergativity in Roviana. Todd (2000) and Ross (1988) discuss the clause structure in Roviana. Schuelke (2020) discusses grammatical relations and syntactic ergativity in Roviana. [2]
Labial | Alveolar | Velar | Glottal | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Nasal | m ⟨m⟩ | n ⟨n⟩ | ŋ ⟨ng⟩ | ||
Plosive | voiced | ᵐb ⟨b⟩ | ⁿd ⟨d⟩ | ᵑɡ ⟨q⟩ | |
voiceless | p ⟨p⟩ | t ⟨t⟩ | k ⟨k⟩ | ||
Fricative | voiced | β ⟨v⟩ | z ⟨z⟩ | ɣ ⟨g⟩ | |
voiceless | s ⟨s⟩ | h ⟨h⟩ | |||
Rhotic | r ⟨r⟩ | ||||
Lateral | l ⟨l⟩ |
The Roviana alphabet is based on the Latin alphabet and consists of the above letters.
allophones:[h]~[ɦ] ([+voiced]) / V_V → /huhuβe/[huɦuβe] 'bathing'
[ŋ]~[ɲ] / _V [-back] → /ŋiɾa/[ɲiɾa] 'strong'
/r/ is lightly trilled in unstressed syllables and strongly trilled in stressed syllables.
Front | Back | |
---|---|---|
High | i | u |
Mid | e | o |
Low | a |
V → V: / stressed Vs
V → Ṽ / _N
[a]~[ə] / _V → /leana/[leəna]
(C) V
(C represents a single consonant and V represents a monophthong or diphthong.)
There are five diphthongs; /ei/, /ai/, /ae/, /au/, and /oi/
The majority of lexical morphemes consist of two or three syllables. Lexical morphemes consisting of four syllables or a single syllable are uncommon whereas morphemes consisting of more than four syllables have never occurred.
Stress is not contrastive.
The nominalising infix ⟨in⟩ occurs within the first syllable of the root, it always receives stress;
All material which precedes the root (prefixes and reduplicated material) is assigned stress as if it were a single root;
The transitive suffix /-i/ takes stress;
Other suffixes, however, do not take stress and are ignored in determining the placement of stress. Material following the root is not treated as a unit for the purpose of stress assignment;
The suffix /-ɣami/ does not receive stress.
Stress is assigned independently to each root in a compound:
Roviana word order is verb–subject–object (VSO).
Person | Absolute | Ergative | Focal | English |
---|---|---|---|---|
1st person | arau | rau | arau | I, me |
gita | gita | gita | we (incl) | |
gami | gami | gami | we(excl) | |
2nd person | agoi | goi | agoi | you (sg) |
gamu | gamu | gamu | you (pl) | |
3rd person | asa | sa | asda | s/he/it |
sarini | ri | arini | they |
singular | plural | ||
---|---|---|---|
1st person | exclusive | -qu | -mami |
inclusive | -da | ||
2nd person | -mu | -mia | |
3rd person | -na | -di |
These are suffixed to direct/inalienable possessions such as kin terms and parts of the body.
lima-na
hand-3SG
'his/her/its hand'
tama-qu
father-1SG
'my father'
singular | plural | ||
---|---|---|---|
1st person | exclusive | qua | mami |
inclusive | nada | ||
2nd person | mua | mia | |
3rd person | nana | dia |
These are suffixed to indirect or alienable possessions:
nana
POSS:3SG
hore
canoe
'his/her canoe'
mia
POSS:2SG
popoa
home
'their home'
singular | plural | ||
---|---|---|---|
1st person | exclusive | taqa | tani/tami |
inclusive | tani | ||
2nd person | tamu | tani/tamu | |
3rd person | tadi | tadi |
These are suffixed to a second kind of indirect or alienable possessions:
Hie
This
sa
DEF
lose
room
tanisa
POSS:3PL
'This is his/her room'
singular | plural | ||
---|---|---|---|
1st person | exclusive | gequ | gemami |
inclusive | gada | ||
2nd person | gemu | gemi | |
3rd person | gena | gedi |
The possessive for food is prefixed ge or ga:
gemi
POSS:2PL
ginani
food
'your food'
singular | plural | ||
---|---|---|---|
1st person | exclusive | equ | emami |
inclusive | eda | ||
2nd person | emu | emi | |
3rd person | ena | edi |
The possessive for desire is prefixed o or e:
equ
POSS:1SG
puta
sleep
'I want to sleep'
Inter.Pronouns | English |
---|---|
esei | who |
arisei | who (pl) |
esei ri kara | who (of two persons) |
tesei | whose |
sa/na sa | what |
sa sari | what (pl) |
savana | which |
na sa ri kara | which (of two things) |
Indef.Pronouns | English |
---|---|
keke tie | a man |
ke tie | any man |
isa si keke | another |
keke nana koburu | one of his/her kids |
kaiqa pule | others/some more |
ka visavisa/kaiqa | some/few |
loke tie | no one |
votiki zinama | different language |
loke toŋa | nothing/none |
Dem. pronouns | E.g. sentence/question | English |
---|---|---|
hie/si hie/hiera | sa tie hie/sa si hie? /hiera sa qua vetu | this man/what is this?/this is my house |
hoi/sana/asa | asa sa vineki hoi/na tie sana/asa se Maria | that's the girl there/that man/that's Maria |
hire/si hire | hire mua buka/tamu goi si hire? | these are your books/are these yours? |
hiroi | hiroi mua buka | those are your books |
There are two classes of nouns in Roviana. The first includes kin terms, body parts and some local nouns. These are used with suffixed personal pronouns such as:
Nouns of the second class are used with separate possessive words such as:
Local nouns are formed from verbs by the suffix ana. They denote a place where an action is performed:
Nouns are formed from verbs & adjectives by the infix ⟨in⟩. When the verb or adjective begins with a vowel, ⟨in⟩ is prefixed:
When the verb or adjective begins a consonant in is infixed after the first consonant:
A noun can also be formed by in from the causative or reciprocal forms of verbs:
Articles in Roviana occur before the noun, marking the noun phrase as common or proper. Roviana has definite and indefinite articles.
The indefinite article is na:
na
INDEF
nana
POSS:3SG
buka
book
'his/her book'
na can also be exchanged with sa:
sa
hore
'the canoe'
na and sa may also be applied with plural nouns:
na
INDEF
tie
person
habahuala
poor
-di
POSS:3PL
'the poor people'
The definite article is sa:
sa
DEF
dia
POSS:3PL
vetu
house
'their house'
The personal articles are the non- absolutive e and absolutive se. E is commonly used with a proper noun in the subjective case, se in the objective:
Dogoria
saw
rau
1SG
se
ABS
Nate
Nate
rane
rane
sarere
sarere
lahe.
lahe
'I saw Nate last Saturday.'
An actor can optionally be omitted (1); otherwise there is no structural difference from a declarative clause (2).
La
go
(si
ABS
goi).
2SG
'Go!'
Va-mae-a
CAUS-come-3SG
sa
DEF
magu.
knife
'Give me the knife!'
Yes–no questions are structurally identical to declaratives, but have a distinct rising intonation. The two single word answers are uve 'yes' and lokari 'no'.
Wh-questions or information questions contain an interrogative phrase in focus position (i.e. clause initial) and optionally is followed by the focal particle si; for example,
Esei
who
poza-mu
name-2SG
si
FOC
agoi?
2SG:FOC
'What is your name?' (lit. 'Who is your name?')
Interrogative morphemes are frequently preceded by the disjunctive particle na;
Coordination is marked by a conjunction between the two clauses; the conjunction belongs with the second clause;
Gina
maybe
tourism
tourism
kamahire
now
kote
FUT
sage
go.up
mae
come
ba
but
lopu
NEG
ta-gilana.
PASS-know
'Maybe tourism will pick up, but we don't know.'
Three major classes are relative clauses, complement clauses and adverbial clauses.
Relative clauses follow the head N and are introduced by the invariant relative clause marker sapu. They may only be formed on A, S and O and on the argument nominal of a verbless clause. A more detailed explanation is below.
Complement clauses are introduced by the subordinator sapu; otherwise, they are no different from main clauses. Complement clauses occur after verbs of cognition, speech or perception, whereas subordinate clauses (with the exception of relative clauses) occur in focus position;
Lopu
NEG
hiva-ni-a
like-TR-3SG
ri
3PL
sapu
COMP
tangin-i-a
hold-TR-3SG
rau
1SG
sa
DEF
vineki
girl
'They didn't like me holding the girl.' (lit. 'They didn't like it, that I was holding the girl.')
Complement clauses are considered to be intermediate between main and subordinate clauses. In texts, complement clauses in Roviana are rare. Direct quotation is more frequent than subordination to higher predicates of information, while epistemic modals (e.g. gina 'maybe', tu 'EMPH) are often used rather than subordination to higher predicates of cognition (ergativity).
Adverbial clauses occur in focus position and never contain new mentions in core argument positions. They are introduced by a subordinator and followed by the focal particle si, a consequence of being in focus position;
Ke
so
beto
after
vagi
gather
ri
3PL
sarina
DEF:PL
⟨in⟩avoso
⟨NOM⟩know
si
FOC
la
go
buna-i-a
bomb-TR-3SG
ri
3PL
sa
DEF
vasina
place
asa.
that
'So after they had gathered all the information, they went and bombed that place.'
Subordination is extremely limited in Roviana. Subordinate clauses never contain other subordinate clauses, nor do they contain relative clauses. Similarly, relative clauses do not contain either subordinate clauses or relative clauses.
The subject of an intransitive verb has the same morphological marker as a direct object, and a different morphological marker from the subject of a transitive verb.
A – transitive subject, O – transitive direct object, S – intransitive subject, respectively.
Whether Roviana is an ergative language or not is argumentative, however; relative clauses in this language can be categorised by ergativity, so it can be described as an ergative language.
Relative clauses in Roviana follow the head N and are introduced by an invariant relative marker sapu. The coreferent of the N in the matrix clause is never overt within the relative clause. This feature may be according to whether the notional coreferent within the relative clause is A, S or O.
Relative clauses on A use clausal nominalisation. The notional A has no overt realisation. The nominalised verb in a relative clause on A carries a suffix 'NSUF', which is also used to index the possessor in possessives;
sa
DEF
huda
tree
noma-na
big-3SG.NSUF
'the big tree'
When the O in the relative clause is a proper N, it is marked with the article e;
Hierana
this
sa
DEF
koreo
boy
sapu
REL
tupa-na
punch-3SG.NSUF
e
ART
Zone.
John
'This is the boy that punched John.'
Given that the coreferent in the relative clause does not have overt realisation;
Hierana
this
sa
DEF
tie
man
sapu
REL
kote
FUT
taloa.
leave
'This is the man who is going away.'
In relative clauses on O, A is overt in the relative clause and full verbal morphology is used to index the O. The nominal suffixes are not used in relative clauses on O;
Hierana
this
sa
DEF
koreo
boy
sapu
REL
tupa-i-a
punch-TR-3SG.DO
e
ART
Zone.
John
'This is the boy that John punched.'
In the context of a relative clause which is by definition subordinate, e is glossed simply ART, since it is used with proper Ns which are A or O. These following two examples have got e; the first one is on A whereas the second one is on O.
Hierana
this
sa
DEF
koreo
boy
sapu
REL
tupa-na
punch-3SG.NSUF
e
ART
Zone.
John
'This is the boy that punched John.'
Hierana
this
sa
DEF
koreo
boy
sapu
REL
tupa-i-a
punch-TR-3SG.DO
e
ART
Zone.
John
'This is the boy that John punched.'
'When' clauses are introduced by the subordinator totoso 'time' or the syncopated form totso, but they do not specify the precise nature of the temporal relation involved;
Totso
time
koa
stay
goi
you.SG
pa
PREP
korapa
inside
tropic
tropic
si
FOC
kaqu
must
pezaku
wash.hands
lamo
always
si
ABS
goi.
you.SG
'When you stay in the tropics, you must always wash your hands.'
The event of an 'after' clause is introduced by the subordinator beto 'finish' and temporally precedes the event of the matrix clause to which it is syntactically subordinate;
Ke
so
beto
finish
vag-i
gather-TR
ri
they
sarina
DEF.PL
⟨in⟩avoso
⟨NOM⟩know
si
FOC
la
go
buna-i-a
bomb-TR-3SG.DO
ri
they.ERG
sa
DEF
vasina
place
asa.
that
'So after they had gathered all the information, they went and bombed that place.'
'Contemporaneous' clauses have imperfective aspect, usually accompanied by reduplication of the verb, with the meaning 'While ...-ing' or 'As ...-ing';
En-ene
REDUP-walk
ri
they
la
go
hoirana
there
si
FOC
tutuvi-a
meet-3SG.DO
ri
they.ERG
se
ABS
Manue.
Possum
'As they were walking along, they met Possum.'
In a conditional, the protasis is a subordinate clause. As with the subordinate clauses, there is a neutral system of case marking;
...ba
but
pude
if
gore
go.down
vura
come.out
mae
come
sa
it
si
FOC
kote
FUT
taloa
leave
si
ABS
rau.
I
'...but if it works out, I'll leave.'
Fijian is an Austronesian language of the Malayo-Polynesian family spoken by some 350,000–450,000 ethnic Fijians as a native language. The 2013 Constitution established Fijian as an official language of Fiji, along with English and Fiji Hindi and there is discussion about establishing it as the "national language". Fijian is a VOS language.
Taba is a Malayo-Polynesian language of the South Halmahera–West New Guinea group. It is spoken mostly on the islands of Makian, Kayoa and southern Halmahera in North Maluku province of Indonesia by about 20,000 people.
Dameli (دَميلي), also Damia, Damɛ̃ḍī, Dāmia bāṣa or Gidoj, is an Indo-Aryan language of the Dardic subgroup spoken by approximately 5,000 people in the Domel Town, in the Chitral District of Khyber-Pakhtunkhwa province of Pakistan.
The Nafsan language, also known as South Efate or Erakor, is a Southern Oceanic language spoken on the island of Efate in central Vanuatu. As of 2005, there are approximately 6,000 speakers who live in coastal villages from Pango to Eton. The language's grammar has been studied by Nick Thieberger, who has produced a book of stories and a dictionary of the language.
Manam is a Kairiru–Manam language spoken mainly on the volcanic Manam Island, northeast of New Guinea.
Tamambo, or Malo, is an Oceanic language spoken by 4,000 people on Malo and nearby islands in Vanuatu. It is one of the most conservative Southern Oceanic languages.
Cavineña is an indigenous language spoken on the Amazonian plains of northern Bolivia by over 1,000 Cavineño people. Although Cavineña is still spoken, it is an endangered language. Guillaume (2004) states that about 1200 people speak the language, out of a population of around 1700. Nearly all Cavineña are bilingual in Spanish.
Hoava is an Oceanic language spoken by 1000–1500 people on New Georgia Island, Solomon Islands. Speakers of Hoava are multilingual and usually also speak Roviana, Marovo, Solomon Islands Pijin, English.
Ughele is an Oceanic language spoken by about 1200 people on Rendova Island, located in the Western Province of the Solomon Islands.
Bangime is a language isolate spoken by 3,500 ethnic Dogon in seven villages in southern Mali, who call themselves the bàŋɡá–ndɛ̀. Bangande is the name of the ethnicity of this community and their population grows at a rate of 2.5% per year. The Bangande consider themselves to be Dogon, but other Dogon people insist they are not. Bangime is an endangered language classified as 6a - Vigorous by Ethnologue. Long known to be highly divergent from the (other) Dogon languages, it was first proposed as a possible isolate by Blench (2005). Heath and Hantgan have hypothesized that the cliffs surrounding the Bangande valley provided isolation of the language as well as safety for Bangande people. Even though Bangime is not closely related to Dogon languages, the Bangande still consider their language to be Dogon. Hantgan and List report that Bangime speakers seem unaware that it is not mutually intelligible with any Dogon language.
Biak, also known as Biak-Numfor, Noefoor, Mafoor, Mefoor, Nufoor, Mafoorsch, Myfoorsch and Noefoorsch, is an Austronesian language of the South Halmahera-West New Guinea subgroup of the Eastern Malayo-Polynesian languages.
Farefare or Frafra, also known by the regional name of Gurenne (Gurene), is a Niger–Congo language spoken by the Frafra people of northern Ghana, particularly the Upper East Region, and southern Burkina Faso. It is a national language of Ghana, and is closely related to Dagbani and other languages of Northern Ghana, and also related to Mossi, also known as Mooré, the national language of Burkina Faso.
Iatmul is the language of the Iatmul people, spoken around the Sepik River in the East Sepik Province, northern Papua New Guinea. The Iatmul, however, do not refer to their language by the term Iatmul, but call it gepmakudi.
Mav̋ea is an Oceanic language spoken on Mavea Island in Vanuatu, off the eastern coast of Espiritu Santo. It belongs to the North–Central Vanuatu linkage of Southern Oceanic. The total population of the island is approximately 172, with only 34 fluent speakers of the Mav̋ea language reported in 2008.
Tawala is an Oceanic language of the Milne Bay Province, Papua New Guinea. It is spoken by 20,000 people who live in hamlets and small villages on the East Cape peninsula, on the shores of Milne Bay and on areas of the islands of Sideia and Basilaki. There are approximately 40 main centres of population each speaking the same dialect, although through the process of colonisation some centres have gained more prominence than others.
Lewo is an Oceanic language spoken on Epi Island, in Vanuatu.
Vamale (Pamale) is a Kanak language of northern New Caledonia. The Hmwaeke dialect, spoken in Tiéta, is fusing with Haveke and nearly extinct. Vamale is nowadays spoken in Tiendanite, We Hava, Téganpaïk and Tiouandé. It was spoken in the Pamale valley and its tributaries Vawe and Usa until the colonial war of 1917, when its speakers were displaced.
Toʼabaita, also known as Toqabaqita, Toʼambaita, Malu and Maluʼu, is a language spoken by the people living at the north-western tip of Malaita Island, of South Eastern Solomon Islands. Toʼabaita is an Austronesian language.
Neveʻei, also known as Vinmavis, is an Oceanic language of central Malekula, Vanuatu. There are around 500 primary speakers of Neveʻei and about 750 speakers in total.
Longgu (Logu) is a Southeast Solomonic language of Guadalcanal, but originally from Malaita.