Imprinted brain hypothesis

Last updated

Imprinted brain hypothesis
Bernard Crespi 2016.jpg
Bernard Crespi, the primary originator of the hypothesis, in 2016
ClaimsAutism and schizophrenia are genetic opposites via parental genomic imprinting
Related scientific disciplines Autism, schizophrenia, developmental disability, evolutionary psychology
Year proposed2008
Original proponents Bernard Crespi, Christopher Badcock
Hypothetical concepts

The imprinted brain hypothesis is an unsubstantiated hypothesis in evolutionary psychology regarding the causes of autism spectrum and schizophrenia spectrum disorders, first presented by Bernard Crespi and Christopher Badcock in 2008. It claims that certain autistic and schizotypal traits are opposites, and that this implies the etiology of the two conditions must be at odds.

Contents

The imprinted brain hypothesis is based around genomic imprinting, an epigenetic process through which genes are expressed differently by way of one parent's contribution having more effect than the other. Specifically, proponents of the imprinted brain hypothesis propose that autism spectrum disorders are caused by paternal overimprinting, while schizophrenia spectrum disorders are caused by maternal overimprinting; they point to a number of supposed correlations and anticorrelations seen between the disorders and other traits to support the hypothesis.

While the hypothesis has found some attention in popular science, it lacks scientific backing. [1] [2] It has also been attacked as unfalsifiable, exaggerated, and overly broad. [3] Specific issues for the hypothesis include that the predictions it makes about genetic disorders are falsified, that the effects of the two disorders on empathy and mentalizing are contrary to Crespi and Badcock's model, and that many neuroimaging findings fail to support the hypothesis.

Genomic imprinting

Genomic imprinting is an epigenetic process by which certain genes are expressed in a parent-of-origin-specific manner. The imprinted brain theory is a variant of the kinship theory of genomic imprinting, also known as the conflict theory of genomic imprinting. The kinship theory argues that in diploid organisms, such as humans, the maternal and paternal set of genes may have antagonistic reproductive interests since the mother and father may have antagonistic interests regarding the development of the child. The kinship theory is one of multiple competing major hypotheses regarding genomic imprinting and is supported by proponents of the imprinted brain hypothesis, but the precise matter of how genomic imprinting works has not yet been resolved. [4] [5]

Hypothesis and background

Proponents of the imprinted brain hypothesis argue that since it is uncertain if a woman's other and future children have and will have the same father, as well as the father generally having lower parental investment, it may be in the father's reproductive interest for his child to use more of the mother's resources than other children, while it may be in the mother's interest for a child to take fewer resources and free up more for herself and future children. [6] [7] Thus, a genomic imprinting with slight maternal bias would supposedly be associated with factors such as decreased growth, more tractable behavior, and an empathizing and less self-centered personality causing less demands on the mother. The opposite would occur for a slight paternal bias. [6] [7]

However, an extreme genomic imprinting in favor of maternal genes is argued to cause psychosis such as in schizophrenia spectrum disorders, while an extreme genomic imprinting in favor of paternal genes is argued to cause autism spectrum disorders. This claims the symptoms of schizophrenia are caused by overempathizing, resulting in delusions and paranoia, while those of autism are caused by underempathizing. Specifically, autism is considered to be a tendency to under-mentalize and under-empathize in a way that treats people as objects, while schizotypy is considered to be the inverse tendency to over-mentalize and over-empathize until objects are treated as people. Certain neuroimaging findings lend support to the hypothesis, [8] although neuroimaging in schizophrenia is controversial due to the neurological impact of neuroleptic medication, [9] [10] and other neuroimaging findings have results inconsistent with the hypothesis. [11] [12]

Traits such as the ambivalence seen in negative symptoms versus the single-minded focus of autistic special interests are also posited to be distinctions, [6] although the pronounced similarity and overlap between negative symptomatology seen in the two disorders weakens this claim substantially. [13] [14]

The imprinted brain hypothesis was first proposed in 2008 by biologist Bernard Crespi and sociologist Christopher Badcock, neither of whom had previous experience with cognitive science or behavioural genetics. [7] Publishing their first presentation of their claims in the influential cognitive science journal Behavioral and Brain Sciences , the hypothesis attracted significant attention, both interested and critical. [15]

Arguments of proponents

The imprinted brain hypothesis has some similarities with the extreme male brain theory of autism, but they come apart significantly. Proponents of the imprinted brain hypothesis state that the hypothesized imprinting mechanism may have detrimental interactions when extreme genomic imprinting occurs in the opposite sex, which they claim provides an explanation for something that would be a 'problem' for the extreme male brain claims—specifically, that female autism tends to be particularly severe. [6] This is also used as an explanation for the relative severity of schizophrenia in males. [6] However, proponents of the extreme male brain theory believe sexual dimorphism in autism severity is already explained by diagnostic discrepancy. [16]

In both autism and schizophrenia, the theory of mind is impaired, which the imprinted brain hypothesis posits occurs via different mechanisms and is not generalizable to the broader underlying neurotypes. Proponents of the hypothesis claim people with schizotypal personality have an enhanced theory of mind, increased emphatic ability, and an improved ability to recognize the emotions of others, [15] but this is not supported by research on the schizotypal personality disorder population [17] or on measures of 'healthy' schizotypy in the general population. [18] [19] Rather, theory of mind appears to be impaired in all schizophrenia-spectrum conditions even in the absence of frank psychosis. [20]

Factors such as nutrition during pregnancy are believed to affect imprinting. Proponents note that schizophrenia is associated with maternal starvation during pregnancy while autism has increased in diagnostic prevalence in affluent societies, [21] although the general scientific consensus is that rising rates of autism diagnosis in wealthy societies are related to awareness rather than prevalence. [22]

Autism and schizophrenia appear to be related to birth weight in opposite ways, with autism being associated with high birth weight and schizophrenia with low birth weight. Supporters of the hypothesis point towards this association as evidence. [23]

Proponents of the hypothesis also point towards genetic disorders with an elevated risk of one disorder and not the other, especially imprinting disorders, to support their claims. For instance, Beckwith-Wiedemann syndrome is caused by increased effects of paternally imprinted genes and has an increased incidence of autism. [4] [23] [24] Contrary to the overall claim here, the majority of disorders that raise the risk of one of autism and schizophrenia also do so for the other, including in ways that directly contradict the imprinted brain hypothesis such as for imprinting disorders. For instance, velocardiofacial syndrome is associated with significant increases (10- to 40-fold above the general population) in the risk for both autism and schizophrenia. [25] [26]

Data from copy number variation and genome-wide association studies support shared genetic mechanisms causing schizophrenia and autism, although this only lends circumstantial support to the imprinted brain hypothesis and can also be used to support many competing hypotheses. [27] [28]

The role of oxytocin in autism and schizophrenia has also been researched, and some findings and characterizations of the research have been used in favour of the hypothesis. Oxytocin appears to have treatment potential in both autism [29] and schizophrenia, likely including schizotypal personality disorder. [30] [31] Crespi proposes that, contrary to this suggestion, high oxytocin is a contributing factor in population schizotypy and creates "hyper-developed" social understanding that induces psychosis. [32] Supporters of the hypothesis point towards associations between higher oxytocin levels and personality traits that are also associated with schizotypy, such as creativity and divergent thinking. [33] [34] However, both schizotypal personality disorder and schizotypy in the general population appear associated with the same oxytocin deficits as observed in schizophrenia, [31] and the degree to which positive symptoms of schizophrenia and schizotypy are associated with high oxytocin—as argued by proponents—appears unclear and contradicted. [30]

Issues

The broad claim that autism and schizophrenia are in opposition on the biological level is not supported by research. In samples of autistic adults, schizophrenia and other non-affective psychotic disorders occur at far higher rates than the general population, occurring in about 8-10% of the broad ASD population [35] [36] and as high as one-third in PDD-NOS. [37] The same association occurs in childhood onset schizophrenia, which is considered a more homogenous form of the disorder that hews closest to the hypothetical neurodevelopmental disorder underlying schizophrenia-spectrum conditions; approximately one-quarter of children with schizophrenia fit the criteria for an autism spectrum disorder prior to the onset of psychosis, and the majority have clinical or subclinical disturbances of social, motor, or language skills similar to those seen in autistic children. [38] Adults with schizophrenia and related psychotic disorders also have higher rates of autistic symptomatology than healthy controls. [39]

Crespi and Badcock make a number of claims about genetic disorders and their relationship to the hypothesis; for instance, that the relationship between those disorders and sex chromosome aneuploidy supports the hypothesis, with trisomy X and Klinefelter syndrome (extra X chromosomes) increasing schizophrenia risk and Turner syndrome (one X chromosome) increasing autism risk. [4] However, polysomy X conditions are associated with increased autism as well as schizophrenia risk, [40] [41] [42] and Turner syndrome is approximately three times as common in schizophrenic women as the general female population. [43] Genetic syndromes in general lend credence to the suggestion that autism and schizophrenia are related rather than contraindicated, with conditions that sharply increase one risk tending to also increase the other. For instance, velocardiofacial syndrome, which is associated with a 20- to 30-fold increase in schizophrenia risk, [25] also significantly increases the risk of autism. [26] Other chromosomal disorders notable for significantly increasing the risk of both autism and schizophrenia include 15q11.2 microdeletions [44] and 17q12 microdeletion syndrome. [45]

Imprinting disorders such as Prader-Willi syndrome tend to have phenotypes that contradict the hypothesis. Pws.jpg
Imprinting disorders such as Prader-Willi syndrome tend to have phenotypes that contradict the hypothesis.

Moreover, the specific predictions the imprinted brain hypothesis makes about imprinting disorders are for the most part falsified. The imprinted brain hypothesis predicts that Prader-Willi syndrome, a disorder of maternal overimprinting, should have decreased autism and increased psychosis, while Angelman syndrome, a disorder of paternal overimprinting, should have the opposite. [4] However, autism rates are substantially above those of the general population in PWS and similar to or below it in AS, [46] while non-affective psychosis appears to occur at rates comparable to the general population in PWS. [47] Indeed, it has been suggested that maternally imprinted cases of Prader-Willi syndrome have an elevated autism prevalence compared to all etiologies of the disorder, the exact opposite of the imprinted brain hypothesis. [46] [48] Despite being contradicted by other research, Crespi nonetheless claims that imprinting disorders as a rule fit his hypothesis. [4]

While Crespi and Badcock have claimed neuroimaging studies lend support to the imprinted brain hypothesis, other neuroimaging studies have found contradictory results. Several neurological findings are common to both autism and schizophrenia. [11] [12] The brain regions that distinguish schizophrenia from autism are also those at the centre of the controversy regarding neuroleptic medication, [9] [10] reducing the degree to which they can be used to distinguish the disorders; autistic subjects taking psychotropic medication share some of the altered neuroconnectivity that Crespi and Badcock ascribe to schizophrenia. [49]

The imprinting brain hypothesis has also been criticized for inaccurately presenting the schizophrenia spectrum and making claims about schizophrenic disorders that are at odds with their clinical profiles. The claim that milder schizophrenia-spectrum disorders are associated with intensified empathy and strong theory of mind is imperiled by research showing the opposite. [17] [18] [19] [20] The specific pattern of empathy deficits also appears to be consistent between autism and schizophrenia, with both demonstrating impaired cognitive empathy and relatively preserved affective empathy. [50] [51] Crespi and Badcock's attempt to conceptualize schizophrenia as a relatively homogenous disorder that slots neatly into one end of a spectrum has been criticized due to the clinical heterogeneity in even individual cases of schizophrenia, due to the different presentation and course of positive and negative symptoms. [52]

Crespi and Badcock have also been criticized for avoiding falsifiable claims, in addition to the falsification of those claims they do make. Responses to Crespi's major publications on the topic have noted the broad range of evidence it attempts to 'tie up' and explain, without considering the consequences such a sweeping conjecture would have for known traits and comorbidities that contradict the claims, and for avoiding the matter of what would constitute a falsification or disconfirmation. [3]

One of the most significant components of the hypothesis is that it predicts autism should be associated with "hypo-mentalizing" and schizophrenia with "hyper-mentalizing"; that is, that people in each group should have radically different impairments in mentalizing. [4] [15] This is a core component of the imprinted brain hypothesis, and one of the fundamental assumptions involved. However, meta-analysis of mentalizing skills in autism and schizophrenia does not support this claim. Rather, both conditions appear to be associated with similar mentalizing impairments that share common processing deficits. [53]

See also

Related Research Articles

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Asperger syndrome</span> Formerly recognized neurodevelopmental condition

Asperger syndrome (AS), also known as Asperger's syndrome or Asperger's, is a diagnosis formerly used to describe a neurodevelopmental condition characterized by significant difficulties in social interaction and nonverbal communication, along with restricted, repetitive patterns of behavior and interests. Asperger syndrome has been merged with other conditions into autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and is no longer considered a diagnosis. It was considered milder than other diagnoses which were merged into ASD due to relatively unimpaired spoken language and intelligence.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Anhedonia</span> Inability to feel pleasure

Anhedonia is a diverse array of deficits in hedonic function, including reduced motivation or ability to experience pleasure. While earlier definitions emphasized the inability to experience pleasure, anhedonia is currently used by researchers to refer to reduced motivation, reduced anticipatory pleasure (wanting), reduced consummatory pleasure (liking), and deficits in reinforcement learning. In the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5), anhedonia is a component of depressive disorders, substance-related disorders, psychotic disorders, and personality disorders, where it is defined by either a reduced ability to experience pleasure, or a diminished interest in engaging in previously pleasurable activities. While the International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems, Tenth Revision (ICD-10) does not explicitly mention anhedonia, the depressive symptom analogous to anhedonia as described in the DSM-5 is a loss of interest or pleasure.

Schizotypal personality disorder, also known as schizotypal disorder, is a cluster A personality disorder. The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) classification describes the disorder specifically as a personality disorder characterized by thought disorder, paranoia, a characteristic form of social anxiety, derealization, transient psychosis, and unconventional beliefs. People with this disorder feel pronounced discomfort in forming and maintaining social connections with other people, primarily due to the belief that other people harbor negative thoughts and views about them. Peculiar speech mannerisms and socially unexpected modes of dress are also characteristic. Schizotypal people may react oddly in conversations, not respond, or talk to themselves. They frequently interpret situations as being strange or having unusual meanings for them; paranormal and superstitious beliefs are common. Schizotypal people usually disagree with the suggestion that their thoughts and behaviors are a 'disorder' and seek medical attention for depression or anxiety instead. Schizotypal personality disorder occurs in approximately 3% of the general population and is more commonly diagnosed in males.

In psychology, schizotypy is a theoretical concept that posits a continuum of personality characteristics and experiences, ranging from normal dissociative, imaginative states to extreme states of mind related to psychosis, especially schizophrenia. The continuum of personality proposed in schizotypy is in contrast to a categorical view of psychosis, wherein psychosis is considered a particular state of mind, which the person either has or does not have.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Conditions comorbid to autism</span> Medical conditions more common in autistic people

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder that begins in early childhood, persists throughout adulthood, and affects two crucial areas of development: social communication and restricted, repetitive patterns of behavior. There are many conditions comorbid to autism spectrum disorder, such as attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, anxiety disorders, and epilepsy.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Simon Baron-Cohen</span> British psychologist and author

Sir Simon Philip Baron-Cohen is a British clinical psychologist and professor of developmental psychopathology at the University of Cambridge. He is the director of the university's Autism Research Centre and a Fellow of Trinity College.

Mind-blindness, mindblindness or mind blindness is a theory initially proposed in 1990 that claims that all autistic people have a lack or developmental delay of theory of mind (ToM), meaning they are unable to attribute mental states to others. According to the theory, a lack of ToM is considered equivalent to a lack of both cognitive and affective empathy. In the context of the theory, mind-blindness implies being unable to predict behavior and attribute mental states including beliefs, desires, emotions, or intentions of other people. The mind-blindness theory asserts that children who delay in this development will often develop autism.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Heritability of autism</span> The rate at which autism is inherited

The heritability of autism is the proportion of differences in expression of autism that can be explained by genetic variation; if the heritability of a condition is high, then the condition is considered to be primarily genetic. Autism has a strong genetic basis. Although the genetics of autism are complex, autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is explained more by multigene effects than by rare mutations with large effects.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Causes of autism</span> Proposed causes of autism

Many causes of autism, including environmental and genetic factors, have been recognized or proposed, but understanding of the theory of causation of autism is incomplete. Attempts have been made to incorporate the known genetic and environmental causes into a comprehensive causative framework. ASD is a neurodevelopmental disorder marked by impairments in communicative ability and social interaction, as well as restricted and repetitive behaviors, interests, or activities not suitable for the individual's developmental stage. The severity of symptoms and functional impairment vary between individuals.

The empathising–systemising (E–S) theory is a controversial theory on the psychological basis of autism and male–female neurological differences originally put forward by English clinical psychologist Simon Baron-Cohen. It classifies individuals based on abilities in empathic thinking (E) and systematic thinking (S). It measures skills using an Empathy Quotient (EQ) and Systemising Quotient (SQ) and attempts to explain the social and communication symptoms in autism spectrum disorders as deficits and delays in empathy combined with intact or superior systemising.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">LRRTM1</span> Protein-coding gene in the species Homo sapiens

LRRTM1 is a brain-expressed imprinted gene that encodes a leucine-rich repeat transmembrane protein that interacts with neurexins and neuroligins to modulate synaptic cell adhesion in neurons. As the name implies, its protein product is a transmembrane protein that contains many leucine rich repeats. It is expressed during the development of specific forebrain structures and shows a variable pattern of maternal downregulation.

Autism, also called autism spectrum disorder (ASD), is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by symptoms of deficient reciprocal social communication and the presence of restricted, repetitive, and inflexible patterns of behavior. Autism generally affects a person's ability to understand and connect with others, as well as their adaptability to everyday situations, with its severity and support needs varying widely across the spectrum. For example, some are nonspeaking, while others have proficient spoken language.

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) refers to a variety of conditions typically identified by challenges with social skills, communication, speech, and repetitive sensory-motor behaviors. The 11th International Classification of Diseases (ICD-11), released in January 2021, characterizes ASD by the associated deficits in the ability to initiate and sustain two-way social communication and restricted or repetitive behavior unusual for the individual's age or situation. Although linked with early childhood, the symptoms can appear later as well. Symptoms can be detected before the age of two and experienced practitioners can give a reliable diagnosis by that age. However, official diagnosis may not occur until much older, even well into adulthood. There is a large degree of variation in how much support a person with ASD needs in day-to-day life. This can be classified by a further diagnosis of ASD level 1, level 2, or level 3. Of these, ASD level 3 describes people requiring very substantial support and who experience more severe symptoms. ASD-related deficits in nonverbal and verbal social skills can result in impediments in personal, family, social, educational, and occupational situations. This disorder tends to have a strong correlation with genetics along with other factors. More research is identifying ways in which epigenetics is linked to autism. Epigenetics generally refers to the ways in which chromatin structure is altered to affect gene expression. Mechanisms such as cytosine regulation and post-translational modifications of histones. Of the 215 genes contributing, to some extent in ASD, 42 have been found to be involved in epigenetic modification of gene expression. Some examples of ASD signs are specific or repeated behaviors, enhanced sensitivity to materials, being upset by changes in routine, appearing to show reduced interest in others, avoiding eye contact and limitations in social situations, as well as verbal communication. When social interaction becomes more important, some whose condition might have been overlooked suffer social and other exclusion and are more likely to have coexisting mental and physical conditions. Long-term problems include difficulties in daily living such as managing schedules, hypersensitivities, initiating and sustaining relationships, and maintaining jobs.

The evolution of schizophrenia refers to the theory of natural selection working in favor of selecting traits that are characteristic of the disorder. Positive symptoms are features that are not present in healthy individuals but appear as a result of the disease process. These include visual and/or auditory hallucinations, delusions, paranoia, and major thought disorders. Negative symptoms refer to features that are normally present but are reduced or absent as a result of the disease process, including social withdrawal, apathy, anhedonia, alogia, and behavioral perseveration. Cognitive symptoms of schizophrenia involve disturbances in executive functions, working memory impairment, and inability to sustain attention.

Evolutionary psychiatry, also known as Darwinian psychiatry, is a theoretical approach to psychiatry that aims to explain psychiatric disorders in evolutionary terms. As a branch of the field of evolutionary medicine, it is distinct from the medical practice of psychiatry in its emphasis on providing scientific explanations rather than treatments for mental disorder. This often concerns questions of ultimate causation. For example, psychiatric genetics may discover genes associated with mental disorders, but evolutionary psychiatry asks why those genes persist in the population. Other core questions in evolutionary psychiatry are why heritable mental disorders are so common how to distinguish mental function and dysfunction, and whether certain forms of suffering conveyed an adaptive advantage. Disorders commonly considered are depression, anxiety, schizophrenia, autism, eating disorders, and others. Key explanatory concepts are of evolutionary mismatch and the fact that evolution is guided by reproductive success rather than health or wellbeing. Rather than providing an alternative account of the cause of mental disorder, evolutionary psychiatry seeks to integrate findings from traditional schools of psychology and psychiatry such as social psychology, behaviourism, biological psychiatry and psychoanalysis into a holistic account related to evolutionary biology. In this sense, it aims to meet the criteria of a Kuhnian paradigm shift.

The history of Autism spans over a century; Autism has been subject to varying treatments, being pathologized or being viewed as a beneficial part of human neurodiversity. The understanding of Autism has been shaped by cultural, scientific, and societal factors, and its perception and treatment change over time as scientific understanding of Autism develops.

Sex and gender differences in autism exist regarding prevalence, presentation, and diagnosis.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Bernard Crespi</span>

Bernard Joseph Crespi is an American professor of evolutionary biology at Simon Fraser University in British Columbia, Canada. His research focuses on social evolution across multiple scales, using genetic and ecological approaches. He is one of the initiators of the imprinted brain hypothesis.

Christopher Robert Badcock is a British sociologist and Emeritus Reader in Sociology at the London School of Economics, from which he retired in 2011. He received his PhD from the London School of Economics in 1973 under the supervision of Ernest Gellner. His thesis, and his early work thereafter, focused on the work of Claude Levi-Strauss. He served as a lecturer in sociology at Polytechnic of the South Bank from 1969 to 1973, and was on the faculty in the Sociology Department at the London School of Economics from 1974 until his retirement. He is known for working with Bernard Crespi to develop the imprinted brain hypothesis, according to which autism results from "a paternal bias in the expression of imprinted genes", whereas psychosis results from a maternal and/or X-chromosome bias in the expression of such genes.

<span class="mw-page-title-main">Double empathy problem</span> Psychological theory regarding individuals on the autism spectrum

The theory of the double empathy problem is a psychological and sociological theory first coined in 2012 by Damian Milton, an autistic autism researcher. This theory proposes that many of the difficulties autistic individuals face when socializing with non-autistic individuals are due, in part, to a lack of mutual understanding between the two groups, meaning that most autistic people struggle to understand and empathize with non-autistic people, whereas most non-autistic people also struggle to understand and empathize with autistic people. This lack of understanding may stem from bidirectional differences in communication style, social-cognitive characteristics, and experiences between autistic and non-autistic individuals, but not necessarily an inherent deficiency. Recent studies have shown that most autistic individuals are able to socialize, communicate effectively, empathize well, and display social reciprocity with most other autistic individuals. This theory and subsequent findings challenge the commonly held belief that the social skills of autistic individuals are inherently impaired, as well as the theory of "mind-blindness" proposed by prominent autism researcher Simon Baron-Cohen in the mid-1980s, which suggested that empathy and theory of mind are universally impaired in autistic individuals.

References

  1. Russell-Smith, Suzanna (2012). The Relationship Between the Autism and Schizophrenia Spectra: An Investigation at the Trait and Cognitive Levels (PhD). Perth: University of Western Australia.
  2. Ragsdale G, Foley RA (July 2012). "Testing the imprinted brain: parent-of-origin effects on empathy and systemizing". Evolution and Human Behavior. 33 (4): 402–410. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.12.002.
  3. 1 2 Dickins BJA, Dickins DW, Dickins TE (2008). "Is this conjectural phenotypic dichotomy a plausible outcome of genomic imprinting?" (PDF). Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 31 (3): 267–268. doi:10.1017/S0140525X08004287.
  4. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Crespi B (November 2008). "Genomic imprinting in the development and evolution of psychotic spectrum conditions". Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society. 83 (4): 441–93. doi:10.1111/j.1469-185X.2008.00050.x. PMID   18783362. S2CID   18769059.
  5. Patten MM, Ross L, Curley JP, Queller DC, Bonduriansky R, Wolf JB (April 2014). "The evolution of genomic imprinting: theories, predictions and empirical tests". Heredity. 113 (2): 119–28. doi: 10.1038/hdy.2014.29 . PMC   4105453 . PMID   24755983.
  6. 1 2 3 4 5 Badcock C, Crespi B (August 2008). "Battle of the sexes may set the brain". Nature. 454 (7208): 1054–5. Bibcode:2008Natur.454.1054B. doi: 10.1038/4541054a . PMID   18756240. S2CID   5890859.
  7. 1 2 3 Carey B (2008). "In a Novel Theory of Mental Disorders, Parents' Genes Are in Competition". The New York Times.
  8. Ciaramidaro A, Bölte S, Schlitt S, Hainz D, Poustka F, Weber B, et al. (January 2015). "Schizophrenia and autism as contrasting minds: neural evidence for the hypo-hyper-intentionality hypothesis". Schizophrenia Bulletin. 41 (1): 171–9. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbu124. PMC   4266299 . PMID   25210055. Consistent with these studies, it has been hypothesized that ASD and SCZ thus may be located at the extreme ends of a cognitive architecture ranging from a mechanistic hypo-intentional (to treat person as objects) to a mentalistic hyper-intentional (to treat objects as persons) mode of cognition, respectively.
  9. 1 2 Torres US, Portela-Oliveira E, Borgwardt S, Busatto GF (December 2013). "Structural brain changes associated with antipsychotic treatment in schizophrenia as revealed by voxel-based morphometric MRI: an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis". BMC Psychiatry. 13: 342. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-13-342 . PMC   3878502 . PMID   24359128.
  10. 1 2 Roiz-Santiañez R, Suarez-Pinella P, Crespo-Facorro B (July 2015). "Brain Structural Effects of Antipsychotic Treatment in Schizophrenia: A Systematic Review". Current Neuropharmacology. 13 (4): 422–434. doi:10.2174/1570159X13666150429002536. PMC   4790397 . PMID   26412062.
  11. 1 2 Cheung C, Yu K, Fung G, Leung M, Wong C, Li Q, Sham P, Chua S, McAlonan G (August 2010). "Autistic Disorders and Schizophrenia: Related or Remote? An Anatomical Likelihood Estimation". PLOS ONE. 5 (8): e12233. Bibcode:2010PLoSO...512233C. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0012233 . PMC   2923607 . PMID   20805880.
  12. 1 2 Voyles Ashkam, Angie (1 October 2020). "Autism shares brain structure changes with other psychiatric conditions". Spectrum News. The conditions that were more similar in their patterns of cortical thickness were also more likely to be genetically similar (a measure assessed in a previous study). For example, autism and schizophrenia, conditions that share genetics and traits, showed the strongest correlation.
  13. Goldstein G, Minshew NJ, Allen DN, Seaton BE (June 2002). "High-functioning autism and schizophrenia: A comparison of an early and late onset neurodevelopmental disorder". Archive of Clinical Neuropsychology. 17 (5): 461–475. doi: 10.1016/S0887-6177(01)00129-9 . PMID   14592000.
  14. Trevisan A, Foss-Feig JH, Naples J, Srihari V, Anticevic A, McPartland JC (June 2020). "Autism Spectrum Disorder and Schizophrenia Are Better Differentiated by Positive Symptoms Than Negative Symptoms". Frontiers in Psychiatry. 11: 548. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00548 . PMC   7301837 . PMID   32595540.
  15. 1 2 3 Crespi B, Badcock C (June 2008). "Psychosis and autism as diametrical disorders of the social brain" (PDF). The Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 31 (3): 241–61, discussion 261–320. doi:10.1017/S0140525X08004214. PMID   18578904. S2CID   346021. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2017-03-20.
  16. Kirkovski M, Enticott PG, Fitzgerald PB (November 2013). "A review of the role of female gender in autism spectrum disorders". Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 43 (11): 2584–603. doi:10.1007/s10803-013-1811-1. PMID   23525974. S2CID   44765026.
  17. 1 2 Ripoll LH, Zaki J, Perez-Rodriguez MN, Snyder R, Strike KS, Boussi A, Bartz JA, Oschner KN, Siever LJ, New AS (November 2013). "Empathic accuracy and cognition in schizotypal personality disorder". Psychiatry Research. 210 (1): 232–241. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2013.05.025. PMID   23810511. S2CID   17288282.
  18. 1 2 Henry JD, Bailey PE, Rendell PG (July 2008). "Empathy, social functioning and schizotypy". Psychiatry Research. 160 (1): 15–22. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2007.04.014. PMID   18511132. S2CID   1300758.
  19. 1 2 Canli D, Ozdemir H, Koçak OM (2015). "Magical ideation associated social cognition in adolescents: signs of a negative facial affect recognition deficit". Comprehensive Psychiatry. 61 (1): 90–6. doi:10.1016/j.comppsych.2015.05.009. PMID   26073064.
  20. 1 2 Bora E, Yücel M, Pantelis C (September 2009). "Theory of mind impairment: a distinct trait-marker for schizophrenia spectrum disorders and bipolar disorder?". Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 120 (4): 253–264. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447.2009.01414.x. PMID   19489747. S2CID   35632790.
  21. Badcock C (June 2011). "The imprinted brain: how genes set the balance between autism and psychosis" (PDF). Epigenomics. 3 (3): 345–59. doi:10.2217/epi.11.19. PMID   22122342.
  22. Jessica Wright (3 March 2017). "The Real Reasons Autism Rates Are Up in the U.S." Scientific American.
  23. 1 2 Byars SG, Stearns SC, Boomsma JJ (November 2014). "Opposite risk patterns for autism and schizophrenia are associated with normal variation in birth size: phenotypic support for hypothesized diametric gene-dosage effects". Proceedings. Biological Sciences. 281 (1794): 20140604. doi:10.1098/rspb.2014.0604. PMC   4211440 . PMID   25232142.
  24. Shuman C, Beckwith JB, Weksberg R (11 August 2016). "Beckwith-Wiedemann Syndrome". In Adam MP, Ardinger HH, Pagon RA, Wallace SE, Bean LJ, Stephens K, Amemiya A (eds.). GeneReviews. University of Washington, Seattle. PMID   20301568 . Retrieved 2019-08-26.
  25. 1 2 Bassett AS, Chow EWC, Abdelmalik P, Gheorghiu M, Husted J, Weksberg R (September 2003). "The Schizophrenia Phenotype in 22q11 Deletion Syndrome". The American Journal of Psychiatry. 160 (9): 1580–6. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.160.9.1580. PMC   3276594 . PMID   12944331.
  26. 1 2 Ousley O, Evans AN, Fernandez-Carriba S, Smearman EL, Rockers K, Morrier MJ, Evans DW, Coleman K, Cubells J (May 2017). "Examining the Overlap between Autism Spectrum Disorder and 22q11.2 Deletion Syndrome". International Journal of Molecular Sciences. 18 (5): 1071. doi: 10.3390/ijms18051071 . PMC   5454981 . PMID   28524075.
  27. Crespi B, Stead P, Elliot M (January 2010). "Evolution in health and medicine Sackler colloquium: Comparative genomics of autism and schizophrenia". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 107 (Suppl 1): 1736–41. Bibcode:2010PNAS..107.1736C. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0906080106 . PMC   2868282 . PMID   19955444.
  28. Stearns SC, Nesse RM, Govindaraju DR, Ellison PT (January 2010). "Evolution in health and medicine Sackler colloquium: Evolutionary perspectives on health and medicine". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 107 (Suppl 1): 1691–5. Bibcode:2010PNAS..107.1691S. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0914475107 . PMC   2868294 . PMID   20133821.
  29. Yamasue H, Domes G (2018). "Oxytocin and Autism Spectrum Disorders". Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences. 35 (1): 449–465. doi:10.1007/7854_2017_24. ISBN   978-3-319-63738-9. PMID   28766270.
  30. 1 2 Shilling PD, Feifel D (5 June 2017). "Potential of Oxytocin in the Treatment of Schizophrenia". CNS Drugs. 30 (3): 193–208. doi:10.1007/s40263-016-0315-x. PMC   5458113 . PMID   26895254.
  31. 1 2 Perez-Rodriguez MM, Derish NE, New AS (2014). "The Use of Oxytocin in Personality Disorders: Rationale and Current Status". Current Treatment Options in Psychiatry. 1 (4): 354–357. doi: 10.1007/s40501-014-0026-1 .
  32. Crespi B (2015). "Oxytocin, testosterone, and human social cognition" (PDF). Biological Reviews. 91 (2): 390–408. doi:10.1111/brv.12175. PMID   25631363. S2CID   24096994.
  33. De Dreu CKW, Baas M, Roskes M, Sligte DJ, Ebstein RP, Chew SH, Tong T, Jiang Y, Mayseless N, Shamay-Tsoory SG (August 2014). "Oxytonergic circuitry sustains and enables creative cognition in humans". Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 9 (8): 1159–1165. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst094 . PMC   4127019 . PMID   23863476.
  34. Mohr C, Claridge G (25 March 2015). "Schizotypy—Do Not Worry, It Is Not All Worrisome". Schizophrenia Bulletin. 41 (2): 436–443. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbu185. PMC   4373632 . PMID   25810058.
  35. De Giorgi R, De Crescenzo F, D'alo GL, Rizzo Pesci N, Di Franco V, Sandini C, Armando M (August 2019). "Prevalence of Non-Affective Psychoses in Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorders: A Systematic Review". Journal of Clinical Medicine. 8 (9): 1304. doi: 10.3390/jcm8091304 . PMC   6780908 . PMID   31450601.
  36. Vorstman JAS, Burbach JPH (2014). "Autism and Schizophrenia: Genetic and Phenotypic Relationships". Comprehensive Guide to Autism. New York: Springer-Verlag. pp. 1645–1662. doi:10.1007/978-1-4614-4788-7_96. ISBN   978-1-4614-4787-0.
  37. Mouridsen SE, Rich B, Isager T (September 2007). "Psychiatric disorders in adults diagnosed as children with atypical autism. A case control study". Journal of Neural Transmission. 115 (1): 135–8. doi:10.1007/s00702-007-0798-1. PMID   17768593. S2CID   34532555.
  38. Driver DI, Gognay N, Rappaport JL (October 2014). "Childhood Onset Schizophrenia and Early Onset Schizophrenia Spectrum Disorders". Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America. 22 (4): 539–555. doi:10.1016/j.chc.2013.04.001. PMC   3771646 . PMID   24012072.
  39. De Crescenzo F, Postorino V, Siracusano M, Riccioni A, Armando M, Curatolo P, Mazzone L (2019). "Autistic Symptoms in Schizophrenia Spectrum Disorders: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis". Frontiers in Psychiatry. 10 (78): 78. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00078 . PMC   6393379 . PMID   30846948.
  40. Tartaglia NR, Howell S, Sutherland A, Wilson R, Wilson L (May 2010). "A review of trisomy X (47,XXX)". Orphanet Journal of Rare Diseases. 5 (8): 8. doi: 10.1186/1750-1172-5-8 . PMC   2883963 . PMID   20459843.
  41. van Rijn S, Bierman M, Bruining H, Swaab H (August 2012). "Vulnerability for autism traits in boys and men with an extra X chromosome (47,XXY): The mediating role of cognitive flexibility". Journal of Psychiatric Research. 46 (10): 1300–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.06.004 . PMID   22884425.
  42. Wilson AC, King J, Bishop DVM (2019). "Autism and social anxiety in children with sex chromosome trisomies: an observational study". Wellcome Open Research. 4 (32): 32. doi: 10.12688/wellcomeopenres.15095.2 . PMC   6567293 . PMID   31231689.
  43. Prior TI, Chue PS, Tibbo P (June 2000). "Investigation of Turner syndrome in schizophrenia". American Journal of Medical Genetics. 96 (3): 373–8. doi:10.1002/1096-8628(20000612)96:3<373::aid-ajmg26>3.0.co;2-z. PMID   10898917.
  44. "15q11.2 microdeletions" (PDF). Unique Rare Chromosome Disorder Foundation. 2018.
  45. Moreno de Luca D, Mulle JG, Kaminsky EB, Sanders SJ (2010). "Deletion 17q12 Is a Recurrent Copy Number Variant that Confers High Risk of Autism and Schizophrenia". American Journal of Human Genetics. 87 (5): 618–630. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2010.10.004 . PMC   2978962 . PMID   21055719.
  46. 1 2 Veltman MWM, Craig EE, Bolton PF (December 2005). "Autism spectrum disorders in Prader–Willi and Angelman syndromes: a systematic review". Psychiatric Genetics. 15 (4): 243–254. doi: 10.1097/00041444-200512000-00006 . PMID   16314754. S2CID   17897692.
  47. Bouras N, Beardsmore A, Dorman T, Cooper SA, Webb T (December 1998). "Affective psychosis and Prader–Willi syndrome". Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 42 (6): 463–71. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2788.1998.4260463.x . PMID   10030442.
  48. Veltman MWM, Thompson RJ, Roberts SE, Thomas SN, Whittington J, Bolton PF (February 2004). "Prader-Willi syndrome". European Child & Adult Psychiatry. 14 (1): 42–50. doi:10.1007/s00787-004-0354-6. PMID   14991431. S2CID   43285881.
  49. Linke AC, Olson L, Gao Y, Fishman I, Müller RA (2017). "Psychotropic medication use in autism spectrum disorders may affect functional brain connectivity". Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging. 2 (6): 518–527. doi:10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.06.008. PMC   5667652 . PMID   29104944.
  50. Rogers K, Dziobek I, Hassenstab J, Wolf OT, Convit A (August 2006). "Who Cares? Revisiting Empathy in Asperger Syndrome". Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 37 (4): 709–715. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0197-8. PMID   16906462. S2CID   13999363.
  51. Montag C, Heinz A, Kunz D, Gallinat J (May 2007). "Self-reported empathic abilities in schizophrenia". Schizophrenia Research. 92 (1–3): 85–9. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2007.01.024. PMID   17350225. S2CID   45332048.
  52. Thakkar KN, Mathews N, Park S (2008). "A complete theory of psychosis and autism as diametric disorders of social brain must consider full range of clinical syndromes". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 31 (3): 277–278. doi:10.1017/S0140525X0800438X.
  53. Chung YS, Barch D, Strube M (May 2014). "A Meta-Analysis of Mentalizing Impairments in Adults With Schizophrenia and Autism Spectrum Disorder". Schizophrenia Bulletin. 40 (3): 602–616. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbt048 . PMC   3984506 . PMID   23686020.